Introduction

In migratory dabbling ducks, mate choice occurs when individuals participate in courtship parties, in which several males display usually to a single female [defined as ‘communal courtships’ by Cramp and Simmons (1977) and ‘social courtships’ by McKinney (1986)]. Wishart (1983), Brodsky et al. (1988) and Hepp (1989) showed that American wigeon (Anas americana), mallard (A. platyrhynchos) and black duck (A. rubripes) females mainly chose mates according to male behavioural dominance (but see Bossema and Roemers 1985; Sorenson and Derrickson 1994). Pair formation generally occurs months before breeding (McKinney 1986) and females benefit from early pairing because they feed more efficiently, since their mates defend them from harassment by other males (Ashcroft 1976; Afton and Sayler 1982; Paulus 1983; McKinney 1986; Rohwer and Anderson 1988; Guillemain et al. 2003; but see Nakamura and Atsumi 2000).

The sex ratio of most migratory dabbling duck species is more male-biased at northern than at southern wintering sites; and the age ratio is also adult-biased in northern areas (Bellrose et al. 1961; Campredon 1983; Van Impe 1984). One possible mechanism for this spatial segregation of sex and age classes is behavioural dominance (Gauthreaux 1978), with adults dominating yearlings, paired ducks dominating unpaired ones and males usually dominating females (Paulus 1983; Hepp and Hair 1984; Campredon 1986; but see Thompson and Baldassarre 1992). Due to the latitudinal segregation of the sexes during winter, males wintering in northern sites, where the sex ratio is more male biased, would be at a mating disadvantage with respect to the presumably less dominant, younger males wintering in southern areas, where the sex ratio is not so male-biased as in northern areas and the mating opportunities may be better. That is, presumably more males would remain unpaired in northern sites utilized by the more dominant birds than in southern sites utilized by the less dominant birds, and this is paradoxical.

A phenomenon whose importance on the pairing strategies of migratory dabbling ducks has not been previously addressed is that male-biased sex ratios along migration routes, coupled with a likely higher dominance status of males having spent the winter in northern sites, may make it advantageous for females having wintered and paired in southern sites to switch mates at stopover sites during northward migration.

Although dabbling ducks generally pair long before breeding (McKinney 1986), the bonds of some pairs may not be firmly established and females may test several males before a strong bond is established. Some females have been observed to switch mates on the wintering grounds (McKinney and Stolen 1982; Wishart 1983; Paulus 1988; Pattenden and Boag 1989). Given this sequential mate-sampling strategy of females, mate-switching could take place along migratory routes due to the likely presence of unpaired and more dominant males at northern sites. Therefore, I predict that, coinciding with the passage of migratory birds at stopover sites, courtship activity should increase in migratory species. I also predict that populations that do not perform long-range migrations should not exhibit an increase in the frequency of occurrence of courtship parties during late winter/early spring. To test these predictions, I recorded courtship rates of mallard and common teal (A. crecca) in southern Spain, where the former species is non-migratory (Sociedad Española de Ornitología 1985), but the latter is migratory (Asensio and Carrascal 1992). Therefore, in southern Spain, the courtship rate of common teals, but not that of mallards, should peak in late winter/spring, coinciding with the migratory passages of teals that spent the winter in northwestern Africa.

Materials and methods

To quantify courtship rates, I scan-sampled (see Altmann 1974) flocks of mallards (October–April) and common teals (December–March) for the occurrence of courtship parties. Courtship parties of mallards and teals involved groups of birds, on average 5–7 males and 1–2 females (personal observation). The number of males participating in these parties could be easily accounted for, because males congregated very closely around the female(s) while performing displays and females performed nod-swimming and incited (McKinney 1986). The observations were conducted during 1984–1989, using a spotting scope, in wetlands that are protected (a national park, three nature reserves) and in which hunting is forbidden. I recorded the number of courtship parties during observation sessions lasting 1.8±2.5 h (SD; n=49) for common teal and 1.9±2.2 h (n=40) for mallard. Every courtship party was recorded only once. The possibility of pseudoreplication was very low because the flocks were scanned only once when the number of birds in wetlands was low (<50); and when the number of birds was high the probability of recording the same individual more than once should have been very small. The observation sessions within months were distributed temporally to ensure coverage from sunrise to sunset. Courtship rate was calculated as the number of courtship parties recorded per hour of observation and expressed relative to 100 individuals. Monthly differences in courtship rates were tested with analyses of variance on log-transformed data.

Results

The mean numbers of mallards and common teals in wetlands where the observations were conducted were 137±131 (n=40) and 278±264 (n=49), respectively. The numbers of individuals of both species in such wetlands were higher in autumn than in winter/spring (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean numbers (±1 SE) of mallards and common teals in wetlands of southern Spain where observations on courtship parties were recorded. Sample sizes are indicated beside datapoints

Courtship parties of mallard were observed between October and April, and in southern Spain its occurrence did not exhibit a peak during spring, as predicted for a non-migratory population. In contrast, the courtship rate of mallards decreased from February onwards (Fig. 2; F6,33=2.44, P=0.046; log-transformed data) coinciding with the onset of breeding (Amat 1982).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Courtship rates (mean ±1 SE, log-transformed) of mallards and common teals in southern Spain during October–April. Courtship rates were calculated as the number of courtship parties recorded per hour of observation during each recording session and expressed relative to 100 individuals. The numbers of observation sessions per month are indicated beside datapoints

Courtship parties of common teals were mainly observed between December and March, and little courtship activity was recorded outside that period, in spite of the regular presence of the common teal in southern Spain during October–March (Amat and García 1979). The rate of courtships of common teals in southern Spain decreased from December to January, but increased in February and March (Fig. 2; F3,45=3.05, P=0.038; log-transformed data) despite decreasing population size (Amat and García 1979).

Discussion

The pattern of occurrence of courtship rates of common teals and mallards in southern Spain supports the hypothesis that, in migratory populations, females may switch mates at stopover sites during northward migration. In southern Spain, where mallards are non-migratory, there was no increase in the courtship rates of this species during late winter/early spring, contrary to what was observed for migratory common teals. However, the courtship rates of migratory populations of mallards, as well as those of gadwalls (A. strepera) and pintails (A. acuta), have been observed to peak in autumn or early winter and again in spring during migration periods (Cramp and Simmons 1977). In the case of gadwalls, Palmer (1976) indicated that ‘there is much pairing activity during spring migration’, despite the fact that nearly all females may be paired in late winter before they initiate migration.

Although the increase in courtship rates at stopover sites during migration is consistent with the hypothesis of mate-switching by females along migration routes, an alternative hypothesis could also explain the increase in courtship rates recorded in migratory populations at stopover sites. The function of courtship parties might be related more to male–male competition and establishing dominance ranking between males than to mate choice by females. Because resident populations may have a more stable social structure than migratory populations, males would have less need to display in resident populations, where dominance status was already established, than in migratory populations, for which an influx of new individuals would create some instability and could lead to an increase in courtship activity to re-establish dominance positions. This hypothesis suggests that the establishment of dominance ranking should be for the acquisition of resources other than mates. However, this hypothesis does not explain why, in resident populations, courtship parties are also recorded once dominance positions have been established (Bossema and Roemers 1985). Furthermore, the duration of the stays of migratory ducks at stopover sites may be rather short (e.g. 5 days for garganey, A. querquedula; Guillemain et al. 2004), and the turnover of individuals may be high, so that under these circumstances it seems unlikely that the function of courting in parties at stopover sites would be to establish dominance positions. Also, the fact that the aim of the stays of migratory individuals at stopover sites is more for resting than refuelling (Guillemain et al. 2004) suggests that the function of courting in parties at such sites is not related to the establishment of dominance ranks for the acquisition of resources. In fact, displays directed by males at rival males when courting in parties function only in competition for mates, mate defence or in appeasement of other males (see McKinney et al. 1990).

From a female’s perspective, it may be very important to reach the breeding grounds mated to a male able to defend her from forced copulation attempts by other males (Wishart 1983; Davis 2002), and in fact, in polygynous species, dominant males could successfully defend more than one female (Sorenson 1992). Interestingly, Afton (1985) suggested and Sorenson (1992) showed that the ability of males to guard their mates during the breeding season is an important determinant of female breeding success.

The possibility that mate-switching by female dabbling ducks may occur along migration routes adds complexity to waterfowl mating systems, in particular to sexual selection and conflict in this group.