Introduction

Next to the genetic equipment of an individual, environmental factors play an important role in shaping different phenotypes and their reproductive capacity (Dufty et al. 2002). Nutrition, stress and bioactive substances can influence reproductive performance not only in adults, but also in developing organisms during pregnancy (reviewed in Rhind et al. 2001). This phenomenon is called “programming” and refers to the ability of an organism to alter its later phenotype according to the environmental condition experienced during early life. In many cases short-term adjustments may however increase the risk of disease as a long-term consequence of fetal programming (reviewed in Gluckman et al. 2008a). Programming effects could be demonstrated in nearly every organ system [e.g., kidney (Moritz et al. 2003), skeletal muscle (Du et al. 2010), brain (Harris and Seckl 2011), immune system (Palmer 2011)]. Also, several factors were found to act as cues of environmental conditions to the fetus—nutrition and stress being the ones best studied. Both cues may act via the same pathway, as glucocorticoids are believed to constitute an underlying mechanism (Seckl 2004). However, the literature is full of contradicting findings in this context. One reason for this is the vast amount of different experimental paradigms used in programming research; especially, sensitive time periods must be considered. The phenotype will be affected according to the organ system under development (Nathanielsz 2006). During early pregnancy, for instance, many cells still are omnipotent and the potential impact of environmental conditions may be even more detrimental.

Reproductive maturation commences with puberty, characterized by an increasing influence of gonadal hormones, which is reflected in body composition and reproductive cycles in females. Puberty varies in onset and duration and both of these variables depend on body condition. Consequently, this time period is of special interest in the study of programming.

The aim of our study was to examine the effects of prenatal stress during early- to mid-gestation on later development and reproductive performance of offspring. Because prenatal stress might act as a cue of harsh environmental conditions, we expected long-term effects of prenatal stress on both variables. If conditions are harsh and survival is at risk, it might be advantageous to invest in fast reproductive maturation even if at the cost of growth (see Gluckman and Hanson 2004). Therefore, we predicted reduced growth, increased sexual hormone levels and an earlier puberty in prenatally stressed as compared to control animals. This would enable prenatally stressed offspring to increase reproductive output during the first year of life. Glucocorticoids are believed to play a pivotal role in transferring maternal cues to the fetus. In mice, glucocorticoid transfer via the placenta is higher in female compared to male fetuses (Montano et al. 1993). If a similar sex difference exists in guinea pigs, we would predict more pronounced effects in females.

Materials and methods

The institutional ethics committee and the Austrian Federal Ministry of Science and Research approved and permitted all husbandry and experimental procedures performed for this work (GZ 68.205/0211-II/10b/2008).

Animals and housing conditions

All animals could be identified by differences in natural coat pattern. A detailed description of animals, housing conditions and procedure of prenatal stress treatment can be found in Schöpper et al. (2011, 2012). Briefly, prenatally stressed and control guinea pigs (Cavia aperea f. porcellus, multicolored, bred in our facilities) of both sexes (prenatally stressed (PS), n = 20, ♀8 ♂12 and control, n = 24, ♀11 ♂13) were monitored for growth from birth to day 124, as well as sex hormone development from day 12 to 124. Mothers of PS animals were stressed once per week for 3 h (09:00–11:00 a.m. and 04:00–05:00 p.m.) with strobe light exposure in an unfamiliar dark room during early- to mid-pregnancy (from 1 week prior to conception until day 42 of gestation, total duration of gestation: ~68 days). They reacted with a short-term increase and a long-term downregulation of glucocorticoids and attenuated body weight during gestation, but no effect on reproductive effort (Schöpper et al. 2011). Mothers of control offspring were left undisturbed. After sexual maturation, females were allowed to mate with unrelated males to establish an F2 generation beginning with the sixth cycle of each individual female. Therefore, the receptive female was placed in the male’s cage for half an hour, if necessary repeatedly until copulation was observed or a copulatory plug was found. All animals were housed under standard conditions either in the same-sex groups or mothers with their litter from approximately 3 days prior to parturition (to avoid disturbance by partner females during course of birth) until weaning of the pups at an age of 21 days. As conception resulted from naturally occurring estrus of females, parturition was not synchronous in all litters. To minimize age differences in newly arranged groups of weaned offspring, animals were housed in the same-sex groups of two to four animals (ground area of enclosure: 100 cm × 50 cm, similar for both treatment groups). Feeding took place at 09:00 a.m. and comprised 15 g of standard chow (Altromin 3013, Altromin GmbH, Lage, Germany), 40 g of fresh fruit/vegetables and a handful of hay per animal as well as water ad libitum. As animals were housed in groups, we cannot exclude that individual feed intake varied between cage mates. Nevertheless, by only housing the animals from the same group together (either PS or control), the total amount per cage/group was always the same and we could exclude possible differences in individual feed intake to be reflected in our results analyzed by group. During the third trimester of gestation and during lactation, mothers received an additional 20 g of pellets, while pups were offered 10 g of standard chow and 10 g of fresh food/day.

Body weight and reproductive measurements

Body weight was determined accurately to 0.01 g from birth (maximum 2 h after birth) until weaning at day 21 and to the closest 0.5 g afterward. Measures were taken during the morning before feeding, starting with the day of birth followed by weekly intervals until weaning (day 0, 7, 14, 21) and again at weekly intervals from day 26 until day 124. We define puberty as the time period of increasing gonadal hormones for both sexes; in females external signs of estrus can be used to define onset of reproductive cycling. The reproductive stage of females was checked on a daily basis by visual inspection of the vaginal membrane, which is a suitable external marker of cycle stage in guinea pigs (Stockard and Papanicolaou 1917; Touma et al. 2001). The first day of a fully ruptured membrane was defined as estrus (day 0 of the cycle), which correlates well with physiological estrus (Young 1937). The duration of a cycle was determined by counting days between estrus (day 0) of one cycle and estrus (day 0) of the successive cycle. The day of conception (day 0 of gestation) was defined either as the day when mating was observed or when a vaginal plug was found. In three cases (one in the control and two in the PS group), when we could observe neither, the last day of fully ruptured vaginal membrane was defined as day 0 of gestation. The expected day of birth was gestational day 68. Parturition could be observed directly in nine control and six PS females. Out of these females, in three control and four PS females, total placentae could be collected and weighed within 5 min accurate to 0.01 g after cutting adjacent membranes. The gestational effort of females was calculated after Laurien-Kehnen and Trillmich (2004) as total litter weight (g)/maternal body weight at conception (g) × 100. Three newborn pups were found dead in the cage when birth had not been observed directly. All three pups originated from different litters (two control and one PS female). Those animals were included in measurements of reproductive performance to assess actual maternal effort during gestation.

Blood sampling and analysis of plasma progesterone and testosterone

To determine the concentrations of sexual hormones, blood samples were taken by puncturing marginal ear vessels and collecting the emerging blood in a heparinized capillary tube. This procedure is described by Sachser and Pröve (1984), takes less than 3 min per animal and can be performed without anesthesia. Reproductive maturation of females is characterized by the onset of the first estrus. In contrast, for males, adolescence is not a single event and is best described by increased plasma testosterone levels between days 40 and 80 (Rigaudière et al. 1976; Bauer et al. 2008a). Thus, it was necessary to use different sample regimes for males and females.

The following blood sampling regimes were performed for females: weekly intervals prior to the first estrus (d12, d19, d26, d33, d40) and every third day during the first, third and fifth reproductive cycle (Cy1d0, Cy1d3, …, Cy1d18; Cy3d0, …, Cy5d18). Progesterone was measured as a major female sex hormone fluctuating during the estrous cycle with peaks during mid-luteal phase. We abstained from measuring estrogens as large volumes of blood are needed for the analysis which could have affected the animal’s well-being (Bauer et al. 2008b). In addition, according to Graham and Clarke (1997), progesterone is probably the more important gonadal steroid for the regulation of reproductive cycles and female fertility. Supplementarily, we measured the levels of testosterone, as guinea pig females have been described to show increased levels of testosterone when being raised by mothers that experienced social instability during the time of gestation until the end of the lactational period (Kaiser et al. 2003). For analysis of testosterone in females, only samples of the days of estrus after puberty were used to avoid bias by, and influences of, the phase of reproductive cycle on hormone concentrations.

In males, testosterone is the primary sex hormone and was measured to monitor reproductive maturation. The weekly sampling regime was started the same day as in females and continued until the age of 4 months (d12, d19, d26, d33, …, d124). With this protocol, the same age range was covered in both males and females, while sex-specific characteristics were still addressed.

To minimize the influence of diurnal fluctuations of hormonal levels, all blood sampling took place at the same time of day (starting at 9:00 h). Samples (400 μl in females and 100 μl in males) were centrifuged immediately after collection and the plasma stored at −20 °C until further analysis. We extracted plasma (diluted in assay buffer to a total of 500 μl) with 5 ml of diethyl ether and measured progesterone and testosterone with an enzyme immunoassay (EIA). Details of the EIA, including cross-reactions of the antibody, are given by Schwarzenberger et al. (1996) for the measurement of progesterone concentration. The intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were 10.4 and 12.8 %, respectively. For the analysis of testosterone, we used an assay described by Palme and Möstl (1994). The intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were 7.9 and 12.9 %, respectively. All samples were run in duplicate.

Statistical analysis

Analysis of data was performed with R 2.9.1 (R Developmental Core Team 2007) with the additional packages nlme and multcomp (Pinheiro et al. 2007; Hothorn et al. 2008) and tested two-tailed.

For the analysis of body weight in offspring, we used a linear mixed effects (LME) model with time (used as a 2nd order polynomial as the quadratic term was significant), litter size (one to five pups per litter), sex (females and males) and group (control and PS) as fixed factors. Animal was entered as random factor to adjust for repeated measurements, and mother was entered as random factor to adjust for relatedness between litter mates. All interactions were included at first and the model then reduced stepwise by exclusion of non-significant interactions.

For the analysis of reproductive parameters in females, the onset of first estrus and body weight at this point of time (one female had to be excluded from analysis due to unclear onset of reproductive cycles) as well as reproductive performance were analyzed using LME models with group (control and PS) as fixed factor and mother as random factor. For the analysis of cycle length, additionally the number of the cycle was included as fixed factor and animal as random factor. Post hoc comparison of groups at the different cycles was performed to unravel significant interactions. Sex ratio of F2 offspring was analyzed using Fisher’s exact test.

Differences in concentrations of plasma progesterone in females were tested using an LME model with day, group and—only in cycling females—the number of the cycle as fixed factors, and animal and mother as random factors. Post hoc comparisons were performed to analyze significant group:cycle interactions.

Analysis of plasma testosterone concentrations both in F1 males and F1 females was carried out using an LME model with day and group as fixed factors and animal as random factor. As inclusion of mother as the second random factor increased model complexity but did not decrease residual deviance, we used the model without this random factor. For data collected during reproductive maturity of females, the LME model for testosterone included the number of the cycle and group as fixed factors and both mother and animal as random factors.

All response variables were Box–Cox transformed for the analysis if necessary for normal distributions of residuals. Achievement of this was checked via visual inspection of histograms, qq plots and random distribution of fitted values. Results are presented as F values with degrees of freedom and corresponding p value. If both the interaction and the main effect were significant, only interactions were the subject of interpretation. Note that the numerator degrees of freedom of 2 for the time effect indicate the use of a quadratic polynomial. The observed differences were considered significant at p values <0.05.

Results

Body weight

Body weight increase from birth to the age of 124 days was significantly influenced by litter size, with animals from large litters growing more slowly than those from small ones. Additionally, body weight differed significantly between groups over the course of time, with sex as an influencing factor (LME: time:sex:group: F 2/1615 = 22.482, p < 0.001). In males, body weight development was similar in PS and control animals (Fig. 1a). Among females, however, despite similar birth weight, PS animals put on significantly more weight over the course of time than control females (Fig. 1b).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Body weight development during the first 4 months (124 days) according to group (mean ± SEM) a for F1 males (control, open circles, n = 13; PS, closed circles, n = 12) and b for F1 females (control, open circles, n = 11; PS, closed circles, n = 8)

Reproductive parameters

In females, onset of the first estrus occurred somewhat earlier in PS than control animals, at a mean age of 34 days in PS-compared with 41 days in control animals. However, the difference between 1(LME: group: F 1/9 = 1.447, p = 0.260).

Body weight was not significantly different between groups at this point of time (mean ± SEM: control 344.8 ± 7.7, PS 347.9 ± 19.1, LME: group: F 1/9 = 0.444, p = 0.522).

Duration of the first five cycles was significantly different between groups depending on the cycle number. The length of cycles was relatively constant in PS females, while in control animals, the first cycle was longer and the following cycles were slightly shorter than in the PS animals (Fig. 2, LME: number of cycle:group: F 4/67 = 4.087, p < 0.005). Post hoc test between groups over the cycles revealed a significant difference for the second cycle, with control females showing shorter cycle length than PS females (post hoc: Cy1C–Cy1PS: p = 0.229, Cy2C–Cy2PS: p = 0.003, Cy3C–Cy3PS: p = 0.574, Cy4C–Cy4PS: p = 0.1, Cy5C–Cy5PS: p = 0.512).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Duration of the first five estrus cycles (mean ± SEM) of F1 females according to group (control, open circles, n = 11; PS, closed circles, n = 8)

All mated females conceived and maintained pregnancy until natural parturition. None of the reproductive parameters measured was significantly different between PS and control group (LME’s: p > 0.2).

Hormonal data

Concentration of progesterone and testosterone in females

Prior to the first estrus, levels of progesterone rose with time and were significantly lower in PS compared to control females (Fig. 3a, LME: day: F 3/56 = 5.234, p = 0.003, group: F 1/7 = 6.397, p = 0.039). During the first, third and fifth cycle, progesterone concentrations differed significantly between days of cycle and between groups, both depending on the number of the cycle (Fig. 3b–d, LME: cycle:day: F 12/349 = 2.701, p = 0.002, cycle:group: F 2/349 = 4.923, p = 0.008).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Plasma progesterone concentrations (mean ± SEM) of F1 females separated by group (control, open circles; PS, closed circles) a during the prepubertal phase (n (control) = 11/11/11/9 + 6, n (PS) = 8/8/7/5 + 3) and b–d over the course of the first, third and fifth cycle (n (control) = 11 (except Cy3d6, Cy5d0/d18 n = 10 and Cy3d18 n = 8), n (PS) = 8 (except Cy1d0/d6 n = 7)

Generally, levels of progesterone varied over the course of the cycle, starting with levels <1 ng/ml plasma at estrus and rising thereafter to more than threefold measures at day 9 and declined again to baseline levels for the next estrus. This time course of progesterone levels was less pronounced during the first cycle than the following ones. Especially in control females, progesterone levels did not increase as much as in PS females, during the first cycle, who showed already almost similar peak progesterone values compared to the following cycles. However, post hoc comparison of mean progesterone levels per cycle between groups revealed no significant differences between groups during each cycle (post hoc: Cy1C–Cy1PS: p = 0.120; Cy3C–Cy3PS: p = 0.855, Cy5C–Cy5PS: p = 0.991).

Concentrations of testosterone in females during the first 40 days of life did not differ significantly between groups, but varied over time with mean levels of testosterone ranging between 25 and 85 pg/ml plasma (Fig. 4, LME: day: F 1/57 = 9.991, p = 0.003, group: F 1/17 = 0.424, p = 0.523). Over the course of the first six cycles, concentrations of testosterone increased significantly, with PS females showing consistently higher levels than control females. However, this difference did not reach significance (LME: cycle: F 5/89 = 4.090, p = 0.002; group: F 1/10 = 2.298, p = 0.161).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Plasma testosterone concentrations (mean ± SEM) of F1 females separated by group (control, open circles; PS, closed circles) over the course of prepubertal phase and over the days of estrus of the first six cycles (n (control) = 11/11/11/9/5//11 except Cy5 n = 10, n (PS) = 8/8/7/5/3//8 except Cy1 n = 7)

Concentration of testosterone in males

In males, the levels of testosterone revealed no significant difference between control and PS animals between day 12 and 124. In both groups, overall levels of testosterone increased with a peak at day 75 and subsequently decreased to moderate levels again (Fig. 5, LME: day: F 16/357 = 11.441, p < 0.001; group: F 1/23 = 1.161, p = 0.292).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Plasma testosterone concentrations (mean ± SEM) over the course of the first 4 months (124 days) of F1 males separated by group (control, open circles, n = 13 except d12, 33, 110 n = 12 and d40, 47, 68, 89, 96 n = 11; PS, closed circles, n = 12, except d33, 54, 96, 110 n = 11, d26, 40 n = 10 and d89, 124 n = 9)

Discussion

Our prenatal stress treatment (i.e., exposure of the pregnant mothers to strobe light during the first two-thirds of gestation once a week in an unfamiliar dark room) showed comprehensive effects in female, but negligible effects in male guinea pigs. In females, body weight and reproductive parameters were altered, indicating advanced maturity in prenatally stressed animals. In contrast, among males no significant effects of prenatal stress on body weight or hormonal measures were found.

The stress treatment of the pregnant mothers resulted in a short-term increase in mothers’ cortisol levels, but in a long-term downregulation of mothers’ hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Schöpper et al. 2011). Along with these hormonal changes, maternal weight gain was reduced during gestation in stressed compared to control dams. Maternal behavior and feeding offspring were also slightly altered as stressed mothers showed slightly higher levels of aggression and milk yield was reduced even though offspring suckled for a longer duration (for details see Klaus 2010; Schöpper et al. 2011). We therefore assume that the effects seen in the F1 generation are not only related to the original strobe light exposure of the dams, but rather to the stress treatment with all its effects on the physiology and behavior of the mother. It is clear that chronic moderate stress does not affect a single parameter and all remaining functions remain unaffected, rather glucocorticoids interfere with almost any physiological function and therefore the whole stress–effect complex is addressed here. This is an approach taking the biological unit—the whole animal—into account, instead of trying to split mechanistic pathways that actually interact. The stress treatment of pregnant mothers was capable of having long-term effects on the F1 generation, changing characteristics of growth and reproduction.

Body weight

Animals deriving from large litters grew slower compared to those from small litters. This inverse relationship between litter size and body weight has been shown for guinea pigs before (Fey and Trillmich 2008). Prenatal crowding conditions in the womb are believed to be the cause for these differences in early body weight. Furthermore, this difference depending on litter size could be shown to even increase over time in rats probably due to competition for milk as an energy resource (Chahoud and Paumgartten 2009). Independent of litter size, we found a treatment effect on offspring body weight development as a function of sex. For males, body weight did not differ significantly between prenatally stressed and control animals. This is in accordance with the findings of Kemme et al. (2007, 2008) and Banjanin et al. (2004) who likewise did not find any differences in growth of male guinea pig offspring after prenatal doses of synthetic glucocorticoids or social instability. However, if stress was induced during late gestation, prenatally stressed male guinea pigs were lighter than their control counterparts (Kapoor and Matthews 2005; Emack et al. 2008). The time and origin of stress imposition has already been described as crucial for certain effects (Kapoor and Matthews 2005) and thus are believed to be the cause of the different findings.

In females, however, we found significant differences in body weight development depending on the group. However, the differences were in the opposite direction than we had predicted. Prenatally stressed females put on more and not less weight over time compared to control females. This highly significant result is noteworthy as it shows a distinct sex-specific effect and has not been described for female guinea pigs before. There is some literature showing reduced body weight after prenatal disturbances, especially right after birth, but also findings of increased weight gain are described [lower: e.g., Ikegami et al. 1997 (lambs); Schneider et al. 1999 (primates); McCabe et al. 2001 (guinea pigs); higher: Götz et al. 2008 (rats)]. Götz et al. (2008) for example investigated prenatal stress treatment in rats and found increased body weights in stressed animals comparable to our results, however, in both sexes. Sex-specific effects have been described for female rat offspring with prenatally stressed females putting on more weight over time compared to the control females (Mueller and Bale 2006). One of many possible ideas to explain sex differences and the assumed increased vulnerability of females to prenatal stress is given by Montano et al. (1993). The authors described higher transfer of maternal glucocorticoids via the placenta to female fetus compared to male offspring. Due to our study design, it was not possible to cross-foster the pups, so that mother–pup interaction might also influence offspring’s physiology. Milk intake could be argued as a critical factor influencing growth curves in juveniles (Götz et al. 2008). Yet, Klaus (2010) found no differences in the overall milk yield between groups, but found a slightly lower milk yield on lactational day 12 in PS mothers. As both sexes as well as both groups were fed the same solid diet and we still see a highly significant effect on PS females, we doubt the influence of single day differences in milk intake to be decisive.

We rather suppose the offspring to differ in energy metabolism. Glucocorticoids play a modulating role in weight gain, because neural pathways of stress response and energy homeostasis are tightly linked (Phillips et al. 1998; Nyrienda and Seckl 1998; Nieuwenhuizen and Rutters 2008). There are studies analyzing the effects of prenatal stress on later metabolism [Lesage et al. 2004 (no effect on later body weight, but insulin resistance in rats), D’mello and Liu 2006 (no effect), Mueller and Bale, 2006 (effects seen after stress during mid to late pregnancy in males only and also leptin and glucose measurements were affected)]. We think it possible that the early prenatal stress in our study may have been able to alter metabolic pathways that lead to better energy utilization, especially in females. From studies of undernourishment in humans and animals—for which also glucocorticoids are believed to transfer nutritional cues to the developing brain—it is known that especially early gestation is a crucial time window for programming later body weight of offspring and risk of obesity (Ravelli et al. 1976; Symonds et al. 2007).

According to the mismatch hypothesis of Gluckman and Hanson (2004), if postnatal conditions do not match what was predicted prenatally, effects can become accelerated and even be detrimental at long term. Concerning growth, prenatal deprivation will favor an offspring phenotype that is adapted to poor conditions and is able to cope with low amounts of food. Therefore, metabolic rate will decrease and energy storage will be improved. This phenotype is well suited for harsh conditions—but what if the predicted future is better than expected? The mismatch of predicted and actual condition can lead to a phenotype that is at higher risk to develop obesity, diabetes and cardiovascular disease—taken together as metabolic disease (Gluckman et al. 2008b). Postnatal conditions in our study met all physiological needs of a young guinea pig and therewith were presumably better than the ones predicted by the PS animals. Without intention we might have created a mismatch that led to the PS females putting on significantly more weight than the control animals. We could speculate that the sex difference in body weight of PS animals is due to higher fat mass in females. Stored fat can be directly converted to milk during reproductive activity (Valencak et al. 2009) and might therefore be advantageous especially for females. More mechanistic studies are needed to evaluate this assumption and possible translatability for human medical conditions like obesity and metabolic diseases.

Reproductive parameters

While we found that females of both groups were similar in body weight at first estrus, PS females were younger compared with their control counterparts at this point of time. The reasons why the results did not reach significance were speculated to follow from the high inter-individual variability (range: control 28–55, PS 23–42). According to Trillmich et al. (2006), there is no critical body mass to be obtained for first estrus in guinea pigs. However, the steeper the growth curve until weaning, the earlier onset of estrus occurred in their study (Trillmich et al. 2006). The same might be true in our study, with faster growing PS females being slightly younger at first estrus than control females.

Female guinea pigs are theoretically able to reproduce from the very first cycle onward (Rood and Weir 1970; Trillmich et al. 2006), but successful mating is dependent on normal cycle length (15–17 days) which the majority of females do not reach until the second or third cycle (Rood and Weir 1970). In our study, cycle length was influenced by group in interaction with the number of cycle. In PS females, cycle length was almost constant for all six measured cycles, lasting around 17 days. Control females however had a longer first cycle of a mean over 18 days and slightly shorter subsequent cycles compared to PS females. As proper functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal axis is crucial for regular cycling, we assume hormonal cues to be more mature in PS females during the first cycle (see below).

Concerning reproductive performance of females, there was no indication for differences between groups. None of the parameters taken was significantly different in PS versus control females, which is in accordance with the findings in rats (Götz et al. 2008). There are reports of reduced reproductive success in prenatally stressed female mink and mice (e.g., Politch and Herrenkohl 1984; Jeppesen and Heller 1986). In guinea pigs, however—to the best of our knowledge—no comparable studies are available. Species differences in vulnerability as well as timing, intensity and type of stressor are crucial factors that may influence the variability of the results described.

Hormonal data

Concentration of progesterone and testosterone in females

In our study, concentrations of progesterone were very low before onset of first estrus. Progesterone generally starts to rise during reproductive maturation. It reflects the growth of corpora lutea of the ovaries, showing the highest levels during the mid-luteal phase around days 8 and 9 (Feder et al. 1968; Challis et al. 1971; Blatchley et al. 1976). During the first cycle, the levels of progesterone do not yet approximate the levels of subsequent mature cycles and are normally at medium levels (Westfahl and Vekasy 1988). This was also the case in the control females of our study, whereas PS females showed higher levels of progesterone both prior to first estrus and during the first cycle. Bauer et al. (2009) found a similar situation in guinea pig offspring whose mothers were fed a low quality diet during gestation. Both low protein diet as well as stress exposure of the mothers should act as cues of harsh environments, which might adapt their offspring to the conditions foreseen and therefore accelerate maturation. For both, nutritional interventions and stressful events, a common mechanism in transfer of programming effects is suspected to involve glucocorticoids (Seckl 2004). As cycle length of guinea pigs can be shortened by administration of progesterone during the first 6 days of the cycle (Woody et al. 1967), the elevated levels of progesterone levels found in our PS females may be the reason for the shorter first cycle.

We further tested for testosterone levels in females, as an increase in the levels of this hormone is associated with the disposition to masculinization that has been described in prenatally stressed female guinea pigs (Sachser and Kaiser 1996; Kaiser et al. 2003). Testosterone was also increased in prenatally stressed girls when mothers were pregnant at the time of the Chernobyl disaster (Huizink et al. 2008). However, no such differences could be seen in our females.

Concentration of testosterone in males

In contrast to the distinctive differences in hormonal levels between prenatally stressed and control animals found in females, no such effects could be seen in testosterone levels of males. These results are in accordance with earlier programming studies in guinea pigs, using prenatal stress treatment as well as nutritional challenge (Banjanin et al. 2004; Kemme et al. 2008; Bauer et al. 2009). However, also contradictory findings are available for this species. While increased testosterone levels were apparent in male offspring of mothers that experienced social instability during whole gestation or until weaning (Kaiser et al. 2003; Kemme et al. 2007), Kapoor and Matthews (2005) described decreased levels after strobe light treatment during late gestation. The inconsistency of the results may be due to different stressors used and the time windows during which stress was applied. Furthermore, it might point to more complex interrelations than we are currently able to address and further investigations are needed, especially in the mechanistic approach to evaluate the way of programming testosterone levels in males.

Conclusions

In summary, we could show in this study that prenatal stress during early- to mid-gestation has distinctive effects on hormonal traits of reproductive function in guinea pig offspring. The effects seen proved to be highly sex specific and point to a different biological relevance for males and females.

For males, prenatal stress did not affect growth or levels of gonadal steroids. In females, on the other hand, prenatally stressed individuals showed significantly advanced growth, a slightly earlier onset of estrus and more mature first estrus cycle, probably indicating advanced competence to reproduce. If a female in good condition is additionally early in estrus and the first cycle is more mature compared to the control animals, the potential to successfully reproduce is multiplied. It would be of great interest to compare the actual reproductive output at such an early stage of maturation to assess the biological relevance of such differences between groups. Our animals, however, were not mated before the sixth cycle, when both groups had normal cycle lengths and comparable levels of progesterone. This may explain why reproductive performance per se was not different between groups.

Taken together, our results suggest that prenatal experience of harsh environmental conditions via increased maternal stress might predict a postnatal continuation of such a situation. In line with our findings in females, evidence from animal and human studies indicates that poor conditions during fetal development can lead to premature offspring (Cooper et al. 1996; Gluckman and Hanson 2006; Sloboda et al. 2007). The biological relevance of an early onset of reproduction is evident, especially if the overall expectation for lifespan is expected to be poor, as lifetime reproductive success might be improved if females start reproducing early.

However, when looking at long-term consequences of prenatal disturbances, especially beyond the age of reproductive activity as is the case in humans, negative effects of hormonal alterations may be anticipated. Epidemiological as well as experimental studies indicate that diseases like diabetes, obesity, coronary heart disease and stress reactivity are possibly triggered by prenatal programming (Entringer et al. 2008; Barker 2002; Lesage et al. 2004; Darnaudéry and Maccari 2008). In the majority of cases endocrine function, especially HPA function, is affected, thereby altering physiology and probably accounting for later diseases (review: Bertram and Hanson 2002). Yet, apart from such pathological long-term consequences of poor prenatal condition as seen in our study, shorter-term effects that are rather adaptive for the predicted conditions may well be advantageous.