Introduction

Male fitness depends on traits that maximize mating and/or fertilization success. Such traits often include coercive or manipulative traits that induce females to mate and/or reproduce at a rate that may benefit the males expressing them, even if they have detrimental side-effects to their mates (Parker 1979, 2006; Johnstone and Keller 2000; Chapman et al. 2003; Morrow et al. 2003; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Queller and Strassmann 2018). While a locus that expresses such male benefiting trait evolves due to male specific selection, female specific selection, on the other hand, leads to counteradaptation at a second locus that expresses female specific traits that allow females to either bypass or resist male coercion (Wigby and Chapman 2004; Nandy et al. 2013a; Dougherty et al. 2017; Chapman 2018; Rostant et al. 2020). Such evolutionary conflict between male trait loci and female trait loci is commonly referred to as interlocus sexual conflict (Chapman et al. 2003). Herein, both sexes are selected to evolve sex specific traits often resulting in antagonistic co-evolution between the sexes (Parker 1979; Rice 1992, 1996; Civetta and Singh 1995; Arnqvist and Rowe 2002, 2005; Rönn et al. 2007). As interlocus sexual conflict continues, it may result in the evolution of male competitive traits such as persistent courtship, extravagant display, traumatic insemination, deception and seminal fluid proteins (Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Koene 2012). For example, Drosophila melanogaster males overwhelm the females with incessant courtship, which often involve chasing, attempted mounting and other forms of physical interactions (von Schilcher and Dow 1977) that negatively impact female survival. Furthermore, a copulating male transfers a complex cocktail of peptides/proteins (seminal fluid proteins), which increase egg production in females and make them less receptive to future mating (Wolfner 1997). The side effects of these physiological alterations by these seminal fluid proteins are increased mortality (Chapman et al. 1995) and may even result in significant reduction in lifetime progeny output (Stewart et al. 2005). This detrimental effect of the males on their mates is often termed mate harm (Jiang et al. 2011; Nandy et al. 2013a, b; MacPherson et al. 2018).

In the absence of female counteradaptation, the negative fitness impact of mate harm can potentially lead to extinction of a population/species, an outcome dubbed as the ‘tragedy of commons’ (Le Galliard et al. 2005; Rankin and Kokko 2006; Rankin et al. 2007). However, such an extreme outcome of interlocus sexual conflict is unusual, because of, at least, three reasons. First, as mentioned earlier, female counteradaptation to mate harm can result in a chase-away process where evolution of mate harming ability of males is neutralized by the emergence of female resistance (Holland and Rice 1998, 1999; Wigby and Chapman 2004; Friberg 2005; Rankin et al. 2011; Dougherty et al. 2017; Snow et al. 2019). Secondly, traits that inflict mate harm, i.e. the sexually antagonistic male traits, are energetically expensive to express and, hence, increase in such traits is constrained by trade-offs involving costly life history traits (Wedell et al. 2006; Bonduriansky et al. 2008; Adler and Bonduriansky 2014; Lemaître et al. 2020). Thirdly, survivorship pattern and scheduling of reproduction can also put additional constraints on the evolution of mate harm, for example by restricting mating system and breeding ecology (Mital et al. 2021, 2022). The latter two theories are a more recent development in the field, and we focus on them in the present investigation.

Contrary to a long-standing perception, males pay a non-trivial reproductive cost (Partridge and Farquhar 1981; Kotiaho and Simmons 2003; Lane et al. 2010). If a male invests greater proportion of the finite amount of available resources in such reproductive traits, the potential to invest on other traits, such as soma maintenance, stress resistance physiology and immunity, is expected to reduce. This results in a negative correlation between investment in reproductive traits and these traits defining the cost of reproduction for males. Competitive traits including behaviour such as courtship and copulation (Cordts and Partridge 1996; Clutton-Brock and Langley 1997; Bretman et al. 2013) and physiological traits such as synthesis of a functional ejaculate (Dewsbury 1982; Andersson 1994; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005) are energetically expensive. Hence, male competitive traits constitute a significant part of the total reproductive cost. A comparative study on mammals indicated a significant association between degree of sexual selection on males and male biased reproductive cost (Promislow 1992). Experimental relaxation of sexual selection by enforced monogamy led to reduced investment in such costly competitive traits, including ejaculate composition, in D. melanogaster males and incidentally turned such males into less harming mates (Pitnick et al. 2001). Trade-off between competitive traits and somatic maintenance is a major contributor to the reproductive cost in males (Bonduriansky et al. 2008). Therefore, investment in longevity traits is expected to constrain the expression of male reproductive traits (Partridge and Farquhar 1981; Stearns 1989; Cordts and Partridge 1996; Clutton-Brock and Langley 1997). Life history theory predicts that compared to populations with shorter lifespan, males in populations with longer lifespan (i.e. higher investment in somatic maintenance) may have limited potential to invest in reproductive traits (Rose and Charlesworth 1981; Ernsting and Isaaks 1991; Kirkwood and Rose 1991; Service PM 1993; Tatar et al. 1993; Kotiaho 2001; Hunt et al. 2004). Though, it is not clear whether lifespan evolution is caused by changes in reproductive traits or the other way round or both. Nonetheless, on an average, populations with shorter male lifespan are expected to have the potential to invest more in reproductive traits, including competitive traits involved in inflicting incidental mate harm, especially when there is ample selection for increased male competitive ability. Interestingly, existing evidences suggest that variation in lifespan and aging rate among males appears to reflect variation in reproductive investment (Alcock 1996; Cordts and Partridge 1996; Clutton-Brock and Langley 1997; Prowse and Partridge 1997; Hunt et al. 2004; Bonduriansky and Brassil 2005). Whether such correlation can also be extended to include sexually antagonistic male traits is not clear.

Scheduling of reproduction along with adult lifespan can directly modulate the extent of sexual conflict by constraining the breeding ecology. For example, a semelparous species with only one breeding season may tend to be monogamous and, hence, experience reduced sexual conflict compared to an iteroparous species (Montrose et al. 2004; Clutton-Brock 2017; Griffith 2019). Long et al. (2010) showed that in a set of Drosophila laboratory populations, which are effectively semelparous, timing of mating from the semelparous breeding window could significantly alter the sexually antagonistic outcomes in females. Bonduriansky (2014) showed that change in background mortality rate, resulting from increased predation pressure, could result in a short-term relaxation of sexual conflict. Population with low female life expectancy, increased mate harm by males is expected to bring down the average fitness of the population (Rankin and Kokko 2006) creating a scenario where selection can act against mate harming traits. If shorter lifespan evolves as a result of selection of components of life history (for example, faster pre-adult development and maturation), it can significantly shorten the potential duration of male–female encounter, thereby reducing sexual selection and conflict in a population (Mital et al. 2021, 2022) . There are several clear evidences supporting the notion that male–female encounter rate is, indeed, an important determinant of variation in interlocus sexual conflict across populations. For example, change in (a) the complexity of the physical environment (Byrne et al. 2008; Yun et al. 2017), (b) thermal environment that alters various activities in males (García-Roa et al. 2019) and (c) community structure that alters male–female encounter rate (Gomez-Llano et al. 2018) have been found to alter the level of mate harm in a population.

Here, we investigated the evolution of sexually antagonistic male traits in a set of experimentally evolved populations of D. melanogaster having substantially faster pre-adult development and reduced lifespan compared to their ancestors due to selection for faster development and early reproduction for over 1000 generations. These selected populations — ACO (accelerated CO) — will be hereafter referred as ‘faster aging population’ or ‘evolved populations’ and their ancestral populations — CO (control derived from Os, which themselves were a set of populations selected for reproduction at an Old age) — as ‘ancestral or control population’. The details of the history of these populations can be found in Chippindale et al. (1997). The faster aging populations have been extensively investigated for a range of life history traits since the early 1990s. As a direct response to selection, faster aging populations evolved ~ 20% reduction in pre-adult development time and > 18% reduction in mean life expectancy (Chippindale et al. 1997). Moreover, the maintenance regime of these populations is such that their effective adult lifespan is quite dramatically short (see ‘Methods’). In effect, ACO life history has evolved to adopt the so-called live fast-die young life history. Interestingly, in their short adult life, there is a substantial scope for scramble competition for mating. However, selection under such an ecology can be expected to operate on variation that affects early-life fitness components, including competitive ability and courtship performance. Given that investment in the physiology of somatic maintenance has significantly reduced (as a result of shorter lifespan), theories would, thus, predict a resource re-allocation, whereby male early-life competitive traits are preferentially allocated. However, apart from significantly reduced life expectancy, ACO flies are also visibly smaller in size — possibly due to the effect of selection for faster development. Therefore, compared to the controls, these faster aging populations also have significantly reduced total resource that is available for allocation in different physiological processes, including those connected to the expression of competitive traits. Thus, pertaining to the evolution of mate harming ability of faster aging males, we have two contrasting predictions.

To test the above-mentioned theories, we first quantified mate harm inflicted by the control and evolved males on a standard female type. Then, we specifically compared mating behaviour of these males to assess the divergence in the physical component of mate harassment. Apart from males from the evolved faster aging populations and their matched controls, we used a third treatment where experimental control males were phenocopied to match the size of the faster aging males. This third treatment was used to tease out the effects of genetically evolved difference in mate harm, and that cause is solely due to reduced size. We used a standard laboratory line, Oregon R, as the common female background against which mate harming ability of the treatment males was compared. Importantly, we compared all the measured traits across the evolved and ancestral populations at five different age points to assess the evolved difference in age-specific expression of the sexually antagonistic male traits.

Methods

As mentioned above, the investigation reported here used ten laboratory populations of Drosophila melanogaster — five evolved ACO populations and their matched ancestral CO populations. These populations were kindly provided to us by Prof. Michael R. Rose of University of California, Irvine, USA. They were maintained for 20 (CO) and 80 (ACO) generations in our laboratory before starting the experiment. The details of population history can be found in Chippindale et al. (1997). A brief description is provided in Figure S1 in the supplementary information. On November 1991, five replicate ancestral populations — CO1–5 (subscript refers to replicate identity) — were used to initiate five replicate populations selected for accelerated development, viz. ACO1–5 (Rose et al. 1992). Ancestral CO populations are maintained on a 4-week discrete generation cycle, under 24-h light, 25 °C (± 1), ~ 80% relative humidity on standard banana-jaggery-yeast medium. Larval density is controlled at ~ 70 per 8 ml medium in each culture vial by culturing the desired number of eggs in growth vials. Forty such vials constitute a population. On day 12 following egg culture, adults from all 40 vials are transferred to a population cage. The CO flies take about 9–10 days to complete pre-adult development, and therefore, by day 12, virtually all surviving flies finish development and are in adult stage. A population cage is supplied with food on a petri dish. From day 14 of the generation cycle, an old food plate is replaced with a fresh one every alternate day until day 24. On day 26, food plate smeared with ad lib amount of yeast paste (paste made by dissolving baker’s yeast granules in water) is provided in the cage, replacing the old food. Approximately 48 h following this, i.e. on day 28, an oviposition substrate (two pieces of food cut in a trapezium shape) is introduced inside the cage, and a window of 18 h is allowed for oviposition. Eggs deposited on the substrate, especially on the vertical slants, are collected by cutting out pieces of food with ~ 70 eggs in it. These pieces are introduced in fresh food vials to start the next generation.

The derived faster aging ACO populations employed a selection paradigm that involved strong selection for faster pre-adult development in addition to a much reduced, viz., approximately 24–36 h of adult life (Chippindale et al. 1997). At the beginning of the selection lines, selection was imposed by allowing only fastest 20% developing flies to populate a generation, followed by brief window (~ 24–36 h) of time for reproduction. Upon several hundreds of generations of selection, this maintenance was slightly modified. Their current maintenance regime involves a 9-day discrete generation cycle. The pre-adult development takes about 7–8 days. The adults are transferred to a population cage on day 8 of the generation cycle. This cage is provided with standard food pieces presented as semi-circular oviposition substrate smeared with ad lib amount of yeast paste. Eggs are collected in a manner similar to that used for the CO populations, following 24 h of introducing the flies into the cage, to start the next generation, and ending the 9 days long generation cycle. All other components of the ecology of the ACO populations are identical to those of the ancestral CO populations. The ACO populations had passed through > 1200 generations by the time the following experiments were conducted.

Generation of experimental flies

All experiments described here were done following the standard paradigm of experimental evolution assays in which both evolved and ancestral populations were passed through one generation of standard rearing, including a 14-day rearing schedule for COs and 13-day rearing schedule for ACOs, to equalize the non-genetic parental effects. All experimental flies were reared at a density of 70 eggs per 8 ml medium in a vial under the standard population maintenance regime described above. In all assays, to account for the difference in development time between evolved (ACO) and control (CO) flies (Chippindale et al. 1997), ACO eggs were collected 2 days following the collection of the CO eggs. This roughly synchronized the emergence of adult flies. Hereon, age of the adult flies refers to post-eclosion age, in days, unless mentioned otherwise.

We adopted an experimental design in which all male traits including the extent of sexually antagonistic effect of the experimental males were assessed against a common female background, which is phylogenetically unrelated to either male type, potentially equalizing the effect of female counteradaptation on the final outcome of male effect on female fitness. Furthermore, using females from an inbred line, Oregon R, as a common background, we increased the resolution of assay as such inbred females are expected to be more susceptible to mate harm (Snook 2001, Pitnick and Garcia-Gonzalez 2002). This could maximize our ability to detect even relatively smaller difference in harming ability of males across the two regimes. Eggs were collected from Oregon R line, and females were raised in the same manner as that followed for the control and evolved males stated above, including the larval density of 70 per 8 ml food in a vial. All experimental females were age-matched with the experimental males.

Past selection has resulted in a considerable reduction in the body size of the evolved ACO flies in both sexes (Passananti and Matos 2004; Burke et al. 2010; also, see body weight data in the ‘Results’ section). Since smaller males have been previously shown to be less harming to their mates (Pitnick and García–González 2002; Friberg and Arnqvist 2003), the difference in body size between evolved and control males was a confounding factor in our mate harm assay. We adopted a conservative strategy by introducing an additional treatment in our mate harm assay. In this treatment, we used smaller control (CO) males having comparable body size as evolved ACO males (hereafter referred to as phenocopied control males or CCO males). The phenocopied control males were generated by growing them at the larval density of 240 per 3 ml food in a standard vial, hence the name CCO (i.e. crowded CO). All the assays mentioned in later sections except courtship frequency and components of courtship behaviour were conducted using males belonging to three regimes — ACO, CO and CCO (i.e. evolved, control and phenocopied control). The dry body weight at eclosion of the phenocopied control males was not identical to that of the evolved (ACO) males (see dry body weight results below). However, both evolved and phenocopied control males were found to be smaller compared to the ancestral control males by a comparable degree (approximately 50–54% lighter than the control CO males). To put it simply, the evolved ACO males in our experiments were compared with the control males as well as control males which were phenocopied for smaller body size. We argue that if the difference in a trait between evolved and control regimes is qualitatively identical to that observed between the phenocopied control and control regimes, the evolved vs. control (i.e. ACO-CO) difference can be majorly attributed to the evolved body size difference. For example, if both evolved and phenocopied control males induce reduced mate harm to the experimental females compared to that induced by the control males, the evolved difference in the ACO males’ harming ability can be attributed to the smaller body size.

All the adult flies used in the experiments mentioned below were collected as virgins. At the onset of eclosion (emergence from pupal shell), virgin males and females were collected within 4–6 h of eclosion. Flies were held in groups of 10 individuals per vial with ample food until the assay setup with alternate day food change. A subset of the freshly eclosed males was frozen in − 20 °C for dry weight measurement. For every population, we measured dry weight of 50 males. Frozen males were first air-dried in 60 °C for 48 h, before being weighed in a semi-microbalance in groups of five. All adult collections were done under light CO2 anaesthesia. The design and details of the individual assays are mentioned below. A generalized experimental plan is depicted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the design main assay. Experimental subsets were generated from stock populations following one generation of standardization (common garden rearing). Experimental males were generated in the following manner: CO (i.e. control) subset was used to generate CO experimental males and CCO males (phenocopied controls, these were generated by growing larvae at a density of 240 per 3 ml of standard food) and ACO (i.e. evolved) males were generated from ACO stock. Oregon R females were generated separately under standard conditions as common female for all three regime males. On assay day, the whole set-up was divided into two male-exposure condition, i.e. single exposure (where males and females are allowed to mate once) and continuous exposure (where males and females were housed together after first mating). Trait assays were then carried out for 20 days where female mortality was recorded every day, and fecundity and courtship frequency were noted at every 5-day interval. All vials were flipped into fresh food vial at every alternate day

Assay setup

When adult flies were 1–2 days old, the assay vials were set up by combining males and females in food vials. Each of these vials, therefore, had ten males and ten females. A set of twenty vials was set up for a given population. The vials were left undisturbed for 1 h, and the flies were allowed to mate. Mating was visually observed. No video recording was done. Vials where all ten females did not successfully copulate were removed, leaving the final number of vials at this stage at 18–20 for each population. The entire set of these vials were divided into two male exposure assay conditions — (a) single mating (8–10 vials) and (b) continuous exposure (8–10 vials) treatments. In the single mating subset, after completion of the first round of mating, sexes were separated under light carbon dioxide (CO2) anaesthesia, and females were retained in the same vial while the males were discarded. The continuous exposure vials were retained without separating the sexes. Flies in these vials were also exposed to light CO2 anaesthesia to equalize the handling across the two subsets. All vials were maintained, with alternate day food change, for 20 days. In these 20 days, mortality, fecundity and behaviour components were recorded (see below). Throughout the experiment, except sorting of sexes, all other fly handling including combination of sexes and transfer of flies from spent vials to fresh food vials were done without anaesthesia.

Fecundity and mortality in test females

Female mortality was recorded daily for the all 20 days of the assay. Dead flies were removed from the vials by aspiration. Fecundity was recorded in approximately 5-day intervals, on days 1, 5, 10, 15 and 20. On each of these days, flies (only females for single mating and both sexes for continuous exposure set) from a vial were transferred to a fresh food vial (hereafter, fecundity vial) and allowed to lay eggs for a duration of ~ 24 h after which they were transferred again into a fresh food vial to continue the assay (except day 20 count). The fecundity vials were then frozen immediately to stop further development. The eggs were later counted under a microscope. Per capita fecundity on a given assay day was calculated for a vial by dividing the total number of eggs in that vial by the number of females alive at the start of that day. Per capita fecundity values from individual vials were taken as the unit of analysis. For the analysis of the mortality data, proportion of females (i.e. out of a total ten in a vial) that were recorded dead at the end of the 20-day period in a vial, i.e. cumulative female mortality, was taken as the unit of analysis.

Courtship frequency

Courtship frequency was measured as the average number of courtship bouts a male was found to perform per unit time. The continuous exposure vials were used for this purpose. Hence, courtship frequency of our treatment males was measured against Oregon R females. Since body size is not known to systematically affect courtship frequency, phenocopied control (CCO) males were excluded from this assay. Courtship measurement was done on days 2, 6, 11, 16 and 20 of the assay. On a given observation day, four observations (nc = 4) were recorded for a vial, maintaining a gap of 90 min between two subsequent observations. During an observation, a given vial was measured four times in quick succession in the following manner. A randomly picked male was observed for 15 s, during which the number of bouts of independent courtship (see later) events was counted. This was repeated four times for a vial, male selection being random each time. These four counts constituted one observation for that vial. An average for one such observation was calculated by dividing the total count by four, yielding an observation value (Ci, where i = 1, 2, 3, 4). Mean courtship frequency was calculated for a replicate vial using the following function:

$$\mathrm{Courtship}\;\mathrm{frequency}=\frac{\sum C_i}{n_c}$$

The observations were manual and did not involve any video recording. As the evolved and control flies are visibly different, blind-folded observation did not have any utility and, hence, was not adopted. Observers were well-trained to spot any of the following components of courtship behaviour: oriented toward female, following/chasing female, wing vibration, genitalia licking and attempted copulation (von Schilcher and Dow 1977). Performance of any of these behavioural components is counted as one bout of courtship as long as they were performed contiguously. During the observation, ‘two independent bouts of courtship’ is defined as (a) courtship events to two different females by the focal male or (b) two courtship events performed by the focal male to the same female separated by either the male courting a different female or showing some other behaviour in between.

Pattern of courtship behaviour in ACO and CO males

To investigate the qualitative difference in courtship behaviour evolved ACO and control CO regimes, we observed and quantified different components of courtship ritual in males in a separate trial. Courtship behaviour in male D. melanogaster is characterized by a complex series of discrete courtship components. In this assay, we quantified the frequencies of the five discrete components of courtship — (1) oriented toward female, (2) following/chasing female, (3) wing vibration, (4) genitalia licking, (5) attempted copulation (Ruedi and Hughes 2008). In addition to these five courtship components, males usually show three additional behavioural states — motionless, randomly moving and copulation, which are not part of the courtship ritual (referred here as non-components). The observation vials were set up by introducing a 1–2-day-old virgin male (either ACO or CO) and a 3–4-day-old virgin Oregon R female in a fresh food vial without using anaesthesia. We started courtship observation after approximately 90 min from the initial introduction of the pair to the observation vial. This duration is usually sufficient for all females to undergo a single copulation (Nandy and Prasad 2011; Nandy et al. 2012, 2016). During observation, the behavioural state of the male in an observation vial was recorded by instantaneous scans. A given male was observed every 30 s for 30 min, resulting in a total of 60 observations for a male. This assay was done for all ten populations, i.e. all five replicate population pairs (ACO1-5 and corresponding CO replicate populations). In each population, 60 males were observed. Multiple trained observers (authors and volunteers) carried out the observations manually. However, for a given male, all 60 observations were carried out by a single observer. Vials were randomly assigned among the observers to minimize observer bias in the data. Frequency of a component (for example, oriented toward female) for a male was calculated by using the following definition:

$$\mathrm{Component}\;\mathrm{frequency}=\frac{\left(\mathrm{total}\;\mathrm{count}\;\mathrm{of}\;\mathrm a\;\mathrm{component}\right)}{(\mathrm{total}\;\mathrm{number}\;\mathrm{of}\;\mathrm{observations})-\left(\mathrm{total}\;\mathrm{count}\;\mathrm{of}\;\mathrm{non}-\mathrm{components}\right)}$$

Thus, the assay resulted in 60 component frequency values (corresponding to the 60 males assayed for a population) for each of the five courtship components, for a population. These values were used as the unit of analysis.

Statistical analysis

Cumulative female mortality data were analysed using three-factor mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) where male regime (levels: CO, ACO and CCO) and male exposure type (levels: single mating and continuous exposure) were modelled as fixed factors, and block (levels: 1–5) was modelled as a random factor. Dry body weight and (courtship) component frequency were analysed using two-factor mixed model ANOVA, with male regime and block modelled as fixed and random factors respectively. As female mortality and component frequency data were calculated as proportion values, analysis was performed following arcsine square root transformation (Sokal and Rohlf 1995; Zar 1999). All multiple comparisons were done using Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD). All these analyses were done using Statistica (Tibco Software Inc., version 13.3).

Fecundity (i.e. per capita fecundity) data was analysed in two ways. First, per capita fecundity pooled across the five age classes, i.e. cumulative fecundity was analysed using three-factor mixed model ANOVA with male regime and male exposure type that were modelled as fixed factors, and block was treated as a random factor. Secondly, to assess the effect of continued exposure to males on female fecundity, age-specific per capita fecundity was analysed only for the continuous exposure set. Initial analysis indicated significant block-to-block variation (see the ‘Results’ section and supplementary information, Table S3). Hence, each block was separately analysed using a linear mixed-effect model in R version 3.6.1 using lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2017). In the model, per capita fecundity was modelled as response variable; male regime, age and their two-way interactions as fixed factors; and vial id (replicate vial identity) as a random factor. The model used type III sum of squares. The following model was used for analysis:

$$\mathrm{Per}\;\mathrm{capita}\;\mathrm{fecundity}\sim\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime}+\mathrm{age}+\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime}:\mathrm{age}+(1\vert\mathrm{Vial}\;\mathrm{id})$$

Courtship frequency was also analysed using lmerTest and lme4 package in R. Courtship frequency was modelled as response variable with male regime, age and their two-way interaction as fixed factors and block including all its interactions as random factor. Vial id (replicate vial identity) nested within block was fitted as a random factor. Analysis indicated that block and its interactions were not significant contributors to the overall variance. Detailed analysis with random effects is provided in supplementary information (Table S6). The following linear-mixed model was used to analyse courtship frequency, while post hoc comparisons were done using Tukey’s HSD using emmeans package (Lenth et al. 2018).

$$\mathrm{Courtship}\;\mathrm{frequency}\sim\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime}+\mathrm{age}+\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime}:\mathrm{age}+(1\vert\mathrm{Block}/\mathrm{Vial}\;\mathrm{id})+(1\vert\mathrm{Block}:\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime})+(1\vert\mathrm{Block}:\mathrm{age})+(1\vert\mathrm{Block}:\mathrm{Male}\;\mathrm{regime}:\mathrm{age})$$

Results

Female mortality

Compared to the continuous exposure condition, where sexes were held together for the entire 20 days of the assay, female mortality was substantially lower under single mating assay condition (cumulative mortality < 10%). Moreover, female mortality was substantially less when they were held with faster aging evolved males or the phenocopied control or CCOs. Both the fixed factors, i.e. male regime and male exposure type, were found to have significant effects on cumulative female mortality (Table 1). In addition, the effect of the male regime × male exposure type interaction was also found to be significant (Table 1). Tukey’s HSD indicated that under single mating assay condition, the differences in cumulative female mortality across the three male regimes were not statistically significant. Under continuous exposure condition, however, it was substantially higher, especially when the females were held with control males (> 24% higher compared to either evolved ACO or phenocopied control, CCO males; Fig. 2a). The difference between evolved and phenocopy treatments was not significant. As the analysis indicated a significant effect of male regime × male-exposure type × block three-way interaction (Table 1), the results from each block were analysed separately using two-factor ANOVA (male regime and male-exposure type as fixed factors). The detailed outcome of these analyses can be found in the supplementary material (Table S5). Briefly, block 3 failed to show any effect of the regime; results from all other blocks were qualitatively identical.

Table 1 Summary of results of three-factor ANOVA on cumulative female mortality, cumulative fecundity and two-factor ANOVA on dry body weight of males at eclosion. Mortality analysis was done on arcsine square root transformed values. Regime and male-exposure type are taken as fixed factor and block as random factor in female mortality and cumulative fecundity analysis. Regime is taken as fixed factor and block as random factor in dry body weight analysis
Fig. 2
figure 2

Effect of exposure to treatment males (ACO/CCO/CO) on female mortality and fecundity. (a) Proportion of females died by the end of the 20-day assay period (cumulative female mortality), under the two male-exposure conditions — single exposure and continuous exposure. The vertical bars indicate the mean across all replicate populations. Error bars represent the standard errors of means (SEM); (b) average per capita fecundity of experimental Oregon R females exposed to treatment males across all five age classes, under the two male exposure conditions — single exposure and continuous exposure. Means were calculated over five replicate populations. Only relevant multiple comparisons, which were done using Tukey’s HSD, are shown. Significant differences are marked with horizontal line and an asterisk. The X-axis for both the panels represents male exposure conditions

Dry body weight

Dry body weight results confirmed that evolved ACO males were substantially smaller compared to their ancestral CO males. Furthermore, phenocopied control males qualitatively mimicked a similar size reduction of evolved males compared to that of the controls. We found significant effect of regime (p < 0.001; Table 1). Pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s HSD showed that both evolved and phenocopied controls are significantly smaller than control, COs (weight in mg, mean ± standard error of mean, ACO: 0.162 ± 0.003; CO: 0.271 ± 0.004; CCO: 0.131 ± 0.005). Qualitatively the pattern of dry body weight was similar in all the blocks, but we found significant interaction of regime × block. Hence, summary table of each block is provided in supplementary information (Table S1; Fig. S2).

Female fecundity

Fecundity of females held with evolved males was found to be significantly higher compared to both the ancestral CO males as well as those from the phenocopied control treatment. While the trend in cumulative fecundity across all age points was quite clean, age-specific fecundity results were more complex. Results of the three-factor mixed model ANOVA revealed significant effects of male regime, male exposure type and male regime × male exposure type interaction on cumulative per capita fecundity (Table 1). Multiple comparisons using Tukey’s HSD indicated that under single mating exposure, the three regimes did not differ significantly from each other. Under the continuous exposure assay condition, females held with evolved ACO males were found to have ~ 13.65% higher cumulative per capita fecundity compared to the females held with control CO males. The difference in cumulative fecundity of females held with phenocopied control (CCO) males and that of the females held with control (CO) males was statistically insignificant (Fig. 2b).

Results of the linear mixed model analysis on age-specific per capita fecundity of the females in the continuous exposure set were more complex (see Supplementary information). While in each block significant effects of male regime, age and male regime × age interaction were detected, there was little consistent trend across blocks (Table S4). However, in at least three of the five age points, females held with evolved males showed a significantly higher per capita fecundity compared to the other two treatments, particularly at later age points (Fig. S3).

Courtship frequency and pattern

The evolved (ACO) males were found to be significantly more active in courting females. Linear mixed model analysis on the courtship frequency (i.e. CF) results showed significant effects of male regime, age and regime × age interaction (Table 2). Tukey’s HSD results showed that courtship frequency of evolved males is significantly higher, particularly at early age classes than the control males (Fig. 3a).

Table 2 Summary of the results of linear mixed model (LMM) analysis of courtship frequency using lmerTest function in R. Regime and age were modelled as fixed factors and block as a random factor
Fig. 3
figure 3

Results of the courtship behaviour assays. (a) Courtship frequency (bouts of courtship per observation) was measured for the 20-day assay period on days 2, 6, 11, 16 and 20 in the continuous exposure vials of the main assay. (b) Frequency of the five components (proportional contribution of a courtship component) of courtship ritual was measured in a separate assay, where evolved (ACO) and control (CO) males were held with Oregon R females. The vertical bars indicate the mean across all five replicate populations. Error bars represent the standard errors of means (SEM). Only relevant multiple comparisons are shown in the figure. Significant differences are marked with horizontal line and an asterisk

When we compared the courtship ritual performed by the control and evolved males, trying to assess if the courtship ritual has evolved in response to selection for accelerated life cycle, the latter showed a significantly higher mounting attempt, the final step in the courtship sequence. The results of two-factor mixed model ANOVA performed on the frequency of different courtship components indicated an interesting effect of male regime. Male regime was not found to have a significant effect on four of the five courtship components (Table 3). However, we found a significant male regime effect on attempted copulation (p = 0.029), where evolved males showed ~ 15% higher copulation attempts compared to control males (Fig. 3b; Table 3).

Table 3 Summary of results of two-factor ANOVA on courtship component frequencies. Male regime was modelled as a fixed factor and block as a random factor. Analysis was done on arcsine square root transformed values

Discussion

Our results showed that, in spite of persistent harassment through increased courtship and copulation attempts, evolved (ACO) males are clearly far less harming compared to the control CO males. The females held with evolved males showed significantly less mortality and higher fecundity during the assay period. Interestingly, the evolved males were found to be more active in courtship, especially early in life. This age-specific pattern of change in courtship activity is consistent with the theory of selection for early life fitness components in the ACO males. Despite several previous investigations linking courtship activity to the level of mate harm caused by D. melanogaster males, evolved ACO males were found to be substantially less harming, in spite of having higher courtship activity.

Though faster aging ACO males are expected to have the potential to invest a greater proportion of their resources in competitive traits, potentially making them more harming to their mates, our results clearly show a contrasting trend. One potential explanation of this observation is that the evolved males are substantially smaller and are, hence, less harming — measured in terms of the mates’ survival rate and reproductive output. Such an effect of body size on harming ability of the males in this system has been previously shown (Pitnick 1991, Pitnick and García–González 2002). Our results, however, suggest a more complex picture. Based on the ACO vs. CCO comparison, we could attribute only the mortality effect to the reduced size of the evolved males. However, the fecundity effect of the evolved males was quite unique. Average cumulative fecundity of experimental females held with the evolved males was found to be significantly higher than that with control males. However, no significant difference was found between cumulative fecundity of females held with control and phenocopied control males. Thus, it is reasonable to deduce that the difference in reproductive output of the experimental females held with evolved males and those held with control males was unlikely to be an outcome of the size difference between the males from the two regimes. Thus, either due to reduction in size or due to changes in traits unrelated to size, or both, evolved ACO males are significantly less harming to their mates.

D. melanogaster males are known to physically coerce females through persistent courtship (Fowler and Partridge 1989). Multiple lines of evidence have also shown the correlation between the degree of mate harm and intensity of courtship behaviour (Nandy et al. 2013b; MacPherson et al. 2018). For example, Nandy et al. (2013b) experimentally evolved a set of populations under male biased operational sex ratio that resulted in increased harming ability in males, along with increased courtship frequency. MacPherson et al. (2018) showed that females seemed to avoid mate harm when they were held in complex holding chambers with ample hiding opportunities, possibly by escaping direct exposure to persistent courtship. Different components of courtship behaviour have been found to have ample genetic variation both in natural and laboratory populations of Drosophila (Markow and Hanson 1981; Gromko 1987; Ritchie and Gleason 1995; Colegrave et al. 2000; Snook et al. 2005; Dai et al. 2008). There has been ample evidence suggesting evolvability of courtship behaviour in D. melanogaster (Bedhomme et al. 2008; Nandy et al. 2013b). However, how evolution of courtship behaviour can contribute to the evolution of mate harming ability of the males is not clear. The fact that evolved males, in our study, were found to be less harming despite being more active in courting females is a direct challenge to the conventional wisdom that draws a one-on-one connection between courtship and physical component of mate harm. Not only evolved males courted females more frequently, they also attempted copulation more often — a component of courtship that appears to be more coercive. Hence, our results indicate that the relationship between courtship behaviour and mate harm is more complex than previously anticipated.

In addition to persistent courtship, seminal fluid proteins (Sfp) transferred to females during copulation have been shown to bring down female survival rate (Chapman et al. 1995; Wigby and Chapman 2005; Wigby et al. 2020). Changes in Sfp content of the ejaculate can potentially explain reduction in harming ability of the evolved males. At this point, we do not have data on Sfp to directly test this hypothesis. However, in a separate assay, we observed that copulation between an ACO male and a female from the corresponding population is significantly shorter compared to that between a pair from a CO population (see SI for more details, Fig. S4 and Table S7; mean copulation duration in minutes ± SEM, ACO: 16.15 ± 0.657; CO: 20.32 ± 0.623). Variation in copulation duration has been correlated with variation in the amount of Sfp transferred (Singh and Singh 2004; Friberg 2006; Bretman et al. 2009, 2010). In principle, reduction in post-copulatory sexual selection can bring about changes in Sfp and/or copulatory traits in evolved populations. Monogamy and female biased operational sex ratio have already been shown to lead to reduction in both harming ability and investment in certain attributes of seminal fluid content including Sfp (Holland and Rice 1999; Wigby and Chapman 2004; Crudgington et al. 2005; Linklater et al. 2007). However, it is unrealistic to suggest that our evolved ACO populations have a monogamous breeding system and, hence, post-copulatory sexual selection is absent. Our observations suggest that females in faster aging ACO populations undergo remating quite regularly (Fig. S5). However, the breeding ecology of this regime is vastly different from the ancestral regime. In contrast to the ancestral CO regime, where breeding life is approximately 19 days of adult life, the evolved ACO flies get 24–36 h to reproduce after becoming adults. In this incredibly short breeding life, males are selected to be reproductively active and invest heavily on competitive traits that maximize mating success. Our courtship frequency results support this hypothesis. However, given that there is strong last male sperm precedence in this species (Manier et al. 2010) and ample female remating, the exact nature and intensity of post-copulatory sexual selection is not clear. A related possibility is that the experimental females held with evolved and control males remated at different rates thereby received different quantities of Sfp. Though we do not have data on remating frequency in our assay, separate observation on these populations does not suggest difference in remating rate (see Fig. S5 in supplementary information).

In an independent investigation on a similar set of experimentally evolved populations (Ghosh and Joshi 2012; Mital et al. 2021, 2022), it was found that the intensity of sexual selection and the degree of interlocus sexual conflict to be lower in populations selected for faster development and early reproduction. The parallel between our results and those of Ghosh and Joshi (2012) and Mital et al. (2021, 2022) is somewhat expected and is a robust proof in support of our theory. However, even more interesting is the difference. Whereas Mital et al. (2021) could almost entirely assign the reduction in sexual antagonism in their faster developing and aging populations (viz., FEJ populations) to the reduction in body size, our results point to a different explanation. Comparison of the accelerated life cycle regime in Mital et al. (2021) and that of our ACO populations is interesting. In our faster aging ACO regime, due to a fixed egg-to-adult development time window of 8 days, selection for faster pre-adult development has not been strong for the last several hundred generations. However, the accelerated life cycle regime in Mital et al. (2021) has a strong directional selection for faster development and, as a result, flies under this regime have evolved a more extreme reduction in size, amounting to resource deprivation that did not allow the evolution of male competitive traits, including courtship (Mital et al. 2021). In addition, whereas the accelerated life cycle regime of Mital et al. (2021) practically ensures life-long monogamy, we observed substantial amount of promiscuity in ACO flies. Hence, it is possible that ACO males were specifically selected for traits that maximize mating and fertilization success in such a way that reduces incidental mate harm. Such results clearly show the importance of an incredibly nuanced breeding ecology of animals and its connection to life history.

Theories have long predicted a connection between life history and sexual conflict. Most often, the trade-offs between costly sexually antagonistic traits and one or more life history trait(s), most notably somatic maintenance, have taken the centre stage in such discussions (Bonduriansky et al. 2008; Maklakov and Lummaa 2013; Hooper et al. 2017). However, if the aim is to understand and predict the evolutionary outcome of changes in life history on sexually antagonistic traits, perhaps a more important question is how a given alteration in life history affects the breeding system and, by extension, the ecology of sexual selection. The secondary outcomes in that case can affect a wider range of sexually antagonistic traits, potentially independent of trade-offs. Our results on the reduced mate harming ability in ACO males are evidence in support of this thesis.

Rankin and Kokko (2006, 2007) very explicitly showed a seemingly obvious outcome of interlocus sexual conflict, i.e. population extinction due to unchecked evolution of sexually antagonistic male traits. However, most theories of interlocus conflict view such evolutionary dynamics as an arms’ race between sexes. Hence, counteradaptation in females is fundamental to the theory. Such counteradaptation is also expected to impede the evolutionary increase in the harming ability of males in a population, thereby avoiding the tragedy of commons. In line with such a reasoning, many empirical investigations have reported strong female counteradaptation to mate harm imposed by male (Wigby and Chapman 2004; Nandy et al. 2014). As we have argued above, life history can also be a potent constraint on the evolution of mate harm. However, there is little literature on this subject. To the best of our knowledge, the present study on the ACO males is one of the very few empirical evidence of evolutionary reduction in mate harming ability. The results presented here should be of general importance in explaining the variation in the intensity of sexual antagonism across species.