Early exposure to environmental lead is a major ongoing child public health problem in the United States, as suggested for example by events in Flint, Michigan (Hanna-Attisha et al. 2016). Currently available NHANES data has suggested that 2.5% of U.S. children have blood lead levels (BLLs) ≥ 5 µg/dL, justifying the designation of this value for determining “elevated.” (The value will be updated as new data becomes available.) Two years after changes to the Flint water supply, 6% of children were identified as having elevated BLLs, more than double the rate suggested by the NHANES data. Subsequent investigative reporting by Reuters identified hundreds of cities nationwide with estimated child lead exposure rates far worse than those found in Flint (Pell and Schneyer 2016). One such city was El Paso, Texas. Using aggregated census tract and zip code level data, it was estimated that 10–15% of children living in downtown El Paso had “elevated” blood lead (Pell and Schneyer 2016). Complicating the problem, statistical modeling has suggested that blood lead is either never tested or never reported for hundreds of thousands of children nationwide (Roberts et al. 2017).

It is widely accepted that no level of lead exposure is “safe” for children. Once a child is exposed to lead, there are no interventions available that reverse its damaging physiological effects. Environmental lead source mitigation is the only known method for prevention and intervention; however, national child lead exposure estimates suggest that many high-risk neighborhoods remain un-remediated. Current mitigation policies can vary from state to state, nonetheless focus almost exclusively on homes with children younger than age 5 years.

The most common source of child lead exposure continues to be un-remediated or poorly remediated residential lead paint. Old plumbing and historically contaminated soil contribute to a somewhat lesser extent, and all of these tend to occur more frequently in lower-income neighborhoods. Perhaps for this reason, risk of child lead exposure has been associated with socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, such as family income and race/ethnicity (Adler 2005; Kennedy et al. 2016; Morales et al. 2005; Morrison et al. 2013; Sobin et al. 2015), as well as child age (Bose-O’Reilly et al. 2010; Flora et al. 2012; Goyer 1997; Kostial et al. 1978; Lanphear et al. 2002; Morrison et al. 2013) and sex (Chłopicka et al. 1998; Hanna-Attisha et al. 2016; Morrison et al. 2013; Sobin et al. 2015).

Risk of lead exposure is greatest among children younger than age 5 due largely to hand-to-mouth behavior. This assumption has influenced state and local policy guidelines for residential lead mitigation. Many studies have suggested that older children between the ages of 5 and 12 years also are vulnerable to lead exposure and its effects on neurocognitive function. For example, results from studies conducted by our laboratory showed that in more than 600 children ages 5–12 years, living in downtown El Paso neighborhoods, 14% had levels ≥ 5 µg/dL; 60% of children had BLLs between 2.5 and 7 µg/dL (Sobin et al. 2009, 2011, 2015). These exposure levels were associated with deficits in motor dexterity and working memory. Another issue concerns the extent to which historically contaminated urban settings continue to pose risk of exposure for children of all ages. Studies are needed to characterize lead exposure in older children living in neighborhoods, particularly those previously identified as “high-risk” for child lead exposure.

This study compared the BLLs of children aged 5–12 years living in an urban neighborhood to the BLLs of demographically matched children living in two nearby rural communities (Fig. 1). The urban neighborhood was one of seven in the downtown region designated by federal and state agencies approximately 30 years earlier as “high-risk” for child lead exposure, largely because of its proximity to a smelter site (Fig. 1). The urban and rural children were matched for age, sex, race, ethnicity, and family income level. We hypothesized that geographic location would predict increased lead exposure in older children and that the BLLs of children living in the previously designated “high-risk” urban neighborhood would have significantly higher BLLs compared with o rural children.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The map shows the locations of urban and rural neighborhoods from which children in the study were sampled. The urban neighborhood is located in the downtown city center

Methods

The studies followed all current standards for human subjects’ research and were approved by the University of Texas Institutional Review Board (IRB Protocol #564493-1 and #79085-14), by the El Paso Independent School District Research Board, and by the Canutillo Independent School District Board and Superintendent.

These studies used extant data collected by our laboratory for research on child heavy metal exposure in the southwest United States. Identical methods were used with all study participants, yielding the same demographic, health, and biological data for all children and families. Child participants were recruited from elementary schools in two geographically distinct locations, including one urban area and two adjacent rural townships located within approximately 20 miles of the urban setting. Inclusion criteria were current enrollment at one of the participating elementary schools and being between 5 and 12 years of age. The exclusion criterion was previous diagnosis of lead poisoning (no children had been previously tested positive for lead exposure and none were excluded from participation). Fewer children were available from the rural setting, and to maximize the available samples, data from all of the participating rural children were included (n = 111). Urban child matches were randomly selected according to age (within 6 months), sex, race, ethnicity, and family income level. Children from the rural settings were from two neighboring townships (Rural 1, n = 39 and Rural 2, n = 72); children from the urban setting were from one elementary school. Figure 1 shows the geographic boundaries of children’s urban and rural neighborhoods.

All forms and materials were available in Spanish and English versions. Researchers participating in this study were bilingual and interacted with parents and children in their preferred language throughout the course of the study. Parents were recruited during parent-teacher meetings at the elementary schools; informed consent was obtained at the time of recruitment following full explanation and discussion of the study. Child assent was obtained immediately before the start of child testing. All testing was conducted during physical education periods and included anthropometric measurements and finger stick blood sample collection. Children first washed their hands. Fingers were then wiped with metal-removing towelettes (D-Wipe™, Esca-Tech, Inc. Milwaukee, WI). Saf-T-Pro™ 1.8-mm lancets were used to prick the fourth finger of the left hand, and 50 µl of blood was collected into EDTA microvials and refrigerated until analysis at 4 °C. All blood samples were analyzed via inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (ICP-MS) and were tested for lead, cadmium, and mercury. Specific methods for the ICP-MS analyses conducted have been reported in detail elsewhere (Sobin et al. 2009, 2011, 2015).

Statistical Analyses

All variables were examined for outliers and distribution properties. Descriptive statistics were calculated for demographic and clinical characteristics. To determine whether data from the two rural settings could be combined, the demographic, anthropometric, and BLL values (µg/dL) of children from the two rural schools were compared using ANOVA. The characteristics of rural children did not differ by site (Table 1), and for the urban/rural comparisons, data from the two rural sites were combined. ANOVA and general linear model regression were used to examine whether geographic location predicted child BLL, controlling for gender and age. For comparison purposes, the same regression models were calculated for cadmium and mercury.

Table 1 Clinical and demographic characteristics of children in urban and rural settings and parent’s demographic characteristics (n = 222)

Results

Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics of the analyzed sample of 222 children by site. Mean ages for rural Site 1 and Site 2 and Urban were 7.97 ± 1.77, 7.86 ± 1.88, and 8.01 ± 1.77, respectively. Children living in the urban setting had a slightly higher mean body weight compared with children living in the rural sites; mean height was very similar across all sites. Higher mean BMIs were found in children living in the urban setting (18.64 ± 5.02) as compared to rural sites (17.99 ± 3.97 in Rural 1 and 17.54 ± 3.98 in Rural 2). These differences were not statistically significant.

As shown in Table 1, the majority of the children’s mothers and fathers in all three sites identified themselves as Hispanic or of Mexican descent, and white. Across all sites, the highest education level for a majority of parents was high school. For all sites, the majority of households had an income of ≤ 20 K per year. The mean number of persons living in households also was similar across all sites (5.13 ± 1.26 Rural 1, 5.34 ± 1.56 Rural 2, and Urban, 5.00 ± 1.65) (the U.S. Federal Poverty Level for 2017 for a family of 4 was $24,600). As shown in Table 1, the mean BLL for urban children was higher (2.86 ± 1.29 μg/dL) compared with rural children (1.18 ± 1.32 μg/dL, Rural 1 and 1.07 ± 0.84 μg/dL Rural 2). The same trends were observed for cadmium and mercury.

To determine whether findings from the two rural settings could be combined for models comparing urban versus rural geographic location, one-way ANOVAs were conducted comparing the two rural sites with regard to child blood lead, cadmium, and mercury levels, controlling for age and gender. As shown in Table 2, the differences in heavy metal levels for the two rural sites were not statistically significant (lead, F1,109 = 0.26, p = 0.61; cadmium, F1,109 = 0.84, p = 0.36; mercury, F1,109 = 1.17, p = 0.28). Data from the two rural sites were combined for the remaining analyses.

Table 2 One-way analysis of variance comparing blood lead, cadmium and mercury levels in children from two rural settings

Table 3 summarizes the Type III fixed effects and parameter estimates for associations between child BLL and geographic location, controlling for gender and age. Location predicted BLL (F1,221 = 126.02, p < 0.001). The effects of age (F1,221 = 1.80, p = 0.18) and gender (F1,221 = 0.740, p = 0.39) were not significant predictors of BLL; the interaction of location and gender also was not significant (F1,221 = 0.120, p = 0.73). The nonsignificant effects were dropped from the model and the reduced model was recalculated predicting child BLL from geographic location. In the reduced model, location was a significant predictor of child BLL (F1,221 = 125.12, p < 0.001).

Table 3 Type III fixed effects and parameter estimates for associations between child blood lead levels, controlling for gender and age, living in rural and urban settings (n = 222)

Tables 4 and 5 summarize the Type III fixed effects and parameter estimates for associations between child blood cadmium levels and blood mercury levels respectively, controlling for gender, age, and location. The effect of location was not a significant predictor of child blood mercury levels (F1,140 = 2.61, p = 0.11). Interestingly, mercury level was predicted by age alone (F1,140 = 4.67, p = 0.03); however, the amount of variance explained was negligible.

Table 4 Type III fixed effects and parameter estimates for associations between child blood cadmium levels, controlling for gender and age, living in rural and urban settings (n = 222)
Table 5 Type III fixed effects and parameter estimates for associations between child blood mercury levels, controlling for gender and age, living in rural and urban settings (n = 222)

Multiple linear regression analyses also were conducted to predict blood lead, cadmium, or mercury levels based on location, age, and gender (Table 6). Consistent with the ANOVA results, the regression equation predicting BLL from location was significant (F3,218 = 125.13, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.363). Only location was a significant predictor of BLL in children, p < 0.001. Children living in the urban setting had significantly higher BLLs than children living in the rural area. Also consistent with the ANOVA models, the regression analyses predicting cadmium and mercury from location, controlling for age and gender, were not statistically significant (cadmium, F3,217 = 1.23, p = 0.27, R2 = 0.015; mercury, F3,137 = 3.36, p = 0.069, R2 = 0.024).

Table 6 Blood lead, cadmium, and mercury levels regressed hierarchically on gender, age, and living in rural and urban settings

Discussion

There is heightened national awareness that early chronic lead exposure continues to be a major unresolved pediatric health threat. Remediation and risk abatement policies focus on children below the age of 5 years. It also is critical to understand the risk to children older than 5 years of age. This study compared 5–12-year-old children living in an urban neighborhood designated 30 years earlier as “high-risk” for lead exposure, to demographically matched rural children living 20 miles to the north. The older urban children had significantly higher BLLs. Cadmium and mercury levels were within current limits in all children, and geographic location did not predict cadmium or mercury levels. It is important to note that the 111 urban children were randomly selected for matching from a database of more than 600 tested children and thus did not represent urban children with the highest lead exposures. The BLLs of a majority of children in this study did not exceed 5 µg/dL.

Similar to hundreds of cities nationwide, the urban locale studied had a well-documented history of lead contamination followed by environmental remediation. A smelter built in 1887 within 1 mile of what eventually became the downtown center, and active until 1999, was one major contamination source. Data collected for a lawsuit in this region initiated in 1970 and claiming violations of the Texas Clean Air Act determined that in one 3-year period, between 1969 and 1971, the smelter had emitted more than 1000 metric tons of lead, 560 tons of zinc, 12 tons of cadmium, and 1 ton of arsenic (Landrigan and Baker 1981). The company declared bankruptcy in August 2005, and when the EPA denied permission to restart the facility in March 2009, the property was placed in an environmental custodial trust. In a December 2009 settlement, the company agreed to pay $1.79 billion to settle pollution claims at 80 sites in 20 states. Cleanup of the El Paso site began in 2010, the stack was demolished in 2011, and site remediation was completed in late 2016. The BLLs of children in this study suggested that risk of lead exposure continues among children 5–12 years of age.

Implications of Lead Exposure in Older Children

While historical contamination from industrial emissions are relevant in the urban area studied, nationwide lead-based paint remains the most common source of exposure for children (Centers for Disease and Control Prevention 2013). Using data from the Texas Childhood Blood Lead Surveillance Program for El Paso County, the Texas Department of State Health Services showed that the median age of houses (by census tract) predicted child BLLs (Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry 2018). In the urban neighborhood studied, > 80% of residences were built before bans on lead paint were enacted (1978) (Census: El Paso, TX 2014).

Risk of lead exposure to younger children often is attributed to ingestion of paint chips, contaminated household dirt and/or dust, lead-contaminated water, and/or contaminated soil through frequent hand-to-mouth behavior. Remediation efforts attempt to identify these ingestible sources. Lead exposure in children ages 5–12 may instead suggest that exposure sources are from inhaled contaminated dust, ingested contaminated food or water, or exposure from jewelry. Pernicious sources can include leached lead when acid-based foods, such as tomatoes, are prepared with and/or stored in leaded cookware, lead-glazed pottery, and leaded utensils (Landrigan et al. 1975; Morse 1979; Romero 1997). Children’s jewelry can have high lead content absorbed through abraded skin or piercings. Particularly for exposure among older children, region-specific factors, such as seasonal wind patterns, bioavailability, and lead source isotype, could help to identify lead sources responsible for child blood levels.

It is important to note that there is growing awareness of the discrepancies between the demonstrated ill effects in children of lower-range BLLs and contamination mandates. For example, on December 29, 2017, the U.S. Court of Appeals for the Ninth Circuit delivered a rarely issued writ of mandamus to the EPA. In this action, the EPA was ordered immediately to address the grievances cited in a consumer-generated 2009 petition and, within 90 days of the writ, finally establish lower “acceptable” limits of interior lead-paint residue.

Limitations

This study focused only on child BLLs and did not attempt to identify possible sources of exposure in either the urban or rural locales. While previous studies showed that the estimated child BLLs of children in the urban locale were associated with neurocognitive deficits, the child BLLs represented exposure during only one 6-month period. Longitudinal studies are needed to further characterize the effects of lead exposure over time in older children.

Conclusions

Older urban children living in historically contaminated neighborhoods have significant ongoing risk of lead exposure. In hundreds of cities nationwide, exposure to environmental lead is a violation of environmental justice and requires the development of practical systematic approaches for its characterization and remediation (CDC 2013) .