Introduction

The proportion of Asian, Black, and Latino people in the US population has increased rapidly in recent decades, fueled in part by immigration. Despite the expectation that the mental health of racial/ethnic minority and immigrant groups would be negatively affected by social disadvantages disproportionately experienced by these groups [1, 2], prior studies in the US have found that the prevalence of most mental disorders is lower for Asian, Black, and Latino adults when compared to White adults [3,4,5].Footnote 1 At the same time, a recent rise in mortality among middle-aged White people has been attributed to increase in suicide rates and deaths related to substance use disorders, prompting renewed attention to the mental health of Whites relative to other groups [6]. Research that considers the effects of race/ethnicity versus nativity (i.e., immigrant generation status) is needed to understand these differences in mental health risk across groups.

Studies have found that risk for first onset of a mental disorder is lowest when immigrants are in their home countries and increases after US migration [7, 8]. Once in the US, research has demonstrated a pattern of varying risk of psychiatric disorder by individual nativity. Specifically, foreign-born members of racial/ethnic minority groups often demonstrate better mental health than their US-born counterparts, a pattern referred to as the ‘immigrant paradox’ [9, 10], yet there is less consensus regarding the effects of parental nativity [7, 9, 11, 12]. Asian and Latino foreign-born adults have lower rates of mental disorders compared to their US-born counterparts [8, 9]. Risk for substance use disorders has also been found to increase across immigrant generations for Asian, Black and Latino people, with those with US-born parents having the highest risk [8, 13, 14]. Some research indicates that the immigrant paradox may extend to Whites, for example, research from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) found that foreign-born adults overall had lower lifetime risk of mental health disorders compared to US-born adults, regardless of race/ethnicity [11]. Yet, most prior research on mental health disparities focuses on either race/ethnicity or nativity (i.e., US versus foreign-born) but rarely the intersection of the two or with analyses that detail the effects of both individual and parental nativity [7, 8, 14, 15].

Analysis of the intersection between race/ethnicity and nativity should also consider the underlying processes that may explain the relationship between these demographic variables and risk of mental disorders. Increased risk for mental disorders within those of US nativity may result from the effects of acculturation, meaning adopting US cultural elements and norms [16, 17]. Language proficiency in English is often used as a proxy for acculturation when more nuanced measures are unavailable [18, 19]. Conversely, high levels of ethnic identity reflect greater enculturation, or identification with and pride in the individual’s heritage culture [18]. Ethnic identity may serve as a protective factor against discrimination, and consequently development of mental disorders [20].

The current report analyzes data from four US national household surveys with a combined sample of over 20,000 Asian, non-Latino Black, Latino and non-Latino White adults, including foreign-born respondents and non-English-speaking Asians and Latinos. Racial/ethnic categories only reflect broad sociopolitical categories in the US rather than biological entities, but are linked to the allocation of resources and experiences of discrimination. Our analyses extend prior research by examining how differences in the mental health of various racial/ethnic groups vary by nativity status and how the association of lifetime disorder risk with nativity status varies within racial/ethnic groups. We also investigate whether these differences in lifetime mental disorders can be explained after adjusting for racial/ethnic identity, language proficiency (a proxy for acculturation), and individual and parental educational attainment (a proxy for socioeconomic status), to disentangle the effects of self-identified race/ethnicity from these related cultural and sociodemographic factors. The study provides the most detailed examination to date of differences in lifetime risk of anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders in the US by race/ethnicity and nativity.

Methods

Sample

The four surveys analyzed are the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication (NCS-R) [21], the National Latino and Asian American Study (NLAAS) [22], the National Survey of American Life (NSAL) [23] and the National Comorbidity Survey Re-Interview (NCS-2) [24]. The NCS-R, NLAAS, and NSAL were conducted in coordination to assess mental health among US household residents from varying racial/ethnic groups. NCS-2 was the second wave study of a national household survey of English-speaking participants. Previous reports combined data from the first three surveys into a pooled sample known as the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Studies (CPES) Dataset [25]; we added the NCS-2 and reweighted the data to make the total sample representative of the US household population with respect to the cross-classification of age, sex, urbanicity, Census division and race/ethnicity. All surveys were conducted between 2001 and 2003 and included adults aged 18 years and older, except the NCS-2, which included only 25–64-year-old respondents. African American and Caribbean Black people were oversampled in the NSAL, while Asians/Asian Americans and Latinos were oversampled in the NLAAS [25]. All surveys excluded institutionalized persons and those living on military bases; non-English speakers were excluded from the NCS-R, NCS-2 and NSAL [25]. The NLAAS survey was conducted in English, Spanish, Mandarin, Cantonese, Tagalog and Vietnamese. Interviews in all surveys were conducted by the professional field staff of the Survey Research Center in the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, with language fluency for NLAAS interviewers evaluated by certified examiners. A more detailed discussion of the sample is presented elsewhere [25, 26] and summarized, along with sample sizes and response rates, in Online Resource 1. Recruitment, consent and field procedures were approved by the Human Subjects Committees of the University of Michigan, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge Health Alliance and the University of Washington.Footnote 2 Informed consent was obtained before conducting surveys for all participants.

Measures

Information on survey measures, including cross-cultural validity of measures, has been detailed in prior publications [23, 26, 27] and in Online Resource 1. The current study is based on a dataset constructed to ensure comparability of measures across studies; only items and measures available across all studies were utilized in the analysis.

Diagnostic assessment

DSM-IV diagnoses were based on Version 3.0 of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) [28], a fully structured lay interview that generates diagnoses according to the criteria of both the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision [29] and the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition Revised DSM-IV [30]. Disorders assessed in all surveys included anxiety disorders (panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, agoraphobia without panic disorder, social phobia, post-traumatic stress disorder), mood disorders (major depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder) and substance use disorders (alcohol and drug abuse and dependence). As described elsewhere [31], generally good concordance was found between DSM-IV diagnoses based on the CIDI and those based on blinded clinical reappraisal interviews with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) [32].

Race/ethnicity

Race and ethnicity were assessed separately by participant self-report to questions based on US Office of Management and Budget Standards. Responses were then used to distinguish groups across broad combined race/ethnicity categories. Responses to questions about race/ethnicity—which allowed multiple selections—were used to categorize respondents as Asian, Latino, non-Latino Black and non-Latino White using a hierarchical system. All respondents who reported being Asian were coded Asian regardless of any additional response provided. The same approach was used for the remaining respondents who reported being Latino, then those who reported being Black. Remaining respondents were coded as being White if they reported no other race or ethnicity in addition to being White. This approach led to slightly different totals for each race than previously reported for the individual surveys.

Nativity status

Nativity was coded into five categories. The first three included all respondents born in the US: those with two US-born parents (Nativity 1); one US-born parent (Nativity 2); and neither parent born in the US (Nativity 3). The last two categories included all foreign-born respondents; Nativity 4 included person-years from after the respondent moved to the US, and Nativity 5 included person-years from before the respondent moved to the US.Footnote 3

Covariates

Respondents were asked about racial/ethnic identity, with a question about how close they felt in their ideas and feelings to other people of the same racial/ethnic descent (very close or somewhat close versus not very close or not close at all). Language preference was assessed for NLAAS respondents. Parent and respondent education were classified into four categories: less than high school education, high school graduate/GED, some post-secondary education and a college degree or more. All models controlled for respondent age (18–29, 30–44, 45–59 and 60 +), sex, census division (residence in the following regions of the country, based on Federal Information Processing Standards codes: New England and Middle Atlantic; East North Central, West North Central, and East South Central; West South Central, Mountain, and Pacific) [33] and urbanicity (metropolitan/urban counties, other urban and non-urban). Census division and urbanicity were included as controls due to different distributions of racial/ethnic groups in regions of the country and in urban versus non-urban areas.

Analysis methods

All descriptive statistics are based on weighted data. Multiple imputation [34] was used to impute missing values (see proportions of imputed values in Online Resource 2). We used cross-tabulations to calculate lifetime prevalence estimates of DSM-IV/CIDI anxiety, mood and substance use disorders. The actuarial method [35] was then used to study age-of-onset (AOO; meaning age at which the individual first experienced the disorder) distributions of each class of disorders separately across the four race/ethnicity samples. Finally, discrete-time survival analysis with person-year as the unit of analysis and a logistic link function [36] was used to estimate racial/ethnic differences in lifetime risk of each of these three outcomes. The survival models were based on retrospectively reported AOO to define year of onset of each outcome disorder. Controls were included in the survival models for person-year, sex, age, census division and urbanicity (Model 1).

Subsequent models added racial/ethnic identity, survey language preference, and interactions between covariates found to be significant in prior analyses (Model 2), then all of these variables plus nativity (Model 3). Models examining the association of nativity with mood and substance use disorders separately in each race/ethnicity subsample included all previous controls (Models 4a, b, and c). Models stratified by race/nativity categories included all previous controls (Model 5) and parent and respondent educational attainment (Model 6). Parent education was coded as a time-invariant predictor. We allowed for the possibility of decay in the association of parent education with odds of disorder onset over time by allowing parent education to have different odds ratios (ORs) for person-years 0–17 and 18 +. Respondent education was coded as a time-varying predictor, with high school graduation coded as occurring at age 18 and college education at age 22 for all respondents.

Survival coefficients and their standard errors were exponentiated and are reported as ORs and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). To adjust for clustering and weighting of the data, standard errors were calculated using the Taylor series method in the SURVEY procedures of SAS software, Version 9.4 of the SAS System for Windows. (Copyright © 2013 SAS Institute Inc. SAS and all other SAS Institute Inc. product or services names are registered trademarks or trademarks of SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA.) Multivariate significance tests were conducted using F test based on coefficient variance–covariance matrices adjusted for design effects using the Taylor series method. Denominator degrees of freedom for F test in a multiply-imputed discrete time logistic regression analysis framework were calculated using a method derived by Raghunathan and Dong [37, 38]. Statistical significance was evaluated using two-sided design-based tests and the P < 0.05 level of significance. Only when multivariate tests were significant did we interpret the significance of individual coefficients. This decision rule was used to guard against the possibility of false positive coefficients given the large number of individual coefficients. Although the use of omnibus tests reduces the chance of false positive findings, the only definitive protection against this problem is replication in independent datasets.

Results

Racial/ethnic differences in sociodemographic and geographic characteristics

The consolidated sample included 21,024 respondents (10.6% Asian, 17.3% Latino, 29.6% Black, and 42.4% White; Table 1). Asian, Black, and Latino respondents were generally younger and more likely to live in metropolitan areas than White respondents. Asian and Latino adults more often lived in the West than Black and White respondents.

Table 1 Distributions of all predictors used in the analysis by race/ethnicity

Almost all Black and White respondents reported that they were born in the US (93.9% and 96.6%, respectively). Nearly half of Latinos and more than three-fourths of Asians, in comparison, were born in another country. Consistent with these differences, 30.7% of Latinos and 24.2% of Asians chose to be interviewed in a language other than English. Parent and respondent’s educational levels differed substantially by racial/ethnic group.

Racial/ethnic differences in lifetime prevalence of mental disorders

Lifetime prevalence of any DSM-IV/CIDI disorder was highest among White respondents (45.6%), somewhat lower among Latino (38.8%) and Black respondents (37.0%) and substantially lower among Asians (23.5%; see Table 2). Across each disorder class, prevalence was highest among White respondents (18.8–26.1%) and lowest among Asian respondents (5.1–13.1%; see Table 2 for specific patterns).

Table 2 Lifetime prevalence of DSM-IV/CIDI disorders in the consolidated sample (n = 21,024)

AOO curves were used to project lifetime risk to 74 years of age (the upper age in the sample before data became too sparse for projection). The median AOO of any disorder was in the late teens for all groups (detailed results available on request). The onset distribution of anxiety disorders was latest for Asians (median in mid-20s versus late teens for the other groups), of substance use disorders latest for Black respondents (median in mid-20s versus late teens for other groups) and of mood disorders latest overall but especially for Latinos (mid-30s versus mid-20s for other groups; see Online Resource 2).

Racial/ethnic differences in disorder onset

In a model controlling respondents’ age, sex, urbanicity and Census division (Table 3, Model 1), odds of any lifetime disorder among racial/ethnic minorities was significantly lower than for Whites, with ORs ranging from 0.41–0.76 for anxiety disorders, 0.44–0.68 for mood disorders, and 0.19–0.68 for substance use disorders. In each case, Asians had the lowest OR. In a second model introducing preferred language of interview and racial/ethnic identity (Table 3, Model 2), ORs attenuated but nearly all remained significant—however, Latinos no longer had lower rates of substance use disorders than White respondents. In a third model adding nativity (Table 3, Model 3), only the ORs comparing Black and Asian respondents to White respondents were significant across the three types of disorder.

Table 3 Associations of race/ethnicity with lifetime DSM-IV/CIDI anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders in the consolidated sample

Nativity differences within racial/ethnic groups

Interactions between race/ethnicity and nativity were observed for mood disorders (F12,32956 = 2.11, p = 0.013) and substance use disorders (F12,10760 = 2.34, p = 0.005) but not anxiety disorder (F12,37726 = 0.99, p = 0.45; detailed results available upon request). To explore this variation more concretely, we estimated models for the associations of nativity with mood and substance use disorders separately in each race/ethnicity subsample (Table 3, Models 4a, 4b, and 4c), where nativity was divided into five categories as described above: the respondent and both parents were born in the US (Nativity 1); the respondent and one parent were born in the US (Nativity 2); the respondent but neither parent was born in the US (Nativity 3); and neither the respondent nor either parent was born in the US (Nativity 4 and 5). Given that we were working with person-year models, the latter group was divided into person-years after the respondent moved to the US (Nativity 4) and person-years before (Nativity 5).

It is noteworthy that a consistent pattern was found in Table 4 in which odds of disorder onset across all nativity groups for all outcomes were lowest for foreign-born respondents in the years before moving to the US (Nativity 5). However, given that no information was available for people from the countries of origin who did not move to the US, this result was difficult to interpret. As a result, we excluded person-years prior to first moving to the US from further analysis and focused on the four other nativity categories. Relative odds of disorder onset were found to vary significantly across nativity groups in only three comparisons seen in Table 4: (i) mood disorders among White respondents (F3,8855.2 = 4.3, P = 0.005) due to higher odds of disorder onset among respondents with at least one parent born in the US than with neither parent born in the US; (ii) substance use disorders among Asians (F3,5448.6 = 3.5, P = 0.016) due to higher odds of disorder onset among respondents born in the US than those not born in the US; and (iii) substance use disorders among Latinos (F3,933.86 = 8.0, P < 0.001) due to higher odds of disorder onset among respondents with at least one parent born in the US than those with neither parent born in the US.

Table 4 Associations of nativity with lifetime DSM-IV/CIDI anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders within each race/ethnicity group

We then distinguished within racial/ethnic groups by nativity in ways suggested to be important based on the 3-df significance tests in Table 4. Specifically, individually significant odds ratios dictated the grouping of racial/ethnic and nativity categories for subsequent models in Table 5. We estimated new models of racial/ethnic difference in odds of lifetime prevalence of DSM-IV/CIDI anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders with these revised race/ethnicity-by-nativity categories (Table 5, Model 5). These analyses excluded person-years prior to first moving to the US. For anxiety disorders, the results were very similar to those in Table 2, with White respondents highest and Asians lowest (OR = 0.63) and the relative odds of Asian and Black respondents (OR = 0.79), but not of Latinos (OR = 0.85), significantly different from White respondents. For mood disorders, all racial/ethnic minority groups in addition to White respondents with no parents born in the US (whether or not the respondent was born in the US) had significantly lower odds than White respondents who had at least one parent born in the US. Latinos again were closest to the contrast category of White respondents (OR = 0.75) and all other groups had comparable ORs (OR = 0.56–0.59). The situation was different yet for substance use disorders, where odds of disorder onset were lowest among Asians not born in the US (OR = 0.19); higher, but still significantly less than White respondents, among other Asians (OR = 0.55), Latinos with 0 parents born in the US (0.57), and Black respondents (OR = 0.74); and significantly higher among Latinos with one or more parents born in the US relative to White respondents (OR = 1.21).

Table 5 Associations of race/ethnicity-by-nativity with lifetime DSM-IV/CIDI mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders with and without adjustments for parent and respondent educational level in the consolidated sample

Association with parent and respondent educational level

Finally, we evaluated the extent to which these racial/ethnic differences could be explained by parental and respondent differences in level of education (a proxy for SES). Parental education was coded as time invariant but was allowed to have different ORs person-years 0–17 and 18 +. Results were virtually unchanged when these controls were introduced into the models. (Table 5, Model 6) This was true despite the substantial racial/ethnic differences in both parental education and respondent education documented in Table 1, due to the fact that these education variables were not strongly predictive of the outcomes. To ensure this negative finding was not because the predictive effects of race/ethnicity, nativity and/or education varied across the life course, we evaluated the possibility of interactions between model predictors and person-year and did not find that these explained our results (detailed results are available upon request.).

Discussion

Findings from this study have important implications for our understanding of racial/ethnic variation in mental health disorder risk. Asian, Black, and Latino adults had lower lifetime risk for all major classes of mental disorder compared to White adults—a pattern consistent with prior studies [3, 39]. We anticipated that the lower risk for mental disorders among Asian, Black, and Latino groups in the sample could be partially explained by the protective effects of ethnic identity [40] and foreign-born nativity [7, 9, 11, 12, 41], as well as lack of English language proficiency among Latinos and Asians (a proxy for lower US acculturation) [18, 19]. In our models, all three variables were negatively associated with mental disorder onset and the association of race/ethnicity and disorder onset was somewhat attenuated after adjusting for them. However, the racial/ethnic differences described above remained largely unchanged for Black adults, indicating that the association of racial/ethnic group with disorder onset was not fully explained by ethnic identity, nativity, or English language proficiency. For Asian adults, odds of mental disorder onset increased substantially as these variables were added, indicating they do partially explain the overall lower risk of lifetime disorders among this group. Odds of mental disorder remained significantly lower for Asians as compared to White adults, suggesting factors beyond nativity status should be considered to explain lower overall rates of mental disorders among Asians.

On the other hand, differences in lifetime risk of disorders for Latinos relative to White adults became non-significant after adjusting for ethnic identity, language, and nativity status. This result suggests that overall differences in mental health risk between Latino and White adults are driven, in part, by lower rates of mental disorders among non-English-proficient and foreign-born Latinos [15]. Prior studies have found that US acculturation is a risk factor for mental disorders among Latinos [16, 17, 42], and we similarly found that when controlling for these acculturation and enculturation proxies, risk for mental health disorders among Latino adults approximates risk among White adults. This finding might reflect subtle changes due to acculturation: erosion of collectivistic cultural values [43], such as family obligation and affiliation [44], loss of extended family support [9] and increase in intergenerational conflicts [45]—which can be associated with mental health risk.

Despite making the groups in this study as similar as possible in terms of ethnic identity and language, we still found that the odds of disorder onset varied significantly by parental nativity for mood disorders among White adults, and substance use disorders among Asian and Latino adults. Results suggest that there is an elevated risk for mood disorders among White adults with one or two US-born parents (relative to US-born Whites with two foreign-born parents, foreign-born Whites, and all other racial/ethnic groups in the study). This finding is noteworthy as few studies have explored the effect of nativity status, including both respondent and parental nativity, on prevalence of mental disorders among White Americans [12]; however, some literature suggests a “healthy immigrant” effect for physical health among White people which declines with length of residence in the US [46] or Canada [47]. Parental foreign nativity may be a proxy for exposure to values, norms and social interactions from other cultural contexts that are protective factors against mood disorder onset [7, 48]. For example, an emphasis on family interdependence in Asian and Latin American countries has been identified as a protective factor for these groups once in the US [7, 48, 49], and White immigrant parents may similarly pass down protective cultural processes and values from their own countries.

Yet the higher risk for mood disorders among White adults with one or two US-born parents is striking. First, it is expected that increased stressors due to social disadvantage faced by immigrant and racial/ethnic minority individuals would place them at a greater risk for mood disorders than White individuals [1, 2]; indeed, numerous studies have detailed the negative mental health impact of experiences associated with minority status, such as racial discrimination [50, 51]. Second, as members of the dominant racial/ethnic group, US -born White people with US-born parents benefit from social policies historically favoring White Americans, such as access to higher quality neighborhoods and housing, which should support better mental health [52]. Like recent findings on the increased health risks faced by middle-aged White people [6], these results raise the question of what in the US context might explain greater risk for mood disorders among a relatively more advantaged group—or conversely, what protective effects might explain lower risk for the other groups? A recent review summarizes several lines of research suggesting that in the US context, the mental health of White people is more strongly affected by thwarted expectations for success as members of the dominant group, while traditionally disadvantaged groups have developed more strategies for thriving in the face of adversity [52].

For Asians and Latinos, results indicate that risk for substance use disorders increases across generations, consistent with prior studies [8, 13]. This remains true in our study even after applying multiple statistical controls expected to reduce the effect of nativity status, suggesting that increased US exposure and longer familial history in the US are a strong risk factor for substance use disorders. Stronger social sanctions against substance abuse in immigrant countries of origin may explain these differences; countries like Mexico and China have particularly low rates of substance use disorders compared to the US [53, 54]. When compared across groups, however, Asian adults of any generation status still have substantially lower risk compared to White adults, as do Latino adults with two foreign-born parents. Latino adults with at least one US-born parent have a greater risk for substance use disorder than White adults, again suggesting a differential process of US acculturation for Latinos compared to Asians. Future studies should aim to identify dimensions of US acculturation among White adults that can be used to compare processes across groups and identify what about the US context may affect White and Latino adults differently than other groups.

Of interest is our finding that Asians consistently had the lowest disorder prevalence. In models that stratified by nativity, we saw that both US and foreign-born Asian participants had lower rates of disorders than White participants. This disaggregation of race/ethnicity and nativity is new and suggests that factors other than nativity may partly explain the much lower rates of disorders among Asians. Previously, researchers have questioned whether the lower prevalence of disorders among Asians may be a methodological artifact due to under-reporting or a different conceptualization of disorders than among White respondents [55]. This explanation should be considered along with possible protective factors found in Asian communities. For example, Hornsey et al. [56] demonstrate that members of holistic cultures, such as those shaped by Buddhism, Confucianism, Hinduism, and Taoism, have more moderate expectations of their ideal levels of characteristics such as happiness, health and self-esteem when compared to non-holistic cultures. Other studies show that cultures vary in the extent to which experiencing negative affect is associated with psychological functioning; in one study comparing the US and Japan, experiencing negative affect predicted poor health (including psychological well-being) in both countries, but the magnitude of the effect was weaker in Japan than in the U.S [57]. These studies indicate that members of holistic and collectivistic cultures (which are often found in Asian countries) may interpret negative experiences and affect in a more neutral manner than members of individualistic cultures (which are often found in countries like the United States)—this tendency may in turn provide protection against the development of mental disorders.

Our analysis of the effects of education on racial/ethnic group and nativity differences failed to find effects of either parent or respondent education, though this finding is consistent with some prior studies [58]. Education may not be an equal marker of socioeconomic status across racial/ethnic groups, as groups that are more socially disadvantaged may experience less upward mobility even with increased education, for example, at higher levels of education, Black men and women earn less than their White counterparts [59]. Future analyses of this data will explore how race and socioeconomic status may interact in predicting mental health disorders.

Our study does not find that the intersection of race/ethnicity and nativity status explains lower risk for mental disorders among Black adults, despite prior analyses of the National Survey of American Life (NSAL; one of the studies comprising this sample), finding lower rates of mental disorders among first generation Caribbean Black adults compared to second and third generation [14]. In combining our samples, the overall weighted percentage of Black participants who were foreign-born or had foreign-born parents becomes lower than it was in the NSAL, perhaps limiting our ability to detect a Black nativity effect. Prior research has also indicated that patterns of mental health risk among Black Caribbean adults relative to African-American adults varied by gender [14], and these subgroup differences were not explored in this analysis. Finally, this study did not include variables that may explain lower risk of mental disorder among African–Americans, such as religiosity and personal psychological resources; future studies should examine differences in these variables when comparing across race/ethnicity groups. Black adults in this study had the highest level of ethnic identity as measured by one question, and a more comprehensive ethnic identity measure may have allowed us to further examine its role in reducing risk of mental disorders.

In addition to noteworthy findings, it is also important to recognize this study’s limitations. First, diagnoses were based on fully structured lay interviews rather than on gold standard semi-structured interviews. However, evidence suggests that CIDI diagnoses had good concordance with blinded clinical diagnoses [32]. Second, results are subject to recall bias because lifetime disorders and AOO of those disorders were assessed retrospectively. These biases typically lead to under-reporting of diagnoses [60, 61]. Third, timing of respondent’s educational attainment was not assessed in the surveys. To model respondent’s educational attainment as a time-varying covariate, we assumed high school completion at age 18 and college at age 22, though actual completion may have occurred at a later age. Variations in age of completion may have particularly impacted respondents in the study who were born outside the US. Fourth, we limited our analysis to cultural variables that were available across all surveys, precluding more fine-grained analysis of constructs such as ethnic identity and acculturation. Fifth, we were not able to disaggregate subgroups of Asian, Black, and Latino people in the study, despite prior research that has found subgroup differences [14, 15], due to low sample size when combined with nativity categories. Our findings may not apply to all subgroups within a larger category. Similarly, we were not able to disaggregate by age group within race/ethnicity/nativity categories, though risk factors may vary within distinct age groups such as older adults. Finally, we had low power for some interaction analyses given few respondents with particular combinations of race/ethnicity, nativity, and parent/respondent education. Thus, such negative findings should be interpreted with caution.

Overall, results demonstrate that both racial/ethnic group membership and nativity status continue to be salient factors in predicting lifetime risk of mental disorders, and that the effect of group membership is not fully accounted for by related sociodemographic factors. While some prior studies have undertaken in-depth analyses of cultural factors and acculturation processes within racial/ethnic groups, studies measuring and comparing these cultural dimensions across racial/ethnic and nativity groups—including both US and foreign-born White groups—are needed to understand the mechanisms driving group differences in mental health outcomes.