Introduction

Predation is a major selective force driving phenotypic diversification including adaptive variation in prey species’ life histories (Reznick and Endler 1982; Crowl and Covich 1990; Riesch et al. 2013), morphology (Walker 1997; Langerhans et al. 2004), and behavior (Dixon and Baker 1988; Lima and Dill 1990). Characters that provide protection from predation often show heritable (evolved) differences in mean trait expression between species or populations that are exposed to different predation regimes (Reznick 1982; Langerhans et al. 2004). However, building defensive traits can be associated with considerable costs (e.g., Stevens et al. 1999), and so inducible trait expression (i.e., phenotypic plasticity) ought to be favored under certain conditions (Hebert and Grewe 1985; Dodson 1989; Agrawal 2001; Benard 2004); but see, e.g., DeWitt et al. (1998) and Auld et al. (2009) for costs of phenotypic plasticity. Classic examples of inducible morphological responses to predation risk come from studies on water flees (genus Daphnia), where juveniles produce neck-teeth, helmet-like structures, or elongated spines on the dorsal surface of the carapace in response to predator presence (Krueger and Dodson 1981; Hebert and Grewe 1985; Dodson 1989). One condition that favors the evolution of inducible predator-defense traits is provided when individuals can move between habitats that differ in predation risk across generations. For example, insects with complex life cycles can experience starkly divergent predatory regimes during their (aquatic) larval stages (Wilbur 1980; Palmer and Poff 1997), but since imagines can move freely between water bodies for oviposition, successive generations often experience different predatory regimes, and so plastic responses to predators in larval life histories, morphology and behavior are to be expected (reviewed in Benard 2004).

Predation is often size-selective, and predators tend to consume large individuals to optimize their net energy uptake (Brooks and Dodson 1965; Reznick 1982; Wellborn 1994; Plath et al. 2003) unless gape limitation or problems of handling large prey items lead to different patterns (Werner 1974; Nilsson and Brönmark 2000). In holometabolous insects, life history responses to predation on larval stages ought to be governed by early emergence to evade predation, which can result in reduced body size at metamorphosis (Peckarsky et al. 2001; Benard 2004; Beketov and Liess 2007), as somatic growth is largely restricted to the larval stage (Nijhout and Wheeler 1996). However, even if accelerating larval development is impossible, larvae could still suppress growth to evade size-specific predation. On the other hand, previous studies on culicid larvae reported on delayed developmental times when exposed to kairomones, probably because larvae reduced activity and fed less, resulting in delayed development (Beketov and Liess 2007; van Uitregt et al. 2012). Some studies, however, found no developmental response to predation risk at all (e.g., Caudill and Peckarsky 2003 for larvae of the mayfly Callibaetis ferrugineus hageni), and so holometabolous insects seem to react to predator cues along a continuum of reaction norms.

Accelerated emergence times and/or suppressed larval growth ought to be balanced by trade-offs (sensu Wilbur 1980; Stearns 1989) involving the competitiveness of larvae and ultimately, adults’ realized reproductive potential (Bradshaw and Holzapfel 1992). Such a trade-off was demonstrated for the mosquito Aedes notoscriptus: when larvae of this Australian species were exposed to chemical cues of the native piscine predator Hypseleotris galii they reduced activity and, therefore, were better at avoiding predation than larvae that were not previously exposed to predator cues. However, predator-exposed larvae had reduced larval growth and development, were smaller at metamorphosis and less resistant to starvation than non-exposed ones (van Uitregt et al. 2012). Larvae of our study species, the common house mosquito Culex pipiens molestus (Forskal), inhabit stagnant or slow-flowing waters, ranging from small temporary puddles to large oxbow lakes (Vinogradova 2000). While transient water bodies are mostly predator-free environments, larvae in permanent (and mostly larger) water bodies are heavily preyed upon by multiple predators, including fishes, notonectid backswimmers, dytiscid beetles, dragonfly larvae, and others (Vinogradova 2000). Like several other aquatic invertebrates, Culex larvae detect predators through kairomones—natural chemicals released by their potential predators (Dodson et al. 1994; Ferrari et al. 2010). Little is known about the chemical structure of kairomones; an ongoing debate considers the question of whether (and to what extent) the predator’s diet (Huryn and Chivers 1999; Beketov and Liess 2007), its cutaneous mucus (Forward and Rittschof 2000; Alvarez et al. 2014) or its bacterial flora (Beklioglu et al. 2006) affect the chemical composition of kairomones. However, there is evidence that kairomones differ widely even among similar taxa (Lass and Spaak 2003; Relyea 2003; Ferrari et al. 2010). When exposed to kairomones originating from aquatic heteropterans or beetles, mosquito larvae (Culex spp. and Aedes spp.) reduce their activity (Sih 1986; Ohba et al. 2012), and imagines appear to use kairomones of larval predators as a cue to avoid high-predation environments when searching for oviposition sites (Spencer et al. 2002; Ohba et al. 2012; Afify and Galizia 2015).

A number of studies focused on predator–prey interactions between culicid mosquito larvae (including the genus Culex) and non-indigenous predators that have received attention as potential biological control agents against these vectors of human diseases (e.g., Krumholz 1948; Rosenheim et al. 1995; Kumar and Hwang 2006). For example, Offill and Walton (1999) compared a common native predator of Culex spp., the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus Linnaeus) and Gambusia affinis (Baird and Girard), an introduced predator that is often used in mosquito control programs (Krumholz 1948), in terms of their efficiency of larval predation and found higher predation rates in G. affinis. However, predator–prey interactions between the widely distributed common house mosquito and its common native predator in Europe, G. aculeatus (see Medlock and Snow 2008), have received little attention in empirical research. Therefore, our first question was whether stickleback predation on C. pipiens is size-selective, and we conducted a prey choice experiment in which individual sticklebacks were offered two different size classes of mosquito larvae to answer this question. Our first experiment corroborated an increased predation risk of large-bodied larvae (see “Results”), and so we conducted another experiment to investigate potential predator-induced larval life history responses by raising larvae in water containing kairomones from sticklebacks, or water without kairomones in the control treatment. Specifically, we asked whether (size-specific) predation risk results in an accelerated developmental time and/or reduced larval body size, as measured by the size at pupation. Because we detected several life history responses when larvae were exposed to stickleback kairomones, we expanded our study and asked if mosquito larvae respond specifically to the presence of naturally co-occurring insectivorous fishes, or if any fish species elicits those responses. This question is of particular interest because of the increasing impact of invasive species—including non-native teleost fishes—in freshwaters worldwide (Mack et al. 2000; Sakai et al. 2001). Native, predator-naive prey species may be more vulnerable to predation by introduced, unfamiliar predators due to their inability to recognize novel predators and to show adaptive antipredator responses (Salo et al. 2007). In our third experiment we therefore compared life histories of native mosquito larvae exposed to no kairomones (control), kairomones from a non-native algivorous fish (Ancistrus sp.), sticklebacks, and from Eastern mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki Girard). The latter species is a severe predator of mosquitoes that has been introduced to southern Europe from the USA for malaria prophylaxis in the 1920s (Vidal et al. 2010), but does not presently co-occur with mosquitoes in Germany, where our study population of C. pipiens originated. We predicted the strongest shift in life histories to occur in response to chemical cues of sticklebacks, but a weaker or no response at all when exposed to kairomones from Ancistrus and mosquitofish. In this experiment, we focused on size and weight measurements of imagines, which allowed determination of whether and how larval life history shifts (as seen in our second experiment) translate into an altered adult body size. Taken together, our present study is the first of its kind to not only demonstrate altered larval development in response to predator cues, but to demonstrate how developmental plasticity relates to size-specific predation risk, and to what extent co-evolved and invasive alien (i.e., not co-evolved) predators elicit the same or different responses in essential Culex life-history traits.

Materials and methods

Study organisms and their maintenance

A randomly outbred laboratory strain of Culex pipiens molestus (Culicidae), founded from wild-caught animals collected near Regensburg, Germany, was obtained from Biogents AG (Regensburg). The subspecies C. p. molestus is autogenous, stenogamous, remains active throughout the year, and mainly feeds on mammalian, especially human blood (Harbach et al. 1984). Females produce at least one egg-raft after emergence, for which they do not obligatorily require a blood meal (Twohy and Rozeboom 1957). Mosquitoes were maintained as randomly outbred stocks consisting of several hundred imagines at 22 °C in two cages (60 cm × 60 cm × 60 cm gauze-covered frames), which were equipped with a water-filled Petri dish. Adult females were fed on saturated grape-sugar solution, which we offered ad libitum in the form of sugar-water soaked paper towels. Egg-rafts were removed from the culture and transferred to 10-L aquaria with equal amounts of deionized and tap water for hatching. Larvae were fed weekly on commercially available fine-ground fish food (Tetra Min®).

All fishes used to produce kairomones for the tests were maintained in aerated and filtered ≥80-L tanks at 21 °C and fed ad libitum with flake food twice a day. Three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus; Gasterosteidae) are widely distributed throughout Europe; they feed mainly on crustaceans and aquatic insects (Hynes 1950). Sticklebacks were collected in a small creek in Niederursel, Germany, and were kept in the laboratory for 2–3 weeks before experimental use. In our third experiment we also included a laboratory strain of Eastern mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki; Poeciliidae), presumably of Floridian origin. Mosquitofish were actively released in southern Europe during the 20th century for mosquito prophylaxis, and are nowadays present in nearly all southern European water bodies (Vidal et al. 2010; pers. obs. for Italian, Spanish and southern French streams), but are currently not known to have established permanent populations in Germany. Our third experiment included a domestic form of the algivorous South American armored catfish (Ancistrus sp. Loricariidae), which we obtained from a commercial aquarium breeder.

To prepare water containing specific fish kairomones, groups of fish were transferred into aerated and filtered 10-L aquaria that were maintained at 21 °C. In an attempt to standardize kairomone concentrations, we combined small groups of stimulus fish such that their cumulative body size (standard length, SL) would equal about 160 mm [4 individuals per aquarium for Ancistrus (mean ± SE, SL = 39.3 ± 1.0 mm), 4 sticklebacks (39.6 ± 2.4 mm) and 5–6 mosquitofish (26.8 ± 1.5 mm)]. Fishes were daily fed ad libitum with Tetra Min® fish food. Water was refilled every day after water had been removed for the experiments described below (Experiments 2 and 3).

Experiment 1: prey choice of three-spined sticklebacks

Prey choice tests with sticklebacks were performed in 12-L aquaria. Test tanks were aerated between trials, but the air-stone was removed before mosquito larvae were introduced. Before each trial, we collected 14 larvae from our stock culture, i.e., seven from each of two visibly different size classes. Two individuals per size class were randomly taken from this sample and fixed in 70 % ethanol for subsequent size determination [large size class, mean (±SE) length from the cephalothorax to the tip of abdominal segment VIII: 5.2 ± 0.1 mm; small size class: 2.8 ± 0.1 mm). Since our aim was to detect general patterns of size-selective predation in sticklebacks, we chose mosquito larvae that clearly differed in size, which allowed visual differentiation of size classes by the observer. To initiate a trial, an individual stickleback (SL: 39.2 ± 1.4 mm) was introduced into the test tank and allowed to acclimate for 10 min. Afterwards, the remaining 10 larvae were gently introduced into the test tank. We observed the behavior of the focal fish from approximately 1 m distance and terminated a trial (i.e., removed the focal fish from the test tank) after five larvae had been eaten. All surplus larvae were retrieved from the test tank and their size class noted. In total, we conducted n = 31 independent trials.

Experiment 2: life history responses to stickleback kairomones (pupae size)

To determine larval responses to stickleback kairomones, larvae were reared in 100 mL vessels filled with 60 mL water (equal amounts of deionized and tap water). Each vessel was covered with fine nylon gauze. We introduced eight newly hatched (L1) larvae per vessel and thus tested 10 replicates (80 L1 larvae) in both treatments (control and stickleback kairomones; i.e., n = 160 larvae altogether). Larvae were fed on ground fish food until pupation. The consumption of food per larvae increased stepwise as follows: 0.5 mg (hatching day), 0.5 mg (day 2), 0.5 mg (day 4), 0.5 mg (day 5), and 1 mg per day from day 7 onwards. All experiments were conducted in a climate chamber at 21 °C, with 60 % humidity and a 12:12 h LD photoperiod. We exchanged 50 % of the water in the test vessels every day and replaced it with kairomone-water from the respective fish tanks or untreated water (according to the treatment). We used water from two replicate stimulus tanks containing sticklebacks (cumulative SL = 166 ± 3 mm) and two equal-sized and similarly equipped tanks without fish (control treatment).

Twice a day all mosquito test vessels were checked for mortality, and all dead larvae were immediately removed. Simultaneously, we checked for pupae, which were fixed in 70 % ethanol. We determined the sex of pupae based on the gonocoxopodites, which are large and partially bilobed in males but small and spiculate in females (cp. Harbach et al. 1984; personal observation). The same parameters as described in Müller et al. (2013) were measured under a stereo microscope: abdominal length (AL) from the third to the eighth segment, abdominal width (AW) at the fifth segment, and the area of the cephalothorax (CT) in lateral view.

We asked if any observable life history responses of the mosquito larvae can be ascribed to the presence of fish kairomones or whether it represents a response to metabolic waste products of fish and their related degradation products. We therefore analyzed nitrate and phosphate concentrations in randomly selected experimental vessels, the fish tanks and in the tanks containing control water using colorimetric tests (Merck KGaA, Darmstadt, Germany). Two nitrate and phosphate measurements were conducted per treatment. Sensitivity of the colorimetric tests is low, however, all measurements for nitrate and phosphate were uniformly high in the experimental mosquito vessels (~10 mg l−1 NO3 ; >0.43 mg l−1 H3PO4) and considerably lower in the fish and control tanks (~5 mg l−1 NO3 ; ~0.3 mg l−1 H3PO4). Therefore, waste products of the larvae themselves seem to have affected nitrate and phosphate concentrations in the test vessels, but the addition of fish (or control) water had no obvious effect on this.

Experiment 3: life history responses to different predator types (imago size)

In our third experiment, we used a similar approach as described before, but collected imagines directly after hatching. We set up three replicate stimulus tanks for each of the following fish species and three (empty) control tanks (i.e., 12 stimulus tanks in total): Ancistrus sp. (cumulative SL = 157 ± 4 mm), G. aculeatus (158 ± 3 mm) and G. holbrooki (152 ± 1 mm). We conducted 15 replicates (using 120 larvae) per treatment, amounting to a total sample size of n = 480 larvae.

Emerged imagines were frozen in individual 1.5 mL Eppendorf tubes at −80 °C until further processing. Mosquitoes were then dried at 60 °C for a minimum of 24 h, after which their dry weight was recorded using a Sartorius 4503 microbalance (accuracy: 1 µg). Wing length (as a proxy for body size) was determined to the nearest 0.1 mm as the distance from the axial incision to the R1 vein (Kreß et al. 2014) using a dissecting microscope (Nikon AZ100 Multizoom, Nikon Instruments Europe, Amsterdam, Netherlands) connected to a digital camera (Nikon DS-Fi1) with an image-analyzing system (NIS Elements BR, version 3.22.11, Laboratory Imaging). To determine fat content mosquitos were rinsed four times for at least 1.5 h with petroleum ether to extract nonpolar, non-structural lipids, then dried again and reweighed (Heulett et al. 1995; Riesch et al. 2010).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Numbers of large and small larvae consumed in experiment 1 were compared using a Wilcoxon signed rank test. We used multivariate General Linear Models (GLM) to compare life history traits between treatments in experiments 2 and 3; dependent data were z-transformed to standardize units. In experiment 2, ‘developmental time until pupation’ (days), ‘dry weight’ (mg), ‘cephalotorax area’ (mm2) ‘abdominal width’ (mm) and ‘abdominal length’ (mm) were treated as the dependent variables, and in experiment 3 ‘developmental time until emergence’ (days), ‘dry weight’ (mg), ‘wing length’ (mm) and arcsine(square root)-transformed ‘fat-content’ (%). ‘Sex’, ‘treatment’ and their interaction were used as fixed factors. Since the interaction terms had no significant effects (experiment 2: F 5,127 = 1.37, P = 0.24; experiment 3: F 12,784 = 0.99, P = 0.46), they were removed from the final models. To identify the source of variation in case of significant treatment effects we used univariate GLMs on all four variables separately (using non-transformed data) and employed LSD tests for post hoc pairwise comparisons between treatments. We illustrated significant effects using estimated marginal means from the respective analytical models. Mortality data were analyzed using survival analysis: a log-rank test was performed to check if larval mortality rates varied among treatments.

Results

Experiment 1: prey choice of three-spined sticklebacks

Sticklebacks consumed more large than small larvae in 29 of 31 trials, and a Wilcoxon signed rank test confirmed that overall more large larvae were consumed (i.e., 72.9 ± 3.2 % of consumed larvae; z = −4.63, P < 0.01, n = 31; Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Prey-choice preference of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) between large- (body length: 5.2 ± 0.1 mm) and small-bodied (2.8 ± 0.1 mm) mosquito larvae

Experiment 2: life history responses to stickleback kairomones (pupae size)

A multivariate GLM uncovered significant treatment (F 5,128 = 7.15, P < 0.001) and sex effects (F 5,128 = 179.25, P < 0.001) on the four investigated life history traits. Univariate GLMs—as a touch-down approach to uncover the source of variation—revealed that developmental time was not affected by exposure to stickleback kairomones, while cephalothorax area, abdominal width, abdominal length, and dry weight were significantly reduced in the stickleback treatment (Table 1; Fig. 2). Differences between sexes are reported in supplementary Table S1.

Table 1 Results from ANCOVAs of the experiment 2 using ‘kairomone treatment’ and ‘sex’ as fixed factors
Fig. 2
figure 2

Effects of kairomones on life history traits of Culex pipiens pupae reared as larvae in absence (wave symbol, left) or presence (stickleback drawing, right) of chemical cues from Gasterosteus aculeatus. a Pupae of C. pipiens with additional illustration of the measured distances: c cephalothorax area, d abdominal width and e abdominal length. bf Dependent variables assessed in experiment 2 are depicted as back-transformed estimated marginal means (±SE) from a GLM. Shown are b the time until pupation, c cephalothorax area, d abdominal width, e abdominal length and f dry weight of pupae

Survival analysis revealed that the mortality of larvae did not differ between treatments (log-rank test, χ 2 = 3.59, P = 0.06). While the marginally non-significant effect suggests a treatment effect, larvae exposed to kairomones actually had slightly increased (not decreased) survival (Fig. 3a). Mortality in the control treatment was 20.7 %, which meets the criteria of acceptable experimental baseline mortality for ecotoxicological tests in the non-biting midge Chironomus riparius (Chironomidae) provided in OECD guideline no. 219 (i.e., 30 %; OECD 2004).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Survival curve of Culex pipiens larvae reared a in experiment 2, and b in experiment 3

Experiment 3: life history responses to different predator types (imago size)

A multivariate GLM detected significant treatment (F 12,791 = 2.86, P = 0.001) and sex effects (F 4,299 = 342.61, P < 0.001) on the four investigated life history traits. Univariate GLMs found significant differences in dry weight (Fig. 4b) and body size (Fig. 4c), while developmental duration (Fig. 4a) and fat content (Fig. 4d) did not differ between treatments (Table 2). Post-hoc tests revealed that average dry weight was significantly lower in larvae exposed to any of the three fish treatments compared to the control treatment (LSD tests: P < 0.01 in all cases; Fig. 4b). Body size (wing length) was significantly smaller in the stickleback treatment than in the control (P = 0.011) and Ancistrus treatments (P = 0.004; Fig. 4c). Body size of mosquitoes from the G. holbrooki treatment was also slightly reduced (1.03 %), but was not significantly different from the other treatments (P > 0.05 in all cases). In addition, we found pronounced differences between sexes, with males emerging faster than females, having a lower dry weight, smaller wing length and a higher fat content than females (Table 2; for descriptive statistics see supplementary Table 2).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effects of kairomones on life history traits of Culex pipiens imagines when larvae were reared in absence (control) or presence of chemical cues from non-predatory, algivorous Ancistrus sp., the native predator Gasterosteus aculeatus and the non-native predator Gambusia holbrooki. Shown are back-transformed estimated marginal means (±SE) from a GLM, for a developmental time until emergence, b dry weight, c body size and d fat content of emerged imagines. Letters above the bars represent the results of LSD tests for pairwise post hoc comparisons, where different letters indicate significant differences between treatments (P < 0.05)

Table 2 Results from ANCOVAs on four variables assessed in experiment 3 using ‘kairomone treatment’ and ‘sex’ as fixed factors, and ‘developmental duration’, ‘dry weight’, ‘wing length’ and ‘fat content’ as the dependent variables

The survival of C. pipiens larvae did not significantly differ between treatments (log-rank test, χ 2 = 3.50, P = 0.32); mortality in control treatment was 16.6 % (Fig. 3b).

Discussion

We found sticklebacks to exert size-selective predation upon C. pipiens larvae (“Experiment 1”). In case of newly hatched (L1) larvae exposed to stickleback kairomones, pupae became significantly smaller and lighter compared to the control treatment without kairomones (“Experiment 2”). These observations were corroborated by investigating imagines, where we found a strong reduction in body size after exposure to stickleback kairomones (“Experiment 3”); however, body weight was reduced in all fish kairomone treatments irrespective of fish identity.

Size selective predation on aquatic arthropods by piscine predators is well documented for other teleost fishes; e.g., bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) prefer large amphipod prey (Wellborn 1994), pumpkinseed sunfish (L. gibbosus) prefer large dragonfly larvae (Dixon and Baker 1988), and brook trout (Salvelinus fontinalis) exhibit a preference for large mayfly larvae (Allan 1978). We argue that lower predation risk of small-sized C. pipiens larvae by their common piscine predator in European freshwaters, the three-spined stickleback (G. aculeatus), provides them with a relative advantage over larger larvae in sites with high levels of predation, and so we asked if mosquitoes respond to predation risk with altered larval life histories, especially with suppressed growth, which was confirmed in experiment 2.

We further asked how specific this response is, and whether mosquito larvae differ in their responses to a co-evolved piscine predator, a not co-evolved (invasive) predator, and a non-insectivorous fish. Investigating the response of native prey organisms to novel (invasive) predators is of particular interest in light of the steady increase of biological invasions worldwide (Mack et al. 2000; Sakai et al. 2001). Different predators usually release predator-specific chemical profiles (Relyea 2001; Iyengar and Harvell 2002; Relyea 2003), but it remains to be investigated if prey species can respond to novel predator types with which they have not coevolved. Moreover, our second experiment as well as several previous studies reporting suppressed growth of mosquito larvae exposed to predator kairomones (e.g., van Uitregt et al. 2012 for A. notoscriptus; Ohba et al. 2012 for C. tritaeniorhynchus) did not answer the question of whether suppressed larval body growth is merely an indirect consequence of reduced activity (and thus, reduced feeding), or if mosquito larvae actively alter larval growth patterns as an adaptive life history response to evade predation—experiment 3 in our present study provides answers to both questions.

Our prediction for an adaptive response to predation risk was that larvae might accelerate larval development to evade predation (see “Introduction”), but we found no support for such a pattern. Neither did we find delayed developmental times in our present study (an effect found in previous studies, e.g., Beketov and Liess 2007; van Uitregt et al. 2012), and so it seems that developmental times show rather narrow reaction norms in our study population. Still, the results of experiment 3 suggest that mosquito larvae exhibit an unspecific stress response to kairomones from different fish species, as it seems plausible to explain reduced body weight as a consequence of reduced activity and thus, reduced food uptake (compare Beketov and Liess 2007; van Uitregt et al. 2012). Generally, time spent foraging correlates positively with the likelihood of being detected by visual predators or encountering ambush predators, and so individuals typically decrease foraging under predation risk (Lima and Dill 1990; Benard 2004; Stoks et al. 2005). Also C. pipiens larvae were found to move less when exposed to kairomones from a heteropteran predator, the backswimmer Notonecta undulata (Sih 1986), and reduced foraging under the influence of predator kairomones was also reported from other culicid larvae (Kesavaraju and Juliano 2004; Ohba et al. 2012).

Moreover, we detected another effect—reduced body size—that was strongest in the treatment with stickleback kairomones, considerably weaker when kairomones of insectivorous, but not co-evolved, Gambusia were present, while no effect at all was seen in the Ancistrus treatment. Since we did not measure activity in our study, a link between reduced foraging and reduced body size cannot be excluded. However, if this explanation was true, and if our interpretation is correct that equally reduced body weight in all three fish kairomone treatments is the result of reduced activity (see above), then we would have expected at least slightly reduced body size also in the treatment using Ancistrus kairomones, but no such pattern was uncovered (Fig. 4c). We calculated the ratio between body weight and body size. The non-specific body weight reduction in all kairomone treatments was reflected by a high body mass-to-body size ratio in the control treatment (0.270; calculated from EMMs), but the highest ratio among the fish kairomone treatments was detected when Gasterosteus kairomones were presented (Ancistrus: 0.254, Gasterosteus: 0.261, Gambusia: 0.254), and so it appears as if larvae are indeed actively suppressing growth and thus, become ‘denser’ (i.e., have a higher weight-to-body size ratio compared to the Ancistrus and Gambusia treatments) to reduce predation risk in the presence of their common predator. This suggests that, at least in C. pipiens, the evolutionary history of this predator–prey interaction seems to be more important than the relative risk of predation posed by the predator, and some component of sticklebacks’ chemical cues may allow a specific recognition of the coevolved predator. The significance of evolutionary history in predator–prey recognition was also observed by Alvarez et al. (2014), who investigated predator avoidance behavior of mayfly larvae (Baetis spp.) to chemical cues (cutaneous mucus). The authors showed that larvae exhibited no behavioral response to novel predator species, while larvae did respond to five co-occurring freshwater fishes. However, the response to co-occurring fishes was not predator-specific even though fish species differed in their strength of predation on mayfly larvae. Given that all fish species in our study obtained the same diet, species-specific kairomones likely stemmed from a combination of cutaneous mucus and mucosa-associated bacteria (Beklioglu et al. 2006; Alvarez et al. 2014).

With regards to reduced larval body size, our results suggest that size reduction confers benefits under predation risk. However, since body size of female mosquitoes correlates positively with fecundity (Briegel 1990; Lyimo and Takken 1993; McCann et al. 2009), this reduction may come at a cost at the adult stage, essentially leading to a life-history trade-off. Furthermore, van Uitregt et al. (2012) found smaller mosquito imagines to be less resistant to starvation, while larger imagines can have longer reproductive life-spans (Neems et al. 1990), have an increased ability to disperse (Kaufmann et al. 2013), and tend to be superior in mate competition (Wellborn and Bartholf 2005). This trade-off between costs and benefits of suppressed larval growth likely governs the evolution of the remarkable plasticity of larval life histories in C. pipiens we describe here, as subsequent larval generations can experience starkly different piscine predation pressures.

Several studies have shown how alternative phenotypes can be induced through alterations of hormone release and enzymatic activity, as well as altered gene expression mediated by DNA methylation and transcription factor activation (Gilbert and Epel 2009; Miyakawa et al. 2010; Snell-Rood et al. 2010; Beldade et al. 2011; Sommer and Ogawa 2011; Schneider et al. 2014). These mechanisms are highly interactive: environmental cues can affect the dynamics of hormone production and thus, trigger changes in hormone titers, while hormones can affect gene expression (Gilbert and Epel 2009; Beldade et al. 2011). Future studies will need to address the molecular mechanisms underlying adaptive life-history shifts of mosquito larvae exposed to predator kairomones. This may also answer the question of how specific kairomones of the investigated fish species are and how these affect the entailing cascades. The reduced body weight in all fish treatments (Fig. 4b) implies that this particular response is triggered by chemical cues shared by a broad array of teleost species, whereas the reduction in body size was much more specific: the co-evolved predator triggered the strongest response, but a weak (albeit not significant) response was also triggered by Gambusia. This may be explained by the greater phylogenetic proximity between sticklebacks and Gambusia (Betancur-R et al. 2013), resulting in somewhat similar chemical profiles. Furthermore, it remains to be tested if mosquitoes will evolve predator recognition in regions where Gambusia is invasive.

Finally, the picture becomes even more complex when other studies on adaptive responses to invasive alien species are evaluated. Some studies found no behavioral anti-predator response of native species to an unknown predator (e.g., Kesavaraju and Juliano 2004; Kesavaraju et al. 2007), whereas others found morphological and behavioral adaptations to invasive predators (e.g., Flecker 1992; Pease and Wayne 2014). Thus, our study demonstrates that—with regards to invertebrate prey presented with chemical cues of potential predators—the lack of a specific response does not always equate to an inability of the prey species to discriminate. Rather, the antipredator response can be finely nuanced so that only certain traits exhibit a predator-specific response, while other traits show similar responses across a range of potential (co-evolved native or invasive alien) predators.

Author contribution statement

JJ, RM and MP conceived and designed the study. JJ and JB conducted the experiments. JJ, RR, RM and MP analyzed data. RM and MP provided laboratory, rearing and infrastructural possibilities. All authors wrote, read and approved the manuscript.