Introduction

Xenobiotics are increasingly emitted in the environment as a consequence of human activities. Among xenobiotics, a group of compounds, the persistent organic pollutants (POPs), share common characteristics: They are persistent in the environment and have lipophilic properties and high chemical stability, which confer them a strong capacity for bioaccumulation. As aquatic compartments, including sediments, often constitute an ultimate reservoir for these compounds, fish could be exposed at various life stages to POPs. Besides such direct exposure, POPs can be transmitted from parents to offspring, through blood during gestation and later in milk, and induce physiological effects in the post-natal period for mammals (Crépeaux et al. 2012, 2013; Palanza et al. 2008; Wormley et al. 2004). For egg-laying species, embryonic exposure occurs mainly through yolk (e.g., for PCBs, see Bodiguel et al. 2009; Daouk et al. 2011; Ottinger et al. 2013). In addition to this indirect parental exposure route, physiological disruption in parents can also have consequences in offspring such as the alteration of signaling molecule and/or hormonal transfer: For example, the transmission of elevated cortisol levels in fish eggs after parental stress experience has been shown to have physiological and behavioral consequences in juveniles (Auperin and Geslin 2008; McCormick et al. 1998).

Further, there is a growing body of evidence that POPs can act as neurotoxicant especially in the case of early exposure (Ottinger et al. 2013; Scott and Sloman 2004). This has been demonstrated in various animal species, and there are strong correlations in humans between concentration of some POPs, especially in cord blood and latter cognition deficiencies or behavioral disruptions (Edwards et al. 2010; Jedrychowski et al. 2003; Letcher et al. 2010; Perera et al. 2012; Sagiv et al. 2010; Stewart et al. 2000; Tang et al. 2008).

Among POPs, polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs) constitute a wide family of compounds formed by the association of several aromatic rings. In addition, there are also PAH derivatives substituted with alkyl groups, nitrogen, sulfur, or oxygen. PAHs and their derivatives are produced during organic matter combustion (pyrolytic PAHs) or are present in oils (petrogenic PAHs). In all cases, PAHs are present as complex mixtures including a large number of PAHs and derivatives. PAH mixture composition differs depending on their origin. Pyrolytic mixtures are characterized by high proportions of heavy PAHs such as benzo[a]pyrene (BaP) and few alkylated PAHs while petrogenic mixtures contain less heavy PAHs but more light ones and higher proportions of alkylated PAHs (Benlahcen et al. 1997; Budzinski et al. 1997; Latimer and Zheng 2003; Neff 1979; Yunker et al. 2002). PAHs released into the environment have increased over the last decades (Eisler 1987; Shen et al. 2013; Zhang and Tao 2009). Monitoring networks have documented PAH concentrations of up to 50 μg g−1 dry weight (dw) in sediment from various affected aquatic ecosystems, although the concentrations in highly contaminated areas are more commonly in the 10 μg g−1 range (Baumard et al. 1998; Benlahcen et al. 1997; Cachot et al. 2006; Johnson et al. 2007; Varanasi et al. 1993; Yanagida et al. 2012). The concentration of PAH in the biota varies depending on the surrounding concentration and trophic level. For example, the total concentration of 16 PAHs used as indicators by the US Environmental Protection Agency (US-EPA) in the copepod Eurytemora affinis in the Seine Estuary is in the 0.2–3.9 μg g−1 dw range (Cailleaud et al. 2007), and in mussels, it can be as high as 1.6 μg g−1 dw (Le Goff et al. 2006). In the case of acute accidental exposure, the concentration in mussels can be even higher, for example 3 μg g−1 dw after the Erika oil spill (Jeanneret et al. 2002) and 14.4 μg g−1 dw after Exxon Valdez oil spill (Payne et al. 2008). PAHs bioaccumulate in parts of the lower levels of the food chain in which organisms have poor PAH metabolization activities; consequently, food is an important exposure route. This has been demonstrated by the high concentrations of PAHs found in the stomachs of fish caught in contaminated areas (Johnson et al. 2007; Varanasi et al. 1993; Yanagida et al. 2012).

Few studies have described the behavioral disruptions consecutive to an exposure to PAHs in fish. In most cases, exposure has been performed using waterborne exposure of juveniles to unique compounds (Almeida et al. 2012; Correia et al. 2007; Goncalves et al. 2008; Gravato and Guilhermino 2009; Oliveira et al. 2012). In addition, one study has also described additive effects when using a mixture of three PAHs (Goncalves et al. 2008). The general conclusion of these studies is that PAHs produced lethargy revealed by a decrease of exposed fish swimming ability. More recently, studies have reported behavioral disruption in medaka and zebrafish exposed to PAHs spiked sediment at embryo and larvae stages. Behavioral disruptions were observed either in larvae only (Le Bihanic et al. 2014a) or in larvae, juveniles, and adults (Vignet et al. 2014a). Finally, in another study, fish were continuously exposed through diet to three mixtures of PAHs starting at the first meal (5 days post-fertilization; dpf). In this study, several behavioral disruptions were observed, indicative of the induction of an anxiety-like syndrome (Vignet et al. 2014b).

Besides such direct consequences of exposures through water, sediment, or food, the question of the consequences in fish in the next generation is of utmost importance particularly when building on current mammalian literature underlining several neurotoxicological defects due to maternal exposure to PAHs (Crépeaux et al. 2012, 2013). Several mechanisms may be envisioned including a transfer of PAHs and their metabolites to eggs which can result in a very early exposure of developing embryos and/or transmission of biased signaling molecule such as steroid hormones as cited above, resulting in a degraded ovocytes quality and finally an epigenetic imprinting.

Mechanisms underlying behavioral disruptions after PAH exposure remain generally unknown. Several pathways have been proposed including disruption in cortisol, neurotransmitters, and estrogen pathways. The later hypothesis of estrogen disruption is of particular interest if we consider the fact that PAHs are suspected to disrupt endocrine regulation, and it is now documented in different taxa that early estrogenic imprinting drives several organ development, gender-specific brain development, and associated behaviors (Ottinger et al. 2009, 2013; Panzica et al. 2007; Stocco 2012).

In the present study, we have analyzed behavioral responses of the offspring of dietary exposed fish. Founding generations (F0) have been exposed through diet to three complex mixtures of PAHs representative of environmental situations: a pyrolitic mixture (PY) and two petrogenic mixtures (Vignet et al. 2014c). The behavior of their offspring (F1) was analyzed at larval stage. The behavior of F1-PY has also been analyzed in 2-month-old juveniles. The possible endocrine disruption effects of PAHs were explored by measuring the brain estrogenic activity using transgenic line expressing GFP under the control of brain aromatase (cyp19a1b:GFP). In addition, the behavior of F2-PY larvae has also been monitored.

Material and methods

This study was conducted under the approval of the Animal Care Committee of France under the official licence of M.-L. Bégout (17-010).

Fish rearing ad offspring production

We used the common laboratory TU strain (ZFIN ID: ZDBGENO-990623-3), which was established in the 1990s in Tubingen and in our platform (PEP; http://wwz.ifremer.fr/pep) 6 years ago as large batches of individuals originating from the Amagen platform (Gif/Yvette, France) and the Pasteur Institute fish facility (Paris, France). For the assessment of brain glial cell aromatase expression, we used a transgenic line cyp19a1b:GFP (Tong et al. 2009). Both lines were contaminated through diet following the protocol described by Vignet et al. (2014c) with three PAH mixtures and reared until adulthood for offspring production. We used i) a pyrolytic fraction (PY) extracted from sediments collected in the polluted site of the Seine Estuary (Oissel, France) and ii) two petrogenic fractions obtained from Erika fuel (heavy oil, HO) and Arabian light crude oil (LO). PAH extractions were performed, as previously described (Cachot et al. 2006). Zebrafish were exposed from their first meal (at 5 dpf) onwards and exposed using spiked size-graded food pellets (INICIO Plus 0.5, Biomar, France). Three concentrations, 0.3X, 1X and 3X were used with the 1X concentration corresponding to the Σ[16 EPA PAH] at 5 μg g−1 dw food, representative of the concentrations found in mollusks in the Seine Estuary. Diets are named after the origin of the fraction and its concentration: 1X pyrolytic fraction diet will be named, PY-1X. For each exposure, a fourth control treatment has been included corresponding to the plain food treated as spiked food with dichloromethane which was used as carrier solvent for PAHs spiking. Control and exposed fish were maintained at 27 °C in a controlled 14-h light/10-h dark (14:10) photoperiod and were fed twice a day with spiked diet between 0900 and 0930 hours in the morning and 1630 and 1730 hours in the afternoon and once with plain artemia nauplii (INVE AQUACULTURE nv, Dendermonde, Belgium) between 1130 and 1230 hours.

When the onset of spawning was confirmed (4-month-old PY and HO, 5-month-old LO, and 3-month-old fish for cyp19a1b:GFP), the eggs were obtained by random pairwise or group mating. Eggs were collected in the morning, and the fertilization rate was assessed within 2 h of collection. At the same time, spawns were sorted to remove feces and dead or unfertilized embryos. Developing embryos and larvae were maintained in E3 medium (5 mM NaCl, 0.17 mM KCl, 0.33 mM CaCl2, and 0.33 mM MgSO4) at 28 °C in Petri dishes in an incubator with the same photoperiod as adults. After hatching, chorions were removed manually, usually at 3 dpf, and larvae were used for behavioral experiments.

For PY only, some larvae were also bred according to standard protocols (Nüsslein-Volhard and Dahm 2002) and fed with artemias and size-graded plain pellets (INICIO Plus 0.5, Biomar, France) from 5 dpf onwards until fish reached sexual maturity. F1-PY adult fish were then used to produce offspring (F2-PY) using the same procedure as described above.

Behavioral experiments

We used 4–7 days old F1 larvae issued from PY, LO, and HO parents; 2-month-old sex undifferentiated F1 fish issued from PY parents; and 5-day-old larvae (F2-PY) issued from F1-PY fish for behavioral tests. Tests were done in a dedicated room kept at 27 °C ± 1 °C, with a 14:10 photoperiod synchronized with the rearing room so as to minimize unwanted correlated effects. Daylight started at 0830, and there were no twilight transition periods. F0 exposures were performed successively for the three mixtures (Vignet et al. 2014c); the same is applied for F1 production and behavioral assays. For all tests, we tried as much as possible to evaluate an equal number of F1 larvae from parents exposed to all concentrations (control, 0.3X, 1X, and 3X). In particular, for assays involving 24-well plates, we tried to have larvae from all F0 in the same plate to avoid any trial effect.

Photomotor response in F1 5 dpf larvae

We monitored the photomotor response (Burgess and Granato 2007; Emran et al. 2008) in 5 dpf larvae in a light/dark challenge, following a method slightly modified from that of Ali et al. (2012) and described in details by Vignet et al. (2013). This method is used extensively in the behavioral profiling of zebrafish larvae exposed to diverse compounds for drug screening and to assess anxiety level in larvae.

At 1700, the day before the challenge (4 dpf), single larvae were transferred to individual wells of a 24-well plate (Krystal 24, opaque wall and clear bottom microplate) where they were arranged in a mixed design and visually isolated from each other; the four corner wells were left empty because video acquisition suffered distortion. The 24-well plates were kept overnight in the breeding incubator. The following day, 2 h before the challenge, well plate was transferred to the video acquisition room and placed on top of a size-matched infrared floor, which allowed the larvae to be filmed under both light and dark conditions (Vignet et al. 2013). After a 10-min acclimation period, the 15-min challenge included three 5-min periods: light on-1 (70 lx.), light off (<1 lx.), and light on-2. Challenges were conducted between 1400 and 1800, the most stable activity period in zebrafish larvae (MacPhail et al. 2009). Constant IR lighting was maintained during filming using a three-way switch. The apparatus was enclosed within a lightproof and temperature-controlled box. Locomotor activity was assessed as previously described (Vignet et al. 2013) as well as inner vs. outer area occupancy after (Schnorr et al. 2012). The dependent variables measured were distance travelled (cm) and residence time in inner vs. outer areas (see Figs. 2 and 4 legends for treatment and number of larvae studied).

Seventy-two-hour swimming activity setup in F1 larvae

This method was used to monitor the onset of the swimming activity as well as the expression of the circadian activity rhythm in larvae (Vignet et al. 2013). The same microplate preparation was done as above, except that single larvae were transferred at 3 dpf. The following day (4 dpf), 2 h before the challenge, the well plate was transferred to the video acquisition room under the same conditions as described above. It is to note that larvae from different spawns were used for photomotor response (PMR) and this test. The recording of the larval swimming activity started at 12:00 and continued for 72 h. The dependent variable measured was the distance travelled (cm) (see legend of Fig. 3 for treatment and number of larvae studied).

Novel tank test

In addition to providing information on the exploratory ability of the fish, the swimming characteristics recorded during exploration are giving indications as to how fish cope with this novel environment. Indeed, the use of the novel tank test has been validated to evaluate anxiety level by monitoring vertical position (Egan et al. 2009; Levin et al. 2007). Here, the novel tank challenge was performed in 2-month-old offspring issued from PY parents. After 2 h of acclimatization in the room in 1 L aquarium, fish were transferred to a novel tank (trapezoid 1.5 L tank; Aquatic Habitats, Apopka, FL; sizes in cm: height 15.2 × width 7.1 × length 27.9 at top and 22.5 at bottom) and filmed for 6 min in side view. Twelve fish were challenged per concentration. For space occupancy analysis, tanks were separated into two areas according to Egan et al. (2009): the top area including one half of the volume and the bottom area including the other half; the dependent variable measured was the time spent in each area (top, bottom; s) per minute, and for the all 6-min test duration, the total distance travelled (cm) and the total time spent immobile (s).

Behavioral data recording and analysis

Videos for the PMR, the 72-h swimming activity setup and the novel tank test were recorded with an analogue camera ICD-48E (Ikegami) and 2.7–13.5 mm lens (Fujinon) linked to a PC with an acquisition card and Ethovision XT 8.5 software (Noldus, The Netherlands).

For all experiments, EthoVision XT software was used for track extraction and analysis. Data were acquired by EthoVision at 25 frames per second, and variables were nested for further treatments every 30 s in larvae during the PMR, every 30 min for the 72-h swimming activity setup, and every 1 or 6 min in novel tank experiments with 2-month-old juveniles. For assessments of the time spent immobile, we used the mobility detection function of EthoVision XT and we set the lower threshold for separating immobility from mobility at 20 % (i.e., indicating that a change of no more than 20 % in the pixels of a detected object between two consecutive samples would be considered to indicate immobility; see EthoVision XT Reference Manual).

Aromatase expression in vivo

F1 embryos exposed to the ethinyl estradiol (EE2) were issued from cyp19a1b:GFP transgenic F0 (Tong et al. 2009) exposed through diet to the three same PAH mixtures. At 4–5 h post-fertilization, 30 fertilized eggs were placed in beakers with 30 ml of E3, and EE2 was added to final concentrations of 0.005, 0.017, 0.05, and 0.17 nM. Between 1 and 5 replicates were done depending on fractions and concentrations due to egg availability (see supp. Table 1 for details). The beakers were incubated at 28 °C with the same photoperiod as adults. On day 6 after fertilization, larvae were anesthetized with benzocaine (final concentration 50 mg L−1) and were placed in methylcellulose at 3 % under a fluorescence microscope (Brion et al. 2012). All the larvae were observed in dorsal view, and each was photographed using an Olympus BX41 microscope and a fluorescence source (XCite series 120 Q, SCOP PRO, Itteville, France) equipped with a DMK 31 AU03 monochrome camera and IC-Capture software (The Imaging Sources, Germany). All photographs were taken using the same parameters: Only the head was photographed using X10 objective, with a 774-ms exposure time and maximal intensity (gain at 500). Fluorescence quantification was performed using the ImageJ software. For each picture taken (61 to 384 larvae depending on fractions and concentrations; see supplementary table 1), the integrated density was measured, i.e., the sum of the gray values of all the pixels within the region of interest defined as in the study of Brion et al. (2012).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed with Statistica 9.0 (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK, USA) software. GLM was applied in each case. In all tests, within each fraction (PY, HO, or LO), concentration (control, 0.3X, 1X ,and 3X) and periods (light/day vs. dark/night in larvae PMR and 72-h swimming activity setup tests; light on-1, light off, and light on-2 during the PMR test; min-1 to 6 in novel tank test) or areas (in novel tank test) were tested as fixed factor and fish and session as random factor. Interaction between concentration and period was also analyzed. Total distance travelled and time spent immobile in novel tank test were compared with only concentrations as fixed factor and fish and session as random factor. Fluorescence quantified by the integrated density was also analyzed using GLM. Within each fraction, integrated density was compared with two fixed factors (PAHs concentration (as above) and EE2 concentrations (0.005, 0.017, 0.05, and 0.17 nM) and fish and calibration range (only for PY) as random factors. All statistical analyses were carried out at a 95 % level of significance, and only the fixed factor and the interaction between them are presented in the text. Post hoc test were performed with Newman-Keuls in each case. The results reported in text and all figures are means ± SEM.

Results

Results obtained for the F0 exposure have been described in previous articles (Vignet et al. 2014b, c) and will not be detailed here. However, some information is important for the understanding of this article and will be briefly recalled here. PAH mixtures were different in terms of individual PAHs and alkylated PAHs proportions, but total PAH concentrations were similar in the 4.7–6.7 μg g−1 dw range for the intermediate concentration (1X) and PAH metabolites quantification revealed an effective exposure of F0. Several physiological effects were observed including the alteration of F0 growth (Vignet et al. 2014c) and behavioral disruptions (Vignet et al. 2014b) and depending on the mixture used, with an increasing severity as follow: PY < LO < HO. Another effect was a disruption of reproduction (Vignet 2014). This was particularly severe for PY-3X, and 1X and 3X concentrations for LO and HO mixtures result in very few or no eggs produced. A direct consequence of these disruptions is that results reported here mainly deal with PY exposure (all concentrations), LO-0.3X and HO-0.3X. The second consequence is that only F1-PY fish were bred to adulthood and only F1-PY offspring (F2-PY) were produced.

Photomotor response in F1 5 dpf larvae

The PMR of control larvae followed the expected pattern which was a clear increase (~60 %) of distance travelled during the light off period compared to the previous period (light on-1). Activity during the following light on period (light on-2) then decreased to reach levels observed during light on-1 period (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Photomotor response of 5 dpf offspring larvae. Distance travelled per 5-min periods before, during, and after a light off period for larvae issued from PY (a in details, b averaged per 5-min periods), larvae issued from HO (c in details, d averaged per 5-min periods), and larvae issued from LO (e in details, f averaged per 5-min periods). Values are mean ± SEM; n = 82 control (from N = 8 spawns), n = 51 0.3X (N = 5), n = 68 1X (N = 5), and n = 60 3X (N = 1) for larvae issued from PY fish; n = 121 (N = 3) control and n = 97 (N = 3) 0.3X for larvae issued from HO fish; n = 61 (N = 3) Control and n = 61 (N = 3) 0.3X for larvae issued from LO fraction. Letters indicate significant difference at p < 0.05 between concentrations within 5-min periods

In the case of the PY mixture (Fig. 1a, b), F1 larvae distance travelled was significantly modified. This was revealed by significant period (F = 37.02, p < 0.001) and concentration (F = 5.05, p = 0.002) effects, but no significant interaction was observed. Indeed, whereas larvae issued from PY-0.3X and PY-1X parents displayed the same PMR as larvae issued from control parents, larvae issued from PY-3X parents had a higher level of activity whatever the light period (p < 0.001) and showed no significant activity increase during light off when compared with light on-1 period (p = 0.279).

For larvae issued from HO parents, there was a significant difference between periods for distance travelled (F = 73.97, p < 0.001; Fig. 1c) and a significant difference was also observed between concentrations (F = 40.56, p < 0.001) but no significant interaction was measured. Activity of larvae issued from HO-0.3X parents displayed the usual pattern; however, it was significantly reduced during both light on periods compared to larvae issued from control parents (p < 0.001) as well as during the light off period (p < 0.001; Fig. 1d).

In the case of larvae issued from LO parents (Fig. 1e, f), a difference between periods was observed for distance travelled (F = 20.79, p < 0.001) but no significant difference was observed between concentrations (F = 1.72, p = 0.19; Fig. 1f) and no interaction was measured.

In addition to locomotion analyzed in whole wells, the time spent in inner and outer areas of each well, as well as the activity within each area was monitored. Whatever the mixtures and the concentrations, no difference between inner vs. outer area occupancy or activity was observed (data not shown).

Seventy-two-hour swimming activity setup in F1 larvae

The spontaneous activity of larvae was monitored during 72 h starting from 4 dpf and included day and night periods using different larvae from different spawns than the ones used for PMR assessment. Because of the very low spawning of HO, this was performed only for F1-PY and F1-LO. During day periods, distance travelled by larvae issued from control parents increased progressively during day 4 to reach a maximum during day 5 and then decreased to a low level during day 6. Conversely, during night periods, activity was strongly reduced.

Circadian spontaneous swimming activity of larvae issued from PY parents showed the same general pattern whatever the concentration to which parents were exposed (i.e., no concentration effect was shown, F = 0.73, p = 0.53; Fig. 2a), but significant differences between periods (F = 365.11, p < 0.001) and significant interaction between period and concentration were noteworthy (F = 3.89, p = 0.011) with, in particular, the decrease in the activity of larvae issued from PY-0.3X during all day periods. This resulted in a significant decrease of total distance travelled during diurnal periods (p = 0.003; Fig. 2b). On the contrary, activity of larvae issued from PY-3X parents increased especially during day periods 5 to 7 dpf (Fig. 2a). This resulted in a significant increase of total distance travelled during diurnal periods (p < 0.001; Fig. 2b). The diurnal activity of PY-1X offspring was intermediate between PY-0.3X and PY-3X and at the level of control offspring. No difference was observed between concentrations for nocturnal periods (Fig. 2b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Seventy-two-hour swimming activity setup in F1 larvae. Evolution of distance travelled (cm/30 min) for PY larvae in relation to time. Values are means without SEM for graph readability (a). Averaged distance travelled for day and night periods. Values are mean ± SEM for larvae issued from PY (b) and for larvae issued from LO (c); n = 27 (N = 4 spawns) control, 21 (N = 4) 0.3X, 20 (N = 4) 1X, and 15 (N = 1) 3X for larvae issued from PY; n = 23 (N = 2) control, n = 24 (N = 1) 0.3X, and n = 23 (N = 2) 1X for larvae issued from LO. Letters indicate significant difference at p < 0.05 between concentrations within day periods

Circadian spontaneous swimming activity of larvae issued from LO parents presented the above described pattern obtained for larvae issued from control parents. Whatever the concentration, a clear higher diurnal activity was observed with a significant difference between periods (F = 102.52, p < 0.001; Fig. 2c), between concentration (F = 21.00, p < 0.001; Fig. 2c), and a significant interaction between concentrations and periods (F = 4.21, p = 0.017). Diurnal swimming activity of larvae issued from LO-0.3X (p < 0.001) and LO-1X (p < 0.001) parents was significantly reduced compared to offspring of control parents.

Novel tank test—2-month-old F1-PY

At the beginning of the test, juveniles spent most of the time in the bottom area, and the time spent in top area increased over test duration (F = 12.89, p < 0.001; Fig. 3a) and was different between concentrations (F = 5.39, p = 0.001) with no significant interaction. Post hoc tests revealed that larvae issued from PY-3X parents spent more time in top area compared to larvae issued from control parents (p = 0.008). Total time spent in top zone (during the whole test duration) was, however, not different between concentrations (F = 1.32, p = 0.28; Fig. 3b). Distance travelled and time spent immobile during the whole test duration did not significantly differ between concentrations (F = 0.21, p = 0.88, F = 0.83, p = 0.48 respectively; Fig. 3c, d). Analysis of juveniles morphology revealed no differences according to parents exposure for body length (1.97 ± 0.04 cm, F = 0.39; p = 0.76) or body mass (128.7 ± 9.3 mg, F = 0.29; p = 0.84).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Novel tank challenge in 2-month-old juveniles issued from PY fish. Mean time spent (s) per minute (min-1 to min-6) in the top area of the novel tank (a) is different between concentrations (F = 5.39; p = 0.001) with 3X being different from control (p = 0.008). Total distance travelled (cm) over the 6-min challenge (b) and total time spent immobile (s) over the 6-min challenge (c). Values are mean ± SEM; n = 74 control, n = 55 0.3X, n = 84 1X, and n = 75 3X for larvae issued from PY fish (N = 2 spawns)

Photomotor response in F2 5 dpf larvae

The PMR of F2 larvae issued from control parents followed the same pattern as described for F1 larvae (Fig. 4a, b) with a significant difference between periods (F = 61.32, p < 0.001). A significant difference was also observed between concentrations to which F0 was exposed (F = 5.79, p < 0.001), but no significant interaction was measured. Distance travelled by F2-PY-1X larvae was lower than that of F2-control larvae during both light on periods (p < 0.001 for L1 and p = 0.047 for L2; Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Photomotor response of F2-PY 5 dpf larvae issued from F1-PY. Distance travelled per 5-min periods before, during, and after a light off period (a in details, b averaged per 5-min periods). Values are mean ± SEM; n = 82 (N = 4 spawns) control, n = 51 (N = 3) 0.3X, n = 68 (N = 3) 1X, and n = 60 (N = 3) 3X for larvae issued from F1-PY fish. Letters indicate significant difference at p < 0.05 between concentrations within 5-min periods

Endocrine disruption

To evaluate the possible endocrine disruption in larvae brain which may be explicative of F1 larval phenotypes, a transgenic line was used expressing GFP under the control of brain aromatase (cyp19a1b) promoter (Tong et al. 2009). F0 transgenic fish were exposed as described before for TU line and offspring monitored for GFP expression. Basal activity (in the absence of EE2 stimulation) was similar between all tested offspring compared to their respective control (inserts in Fig. 5). Incubation of embryos and larvae in EE2 increased GFP expression in an EE2 dose-dependent manner for all diets (PY: F = 175.27, p < 0.001; HO: F = 8.87, p < 0.001; and LO: F = 23.09; p < 0.001), but no significant effect of PAHs concentration was measured and no significant interaction either. Post hoc tests revealed a significant reduction of GFP induction by EE2 for PY offspring (concentration 0.3X (p = 0.004), 1X (p = 0.011), and 3X (p = 0.023)) for an EE2 concentration of 0.05 nM. This inhibition was 43 % for PY-0.3X offspring and 33 % for PY-1X and PY-3X offspring. At the highest concentration used for EE2 (0.17 nM), an inhibition was only observed for PY-0.3X offspring (−37 %; Fig. 5a). A strong reduction of GFP expression was observed in the brain of HO-0.3X larvae (−74 %) for an EE2 concentration of 0.05 nM, but because of a high variation in control larvae, this was not significant (Fig. 5b). In the case of LO offspring, none of the LO-0.3X offspring survived to an EE2 exposure at a concentration above 0.017 nM. At lower concentrations, no difference was observed between control and LO-0.3X offspring (Fig. 5c).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Brain aromatase expression in number of fluorescent pixels extracted from images of the head region of larvae issued from tg cyp19a1b exposed through diets to PY fish (a), HO (b), and LO (c) fractions in relation to EE2 concentration (nM). Letters indicate significant difference at p < 0.05 between concentrations to which parents were exposed within each EE2 concentrations. Detailed number of picture taken per fraction and concentration is given in supplementary table 1

Discussion

The three diets used for parents (F0) exposure had similar overall PAHs concentration but differed significantly in their composition, according to fractions used for spiking. PY was characterized by a high level of heavy PAHs and a very low level of alkylated PAHs. LO contained a high level of alkylated PAHs and a low level of heavy PAHs while HO was intermediate for both groups of PAHs (Vignet et al. 2014c). Exposures caused, in F0, growth disruption (Vignet et al. 2014c) and altered behavioral performances (Vignet et al. 2014b). However, phenotype severity differed with respect to the mixture with an increasing toxicity as follow: PY < LO < HO. This ranking was also observed when comparing survival and tumorigenesis (Larcher et al. 2014). In this study, we evaluated the behavior of offspring of fish exposed to those three PAH mixtures. However, as reproduction was also significantly disrupted, very few offspring (F1) were obtained from PY-3X as well as LO- and HO-exposed fish (Vignet 2014). As a consequence, only larval behavior was evaluated for F1-LO and F1-HO for available concentrations. Further, because analysis in F0 suggested an increase in fish anxiety level (Vignet et al. 2014b), the study of F1 behavior focused on this particular angle, in particular for F1-PY at larval and juvenile stages.

F1 larvae morphology was assessed at 96 hpf, and neither morphological defects nor body length differences were observed whatever the diet parents were exposed to (Lucas et al. 2014; Perrichon 2014). The only exception was for PY-0.3X F1 which presented a very weak (+1.4 %) increase in body length (Lucas et al. 2014). Larval heart rate was also monitored revealing no differences for F1 from LO fish (Perrichon 2014). In the case of F1 from PY fish, results are contradictory since some authors indicated a tachycardia for PY-1X F1 (Lucas et al. 2014) while a bradycardia is reported for PY-0.3X, PY-1X, and PY-3X (Perrichon 2014). These contradictions are puzzling and do not allow to conclude for a functional effect. Further, monitoring of PY-F1 aerobic metabolism revealed no differences with control F1 (Lucas et al. 2014). No such data is available for HO or LO F1. Taken altogether, these results suggest that neither morphological nor metabolic disruption could be responsible for the behavioral phenotypes alteration observed in F1.

In F1, two assays were used to evaluate anxiety and/or activity level in 5 dpf larvae and to evaluate the setup of locomotor activity from 4 to 7 dpf. In the case of PY, an elevated activity was observed in the PMR challenge only for F1-PY-3X and during all three phases of the challenge while other F1-PY had the same activity level as F1-control. An increase in the dark phase during PMR may be indicative of a higher anxiety level (Steenbergen et al. 2011). However, this should also be accompanied by an avoidance of the center area of wells (Schnorr et al. 2012; Steenbergen et al. 2011) which was not observed here. In addition, the locomotor activity of F1-PY-3X larvae was increased irrespective of the period (light or dark). Altogether, this suggests that hyperactivity should be a favored hypothesis to explain behavioral responses of F1-PY-3X. This is supported by the analysis of locomotory activity setup which revealed an overall increase of F1-PY-3X activity during diurnal periods compared to F1-control. This is particularly obvious during day 5 which corresponded to the day of highest larval activity (Vignet et al. 2013). In the case of PY-0.3X and 1X offspring, no modification of PMR was observed. Locomotory activity setup was differentially modified according to PY concentration: F1-PY-0.3X had a lower diurnal activity compared to control larvae especially during days 5 and 6, and F1-PY-1X had an intermediate diurnal activity between 0.3X and 3X larvae, therefore not differing from control larvae.

As said earlier, in the case of offspring of petrogenic exposed F0, only partial results were obtained. For HO mixture, only F1-HO-0.3X were obtained and tested only for their PMR. These larvae displayed an overall reduction of their activity, suggesting lethargic effects inducing general hypoactivity. PMR of F1-LO-0.3X did not differ from the one of control larvae, but when assessed during 72 h, locomotory activity in F1-LO-0.3X and F1-LO-1X displayed an overall significant decrease in their diurnal activity. Since it was not possible to perform all assays with offspring from all concentrations, it is difficult to go further in the discussion on the behavior of HO and LO offspring apart from noticing an either overall (HO) or a diurnal only (LO) lethargic effect.

Such differences in severity and/or nature of disruptions according to the mixtures have also been shown in zebrafish F0 after exposure through diet (Larcher et al. 2014; Vignet et al. 2014b, c) as well as in medaka after sediment-contact exposure (Le Bihanic et al. 2014a, b). The authors suggested that these differences likely reflected composition differences between mixtures and the triggering of (at least partially) different mechanisms. It is therefore logical that disruptions identified in F1 were also different.

Because of the low number of HO and LO offspring, only F1-PY were bred to adulthood and, therefore, assessed at later stage. The behavior of F1-PY juveniles was then evaluated at 2 mpf using the novel tank assay. This assay has been described as a good and simple indicator of anxiety level since, at introduction, in this new environment, fish dive to the bottom of the tank and progressively start to explore the upper area (Egan et al. 2009; Levin et al. 2007; Sackerman et al. 2010). The proportion of time spent in the upper area is increased by anxiolytic drugs and decreased by anxiogenic ones (Egan et al. 2009; Levin et al. 2007). Offspring of PY fish displayed the expected pattern with a progressive increase of time spent in the upper area. The only difference observed is an overall higher proportion of time spent in this upper area for F1-PY-3X. Since there was no modification of locomotor activity (distance travelled), this relaxed home-based behavior could be indicative of a lower anxiety level compared to control juveniles. This hypothesis would fit well with the lowering (however, non-significant statistically) of the time spent immobile, as described previously (Cachat et al. 2010). Here, again, no morphological difference was observed between offspring whatever the PY concentrations.

Taken altogether, these results suggest that exposure of parents to PAH mixtures can lead to behavioral disruptions in their offspring at larval stage, and in the case of PY mixture, it appears that after hyperactivity at larval stage, further disruptions can also be observed later in juveniles. Further analyses should be conducted to explore early brain imprinting and evaluate the potential relationship between PAHs and the concentration of neurotransmitters (such as dopamine and serotonin) which are also regulating behavioral repertoire as shown in trout after PAHs injection (Gesto et al. 2008, 2009) or in rats after PCBs perinatal exposure (Boix et al. 2011).

Disruption of fish behavior after exposure to PAHs has already been demonstrated. Indeed, juvenile fish exposed to individual or a simple mixture of PAHs have a decreased locomotor activity compared to control fish (Almeida et al. 2012; Correia et al. 2007; Goncalves et al. 2008; Gravato and Guilhermino 2009; Oliveira et al. 2012). Authors concluded to a lethargic effect of PAHs exposure. More recently, a long-term exposure to three PAH mixtures from the first meal until behavioral assessment (the parents (F0) of fish described in the present article) produced anxiety-like disruptions (Vignet et al. 2014b). Two other studies recently published reported behavioral disruption upon embryo-larval exposure (one in medaka and one in zebrafish). In the first one, medaka embryos have been exposed using sediment spiked with the same three fractions as the ones used in the present article (PY, HO, and LO) and larval behavior was monitored at 4 days post-hatching using a similar PMR procedure (Le Bihanic et al. 2014a) except for LO-1X, LO-3X, HO-1X, and HO-3X larvae which were not tested due to poor or no survival. The classical PMR was observed for all concentrations and fractions; however, larvae presented several behavioral differences compared to the control, such as an increase of the distance travelled during the acclimation period (PY-3X, HO-0.3X, and LO-0.3X) during the following light on period (HO-0.3X and LO-0.3X) as well as an increase in the sum of high mobility periods during dark and light periods (PY-3X) or dark periods only (HO-0.3X and LO-0.3X) (Le Bihanic et al. 2014a). These results are indicative of an induction of activity in larvae after embryonic exposure to PAHs. In this study, no analysis was performed at later stages preventing the identification of long-lasting behavioral disruptions. In the second study, we have demonstrated that early exposure of zebrafish embryo (during the first 4 days) to sediment spiked with a mixture of three individual PAHs produced behavioral disruptions at later stages in juveniles and adults, suggesting lethargic or anxiety-related behavior (Vignet et al. 2014a), and this latter behavior was also identified in their offspring at larval stage.

When comparing perinatal (the above cited studies) and prenatal (the present study) exposures, some differences are revealed. Here, in the case of prenatal exposure with different mixtures, larvae issued from PY-3X fish showed hyperactivity and lower anxiety levels. Such contrasted results cannot be directly compared since mixtures differed, but they echo the findings of Crépeaux et al. (2012, 2013) who highlighted different consequences on adult male rats’ behavior and regional brain metabolism depending on whether exposure to 16 PAH mixture was exclusively prenatal or perinatal.

In the present case of F1 offspring of F0-exposed fish, behavioral disruptions may be due to the exposure of embryos and larvae to PAHs after maternal transfer to the egg since such transfer has been reported in birds (Pereira et al. 2009), fish (Monteverdi and Giulio 2000; Tilghman Hall and Oris 1991) and mollusks (Pelletier et al. 2000). However, no PAH metabolites have been detected in 5 dpf F1 larvae. It is possible however that PAHs or their metabolites were present earlier and may have caused precocious disruption. However, it is noteworthy that Cyp1a enzymatic activity is debated in early zebrafish larvae since, while cyp1a expression is quickly activated by AhR agonists in larvae, Cyp1a activity seems to be not induced before 3 dph (Mattingly and Toscano 2001). Whatever, the absence of metabolites in 5 dpf larvae suggests a very low level of PAHs; if any transferred to the eggs, therefore, behavioral disruptions cannot be attributed to a direct effect of PAHs. Alternatively, behavioral disruptions can also be due to several other non-exclusive events such as i) disruption of egg content in signaling molecules or other component in relation with endocrine disruption (ED) or ii) epigenetic imprinting of gametes which can deregulate embryonic development and/or later fish physiology. Further, some disruptions were also observed in F2-PY. This suggests that behavioral disruption can be transmitted to further generation. It is noteworthy that disruptions are different from those observed in F1-PY, suggesting the involvement of different mechanisms. At this stage, it is not possible to say this is an epigenetically driven disruption since primordial germ cells involved in F2 production were present in F1 at embryonic stages and may have been directly exposed to the disrupting signal (Anway and Skinner 2006).

Because some POPs act as endocrine disruptors and early endocrine disruption has been proposed to mediate some behavioral defects in fish, birds, and mammals (Ottinger et al. 2009, 2013; Panzica et al. 2007; Reyhanian et al. 2011; Stocco 2012; Volkova et al. 2012), we evaluated potential early ED in F1 larvae. This was performed using in vivo assessment of brain aromatase (cyp19a1b) promoter activation upon EE2 exposure (Brion et al. 2012). In F1-PY larvae, a significant reduction of cyp19a1b activation by EE2 was observed whatever the PAH’s concentration F0 was exposed to. In the case of F1-HO larvae, no modification of cyp19a1b inducibility was observed. Finally, exposures of F1-LO to EE2 lead to larval death for concentrations above 0.017 nM. These results suggest an endocrine disruption in F1-PY and F1-LO. They also suggest different disruptions between these two treatments. In the case of F1-PY, results suggest an inhibition of the response to EE2 exposure which may be reminiscent of what has been observed in waterborne exposure to TCDD alone or in combination with EE2 (Brion et al. 2012). In this experiment, TCDD alone did not modify GFP expression monitored using the same transgenic line. However, an exposure combining TCDD and EE2 led to a decreased induction of GFP when compared to EE2 alone. Since TCDD and heavy weight PAHs share common pathways such as aryl hydrocarbon receptor (AhR) (Billiard et al. 2002), it is possible to envision a similar mechanism of action in F1-PY larvae. In the case of F1-LO, it is likely that another mechanism is involved. In this respect, it is noteworthy that early exposure to ED compound can promote sensitization, and therefore, such exacerbated response to later exposure has been demonstrated, albeit on a different time scale with roach exposure to EE2 (Lange et al. 2009), so it is possible that F1-LO larvae were sensitized to estrogen and that an additional exposure to EE2 led to larval death. The fact that differential EE2 responses were observed between F1-PY and F1-LO offspring is in agreement with the differential behavioral disruptions observed in larvae. This does not rule out other mechanism(s) such as differences in DNA methylation profile which can result in genes expression differences and, ultimately, if transmitted to the next generation in epigenetic effects. Such transgenerational effects have been recently reported in zebrafish after pulse exposure of juveniles to an AhR inducer, dioxin (Baker et al. 2014). Recently, changes in DNA methylation and in genes expression have been reported in F0 zebrafish after exposure to two PAHs, benzo(a)pyrene and 7,12-dimethylbenz(a)anthracene (Corrales et al. 2014; Fang et al. 2013; Mirbahai et al. 2011). It is therefore possible that such mechanisms are involved in F1 and F2 phenotypes described here.

Conclusion

This work illustrates the fact that zebrafish is amenable to sophisticated analyses, here effects on F1 generation thanks to its short lifecycle, to evaluate aquatic toxicology. This work also demonstrates that physiological effects can be observed in offspring of fish exposed to PAHs which themselves were not directly exposed to this pollutant. We and the others have already reported this in offspring of fish exposed to PCBs (Pean et al. 2013) and BDE-209 (He et al. 2011) but, contrary to PCBs, PAHs, and PBDE to a lesser extent, are readily metabolized in fish. In this respect, PAH metabolites were not detected in F1. It is thus possible that zebrafish, exposed to environmental PAH mixtures through diet, could also serve as a model to improve the understanding of the mechanisms underlying the appearance of behavioral disorders through generations. Finally, from an ecological point of view, this indicates that detrimental behavioral disruptions (lower exploratory abilities and increase of predation risks) may possibly occur in fish offspring in places devoid of intrinsic pollution as a consequence of parent’s exposure elsewhere in their dynamic habitat. Two more elements point to the potential detrimental effect of PAHs exposure: First, disruptions were also observed in F2 generation for PY mixture, and second, concentrations used were in the range of those measured in the environment.