Introduction

Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has risen immensely in the last 25 years, in large part due to increases in public awareness of parents and healthcare providers (Fountain et al. 2011). The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) reported a 78 % increase in ASD diagnosis between the years of 2002 and 2008. Current estimates indicate that one in 68 children, and one in every 42 boys will be diagnosed with an ASD (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2014).

ASDs are increasingly costly to both families and society as a whole. The lifetime cost of supporting a person with ASD has been estimated at 2.4 million dollars for patients with intellectual disability, and 1.4 million dollars for those without (Buescher et al. 2014). Societal costs are correspondingly high. Treatment of ASD costs the United States 175 billion dollars per year—more than cancer, stroke, and heart disease combined (Buescher et al. 2014).

Early intervention is particularly important for reducing intellectual, behavioral, and functional impairments (Fountain et al. 2011; Rogers and Vismara 2008). Children who benefit from high-quality, early behavioral intervention have significantly better outcomes than those treated later. They demonstrate better developmental functioning, fewer maladaptive behaviors, lower symptom severity, improved language and socio-emotional skills, reduced intellectual impairment, and higher IQ and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Composite scores both in the short and long term (Eldevik et al. 2009; Rogers and Vismara 2008; Vismara and Rogers 2010). Correspondingly, improvements in IQ in childhood predict better prognosis in peer relationships, education, vocational training, employment, and independent living (Billstedt et al. 2005). As a result, not only is early and accurate diagnosis critical to effective interventions, it can directly minimize functional impairment and, subsequently, associated costs to society.

Given the rise in prevalence and visibility of autism, age of initial diagnosis has generally decreased (Hertz-Picciotto and Delwiche 2009). Diagnosis of ASD has historically been influenced by certain demographic variables. Children of privileged and/or Caucasian families have been overrepresented, and families of lower socioeconomic status (SES) and of minority racial and ethnic backgrounds have traditionally been underdiagnosed (Durkin et al. 2010; Fountain et al. 2011). Although community awareness of ASD has begun to reduce these racial and socioeconomic disparities, group differences remain (CDC 2014; Gourdine and Algood 2014; Pedersen et al. 2012; Zuckerman et al. 2013).

To better understand racial disparities in ASD diagnosis, it is important to evaluate demographic barriers involved in access to healthcare. In the United States, SES proves to be the most powerful determinant of primary health care use, with race being a close and related second (Fiscella et al. 2014). Persons of low SES are at risk for lower quality ambulatory and emergency care (Fiscella et al. 2014), worse overall health (Adler and Rehkopf 2008), higher morbidity and mortality (Adler and Stewart 2010), and they are less likely to have a regular source of care (DeVoe et al. 2007, 2008). Racial health disparities are equally pervasive and persistent in adult (Fiscella et al. 2014) and pediatric care (Flores 2010). Minority children are less likely to have had a recent physician visit (Flores and Lin 2013) or a medical home, which includes having a consistent source of care (CSC; Raphael et al. 2009; Strickland et al. 2009). Similar racial disparities are also observed in mental health care where minority children receive less outpatient mental health care and psychotropic medication than Caucasian children (Cook et al. 2013). These children are also less likely to use mental health services (Coker et al. 2009) or to be referred to specialty care, even after controlling for health insurance status (Kenney et al. 2013). Finally, independent of SES and race, health insurance is also an important determinant of health care use. Uninsured children are less likely to have a usual source of care or to have seen a physician in the recent past, and more likely to have unmet healthcare needs in terms of both treatment and preventative care (Cummings et al. 2009; DeVoe et al. 2009).

Although having health insurance is strongly associated with access to primary care (Cummings et al. 2009), healthcare access does not equate to healthcare utilization (Starfield and Shi 2004). Families that are insured do not necessarily use the services at their disposal due to barriers such as employment, language, education, and location (Chan et al. 2006; Devoe et al. 2007; Zuvekas and Taliaferro 2003). For instance, insured low SES individuals use fewer healthcare services than their higher SES counterparts (Fiscella et al. 2002). Likewise, insured Hispanic and African American children are less likely to receive specialty and mental health care than insured Caucasian children (Kenney et al. 2013). Taking into account confounds between demographic variables and insurance, having a consistent source of medical care may be an equally important, if not better, determinant of health care utilization as health care insurance (Blackwell et al. 2009; DeVoe et al. 2008; Gill et al. 2000; Lillie-Blanton and Hoffman 2005).

In ASD specifically, having a consistent source of medical care may improve the likelihood that pediatricians, who see children regularly for scheduled preventive care visits during their first 3 years of life, notice developmental abnormalities (Pinto-Martin et al. 2008; Sices 2007). Since 2006, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) has implemented guidelines to more accurately screen for developmental abnormalities. Pediatricians are instructed to perform a routine developmental screening at nine, 18, and 24 months, and to conduct an additional autism-specific screening at 18 and 24 months (Bright Futures Steering Committee and Medical Home Initiatives for Children with Special Needs Project Advisory Committee 2006; Johnson and Myers 2007).

Referral to specialty care is subsequently recommended for children who use no spontaneous two-word phrases by 2 years of age or who display language or social regressions (Barbaresi et al. 2006). Although similar guidelines were recommended earlier by the American Academy of Neurology (Filipek et al. 2000), surveillance was the AAP recommendation until 2006. The 2006 AAP guidelines were developed to ensure widespread adoption by outlining implementation strategies for various primary care settings (King et al. 2010).

Even with regular primary care and an appropriate specialty care referral, however, the road to diagnosis is often an arduous one. Reports of diagnostic experiences by parents highlight the difficulty in obtaining a definite diagnosis (Goin-Kochel et al. 2006; Keenan et al. 2010). Both symptom heterogeneity and physician uncertainity contribute to diagnostic delays (Goin-Kochel et al. 2006). Although half of parents recall concerns dating back to the first years of life (Zwaigenbaum et al. 2013), there may be a gap of several years between first noticing developmental delays and the confirmation of a diagnosis (Sansosti et al. 2012; Shattuck et al. 2009).

Both physician and parent factors influence diagnosis. Lack of universal screening by primary care practitioners (PCP) may contribute to diagnostic delays. A study by Dosreis et al. (2006) revealed that, while 44 % of the sampled doctors served ASD patients, only 8 % regularly screened for the disorder. Meanwhile, relying on parents to elicit concerns also has consequences. In a controlled trial of ASD screening in primary care, 75 % of parents with children who screened positive for an ASD had not suspected it (Pinto-Martin et al. 2008). Despite these hindrances, consultation with PCPs still remains the crucial first step in the diagnostic process. Most parents express their initial concerns to their child’s pediatrician and rely on the advice they receive to determine follow-up (Barbaresi et al. 2006). Given the importance of primary care in identifying symptoms and referring to specialists, the goal of the current study was to determine whether having a CSC predicted timeliness of ASD diagnosis.

The study tested the following hypotheses. We anticipated that families without a CSC would have children diagnosed at a later age. Given documented disparities in medical practice for ASD, we also expected that children of color would be diagnosed later than their Caucasian peers. With the implication that more severe symptoms would be recognized earlier, we expected autism severity to be negatively related to age of diagnosis. Finally, we posited that the impact of having a CSC on age of diagnosis would depend on both race and autism severity. Given that children with mild forms of ASD tend to wait considerably longer for a diagnosis (Fountain et al. 2011), we presumed that having a CSC would facilitate diagnosis of more severe forms of ASD but not of mild ASD. We also expected the effect of a CSC to be moderated by racial disparities such that being from an ethnic or racial minority would minimize the benefits of a CSC on timeliness of ASD diagnosis.

Since other demographic variables may impact age of ASD diagnosis (Herlihy et al. 2014), we controlled for poverty level, parental education, current health insurance status, and whether children were born before or after the implementation of the 2006 AAP screening guidelines (hereafter referred to as birth cohort). We also included birth order under the assumption that first-time parents would likely recognize ASD later due to less familiarity with typical child development (Fountain et al. 2011).

Methods

Participants

Data were obtained from the CDC National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH) 2011. The sample consisted of parents or guardians of children between the ages of 0 and 17 (M = 8.85, SD = 5.237). The adult in the household most knowledgeable about the child’s health was asked to complete the phone survey. In total, 96,677 child-level interviews were conducted. Of the respondents, 69.5 % were mothers, 21.8 % were fathers, and 8.3 % were other family members or guardians. Of children old enough to receive an ASD diagnosis, 2-years old or older, 1.7 % of children in the sample had an ASD (N = 1624). Twenty-one percent of parents reported receiving the diagnosis from their child’s PCP; the rest received the diagnosis from a variety of specialists. Table 1 contains additional demographic information about the sample of children with ASD. Table 2 includes information about the survey respondents: parents and guardians.

Table 1 Characteristics of ASD participants (N = 1624)
Table 2 Characteristics of respondents (N = 1624)

Procedures

The national cross-sectional survey was conducted via random-digit-dialed telephones, both landline and mobile, between February 2011 and June 2012, using the sampling frame of the National Immunization Survey (CDC 2013). The overall response rate to the survey was 23 %; the completion rate was 54.1 % for landline and 41.2 % for mobile phone interviews (Visser et al. 2014). The interview was based on previous surveys conducted by the CDC in 2003 and 2007 using similar questions and methods (Blumberg et al. 2012).

Measures

Publicly available data files and methodology reports from the 2011–2012 National Survey of Children’s Health were used to test research questions. The purpose of the NSCH was to collect information on children’s health, including physical and mental health status, access to health care, information on the child’s family, neighborhood, and social context (CDC 2013). Collected demographic information included child sex, race and ethnicity, parental education, primary language spoken in the home, and poverty level.

As part of the survey, the responding parent was asked whether the selected child had any of the following diagnoses: Attention Deficit Disorder or Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder, depression, anxiety problems, behavior or conduct problems, autism, Asperger’s Disorder, pervasive development disorder, or other ASD, any developmental delay, intellectual disability or mental retardation, or Tourette Syndrome.

The current study focused on the answers of parents who answered affirmatively to: “Does [CHILD’S NAME] currently have autism or autism spectrum disorder?” Subsequently, parents were asked, “How old was [CHILD’S NAME] when you were first told by a doctor or other health care provider that (he/she) had [CONDITION]?” and “Would you describe (his/her) condition as mild, moderate, or severe?” The other question of interest was: “Is there a place that [CHILD’S NAME] USUALLY goes when [he/she] is sick or you need advice about [his/her] health?” hereafter referred to as CSC.

Statistical Analysis

Prior to running the multiple linear regression, we checked for and confirmed that the assumptions of linearity, normality, and multicollinearity of multiple linear regression had not been violated. A hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis was used to examine the relative contributions of birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, parental education, insurance status, race, ASD severity, and CSC on age of initial ASD diagnosis. We also examined all two- and three-way interactions among race, ASD severity, and CSC on age of diagnosis.

To calculate the effect of the 2006 AAP guidelines on age of diagnosis, we split the data set into two birth cohorts, pre- and post-2006. We first calculated year of birth based on the child’s age at time of interview. Since survey date was not available in the current dataset, a 2-year birth range was estimated based on the data collection date range: 2011–2012. The variable was then dummy coded into pre- and post-2006 birth years, using pre-2006 as the reference group.

Birth order was originally coded categorically as only child, first born, second born, third born, and fourth born. For the purpose of this study, this variable was dichotomized and dummy coded into first born/only child, and younger sibling. To examine the effect of health insurance, we constructed contrast codes to compare insured to uninsured children, and publicly insured to privately insured children.

Household income was collected during the survey but not available in the public dataset. Instead, income was translated into percentage of the Federal Poverty Level (FPL). The continuous variable was originally coded on a four-point Likert scale from 1 representing “0–99 % FPL” to 4 representing “400 % FPL or greater.” In the current study, this item was reverse coded and centered on the mean to facilitate interpretation. Parental education was measured using three categories: “less than high school,” “high school,” and “more than high school.” The highest level of education of either parent or guardian was chosen to estimate the effect of parental education. The variable was subsequently dummy coded, using the most common educational level as the reference group: more than high school.

Both race and ASD severity were dummy coded, using Caucasian and mild ASD as reference groups, respectively. Information about child race was derived from a variable that coded race as follows: Hispanic, White non-Hispanic, Black non-Hispanic (referred to as African American in text), and multiracial/other non-Hispanic. The multiracial/other category consisted of children who reported more than one race (multiracial) or only one race category of Asian, American Indian, Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian, or Pacific Islander (other) (CDC 2013).

Consistent source of care was categorically coded 0 for having no CSC, and 1 for having a CSC. The outcome variable, age of initial ASD diagnosis, was originally collected in both months and years. To facilitate analysis, cases measured in years were recalculated in terms of months. Cases that listed age of diagnosis as 0 months were coded as missing. Given that most diagnoses of ASD do not occur before 1 year of age (Matson et al. 2008), cases that listed age of diagnosis as occurring before 1 year old were recoded as 12 months.

Results

The optimal linear combination of birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, parental education, insurance status, race, ASD severity, and CSC, and the interaction of the latter three variables accounted for 16 % of the variance in age of initial diagnosis, adjusted R 2 = 0.16, F(24, 1533) = 13.38, p < .001. Birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, race, and ASD severity all independently predicted age of diagnosis. Average age of diagnosis was 64.65 months (SD = 41.03). While birth cohort predicted age of ASD diagnosis, children born in or after 2006 were significantly younger (M = 4.15, SD = 0.936) than children born before 2006 (M = 11.52, SD = 3.29), t(1622) = 33.83, p < .001.

The direction of the race predictor was not in the expected direction. While we assumed that Caucasian children would be diagnosed earliest, given racial disparities in diagnosis, being of Hispanic descent significantly predicted earlier diagnosis (b = −25.83, 95 % CI [−50.62, −1.04], p < .05). On average, ethnic minority children (Hispanic M = 57.06, SD = 37.00; African American M = 50.89, SD = 35.76; multiracial/other M = 58.05, SD = 38.78) were diagnosed earlier than Caucasian children in the sample (M = 68.24, SD = 41.95).

We also found that condition severity predicted diagnosis age, with moderate ASD being diagnosed earlier than mild and severe forms of autism (Moderate ASD: b = −24.94, 95 % CI [−44.52, −5.37], p < .05). In terms of control variables, being a younger sibling significantly predicted earlier diagnosis (b = −11.44, 95 % CI [−15.72, −7.17], p < .001). Neither parental education, health insurance status nor CSC independently predicted age of diagnosis, p > .05.

Main effects were qualified by three-way interactions, such that the effect of one predictor on age of initial diagnosis was dependent on the levels of the two other predictors in the interaction. Two of the six three-way interaction terms were statistically significant, both of which involved African American children (CSC × Moderate ASD × AA: t(1) = 2.58 95 % CI [5.12, 37.55], p < .05; CSC × Severe ASD × AA: t(1) = 2.10, 95 % CI [1.46, 44.60], p < .05). None of the two-way interactions between CSC and race or between CSC and ASD proved significant, p > .05.

The three-way interaction indicated that the effect of having a CSC depended on race and ASD severity. Contrary to our hypothesis, having a CSC was not consistently predictive of earlier diagnosis. In African American families, children who had a CSC were diagnosed later than those without (Fig. 1). In contrast, while Caucasian children were diagnosed later than Hispanic and African American counterparts in general, those with a CSC were diagnosed earlier than those without, regardless of ASD severity (Fig. 2). Of note, diagnostician type significantly differed by race, χ2(33, N = 1577) = 73.8, p < .001. While only 18.7 % of Caucasian children were diagnosed in primary care, 36.8 % of African American children were diagnosed by a PCP. Results of the multiple regression model are presented in Table 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Age of initial ASD diagnosis in African American children as a function of having a CSC and ASD severity, for first-born children born before 2006 whose parents had more than a high school education and were at the mean poverty level, controlling for insurance status

Fig. 2
figure 2

Age of initial ASD diagnosis in Caucasian children as a function of having a CSC and ASD severity, for first-born children born before 2006 whose parents had more than a high school education and were at the mean poverty level, controlling for insurance status

Table 3 Results of a multiple regression analysis predicting age of ASD diagnosis from birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, parental education and health insurance, race, ASD severity, and having a CSC

Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine the effects of birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, parental education, health insurance, race, ASD severity and CSC, and the interactions between race, ASD severity, and CSC on age of initial ASD diagnosis. We predicted that, after controlling for birth cohort, birth order, poverty level, parental education, and health insurance, having a CSC would be associated with younger age at time of initial diagnosis. We expected that autism severity would independently influence age of diagnosis, such that more severe symptomatology would yield earlier diagnosis. We also anticipated that children of minority racial or ethnic categories would be diagnosed later than Caucasian children. Finally, we predicted that the relationship between having a CSC and age of diagnosis would depend on both race and ASD severity.

Birth cohort was an important predictor of ASD diagnosis. Children born in or after 2006 (the year AAP guidelines were published) were diagnosed on average 35 months earlier than those born between 1994 and 2005. Compared to the 2014 CDC report, which reported an average age of diagnosis at 53 months for children born pre-AAP implementation, this suggests a noteworthy improvement in the diagnostic process (CDC 2014). However, while this may indicate that nationwide implementation of ASD screening guidelines improved screening and precipitated diagnosis, children in the later cohort were also significantly younger. Given that children born in or after 2006 were so young, they may represent the optimally diagnosed. As a result, interpretation of this effect should be made cautiously. Moreover, research on the success of AAP guideline implementation has produced mixed evidence. While most primary care practices increased their structured screening of ASD, referral of children who screened positive was inconsistent. In a study examining outcomes of the AAP screening guidelines, King et al. (2010) found that more than one-third of children who failed the screen were not referred to specialty care. Moreover, doctors that did refer patients to specialists found that many families failed to follow-up with recommended referrals (King et al. 2010). Consequently, conclusions drawn from the considerable effect size of the cohort predictor may not be solely attributable to AAP guideline implementation. Instead, we can speculate that increases in parental and physician awareness of ASD have also contributed to earlier identification and diagnosis seeking (Fountain et al. 2011; Hertz-Picciotto and Delwiche 2009). We can also postulate that age differences contributed to the magnitude of the measured effect.

Two other covariates significantly predicted age of ASD diagnosis. First, being a younger sibling was associated with earlier diagnosis. Parents’ familiarity with early warning signs of ASD likely explains this effect. Second, poverty level predicted diagnostic age, such that more impoverished children were diagnosed later than wealthier children. As suggested by the literature, families of lower SES are less likely to have a regular source of care and to receive routine medical supervision (DeVoe et al. 2007, 2008) even after controlling for health insurance (Fiscella et al. 2002). Moreover, families of lower SES may encounter additional economic and environmental barriers following referral to specialty care, given an increased likelihood of financial difficulties to afford medical copayments, time off work, and childcare for other family members (Kuhlthau et al. 2004). Consequently, the main effect of poverty level may reflect both pre- and post-consultation disparities. In contrast, while we expected that parental education would have an impact on diagnostic age, the current sample was better educated than that of the average United States citizen, which may explain the lack of anticipated findings.

In terms of interaction effects, the relationship between having a CSC and age of initial diagnosis depended on race. While having a CSC prompted earlier ASD diagnosis in Caucasian children, it delayed diagnosis for African American children. This racial disparity in children who have a CSC suggests two possibilities: race-based differences in practitioner and/or parent behavior.

One theory is that these two groups may be treated differently by physicians when first reporting ASD symptoms. That is, physicians may not refer African American children to specialists at the same rate or with the same speed as they refer other patients. Given that PCPs were only responsible for one-fifth of ASD diagnoses in the sample, and that this number is likely to be an overestimate because parents may be recalling first concern by any medical professional, we can assume that a majority of children were referred to, and eventually diagnosed by, a specialist: psychologist, psychiatrist or other. Racial disparities in referral practices are well documented and have been stable over decades, indicating a reliable difference in referral practices across medical specialty and time (Cook et al. 2009; Murphy et al. 2009; Harstad et al. 2013; Steyerberg et al. 2007). This disparity has also been documented in child healthcare (Boss et al. 2011; Mayer et al. 2004; Shields et al. 2004; Toomey et al. 2013; Zimmerman 2005).

Diagnostic disparities seen in the literature were also observed in the current sample. Diagnostician type significantly differed by race. While only one in five Caucasian children were diagnosed in primary care, over a third of African American children were diagnosed by a primary physician. This latter percentage is much larger than the average estimated in Goin-Kochel et al.’s (2006) study of sources of final, confirmatory ASD diagnoses, which estimated that only 15 % of ASD diagnoses were determined by a pediatrician or general physician, a team of professionals or another health practitioner. Although errors in parent recall of diagnostic timing may have influenced our results, we may nonetheless conclude with some caution that slower or less frequent referrals to the appropriate specialist may have delayed diagnosis of African American children presenting to their regular physician with moderate and severe ASD symptoms. This finding is concerning considering that children with more severe symptomatology ASD are likely to display more noticeable pathological symptoms.

While discrimination in referral practice may be one factor that influences decisions to refer to specialty care, primary care physicians may also be slower to recognize ASD in minority children due to differences in ASD presentations. Sell et al. (2012) found that African American children had higher rates of object preoccupations and inflexible routines, while Caucasian children demonstrated more unusual sensory stimuli preoccupations and abnormal motor development. Becerra et al. (2014) found that ethnic minority children were more likely to display ASD phenotypes with severe emotional outbursts and impaired expressive language than Caucasian children. Consequently, primary care physicians trained to identify typical or common ASD presentations may overlook minority children displaying phenotypic differences.

Nonetheless, the above explanation fails to explain why African American children with moderate and severe ASD who had a CSC were diagnosed later than their counterparts who had no regular place of medical care. One possibility is that children without a CSC tend to come from poorer homes that qualify for programs such as Head Start, which mandate regular tracking of developmental and educational progress of students. Likewise, minority children without a CSC may be more likely to attend low-cost Medicaid-eligible clinics that mandate Early and Periodic Screening, Diagnosis, and Treatment (EPSDT). In fact, Daniels and Mandell (2013) found that children from states with higher EPSDT compliance rates were diagnosed earlier than those with lower compliance.

However, the sample demonstrated no significant difference in poverty level between African American children who have a CSC those who do not. African American children from families that earned 0–199 % of the FPL were equally as likely to endorse having a CSC as children in the 200–400 % group (94.05 vs. 95 % respectively). This therefore minimizes the chance that African American children without a CSC were more likely to benefit from EPSDT or other federally mandated public program screenings.

Given that income differences are unlikely to fully explain diagnostic disparities in the current study, we may posit that primary physicians simply may not be recognizing the appropriate warning signs in African American children. The most recent CDC prevalence report reveals that Non-Hispanic White children were 30 % more likely to receive an ASD diagnosis than non-Hispanic Black children. However, of those diagnosed with an ASD, only 25 % of White children were classified in the range of intellectual disability, compared to 48 % of non-Hispanic Black children and 38 % of Hispanic children (CDC 2014). Following this logic, minority children may be suspected of having an intellectual disability or other psychopathology before the ASD diagnosis is eventually decided.

Insurance type may be another factor implicated in race-based differences in the diagnosis of children with a CSC. Access to outpatient specialty care has been shown to be limited for publicly insured children, likely due to specialists’ reluctance to accept public health insurance (Bisgaier and Rhodes 2011). However, comparison of public versus private health insurance failed to demonstrate significant differences in age of ASD diagnosis in our sample.

Another possibility is that the difference between groups lies at the patient rather than at the physician level. First, a CSC may mean different things across demographic groups. African Americans and Latinos are more likely to identify an emergency department as their primary source of care (Baker et al. 1994). Since patients who rely on emergency services tend to have 25 % fewer physician visits, this may explain the observed race-based discrepancy in children with a CSC (Baker et al. 1994).

Second, patient perception of discrimination may also promote consultation delays. In general, African, Hispanic and Asian American patients report more perceived provider discrimination and poorer health than Caucasians (Lee et al. 2009). Perceived discrimination has been shown to predict a variety of negative health behaviors. Perceived bias is related to underutilization of medical care even after controlling for socio-demographic characteristics, health care access, and physical and mental health (Burgess et al. 2008). It has also been tied to delays of medical tests and treatments (Casagrande et al. 2007; Facione and Facione 2007; Wagner and Abbott 2007), avoidance of preventative services (Trivedi and Ayanian 2006), and avoidance of alternative care options (Bazargan et al. 2005). As a result, minority patients treated by doctors they perceive as discriminatory may delay ASD consultation for their child in an attempt to avoid anticipated bias.

Third, diagnostic delays for minority children with a CSC may reflect differences in both parental initiation of diagnosis and in referral attendance. Minority parents have been shown to follow through with referrals to specialists at a lower rate than Caucasian parents (Zuckerman et al. 2011), a difference that has also been established in regards to autism referral attendance (Kalb et al. 2012). Minority parents may face more socioeconomic, scheduling, and transportation barriers to visit specialists (Broder-Fingert et al. 2013; Kalb et al. 2012). They may also hold different beliefs about the importance or seriousness of specialty consultations and are more likely to have had prior negative experiences in specialty care (Broder-Fingert et al. 2013). Another possibility is that parents of different backgrounds self-refer for autism diagnosis at disparate rates. Carr and Lord (2013) found that African American mothers with lower education reported less negative impact related to having a child with ASD than Caucasian mothers. Feeling less caregiver burden may result in later diagnosis seeking by African American parents.

While this study replicates others in underscoring the importance of demographic variables in ASD diagnosis, it is novel in its exposition of race and autism severity as moderators of the effect of having a CSC on age of diagnosis. Any existing differential medical practices across race and ASD type must be addressed to prevent further delays in diagnosis and treatment, as must the possible influence of discrepant parent behavior on delayed diagnosis seeking. In terms of strengths, this study has strong external validity given its sample size and demographic variety. The NSCH is the only survey of its kind to collect such extensive child health information on the national level.

The results of the study must also be considered within the context of several study limitations. First, the survey relies on retrospective recall and parent-report of ASD severity. Parents may have rated severity in a subjective manner, likely influenced by intellectual disability and behavioral problems, rather than having received a severity rating by a diagnostician. Similarly, age of ASD diagnosis was parent-reported and therefore subject to recall error or to confusion with age of first concern elicited by a healthcare professional. Given the possibility that minority children may have received an intellectual disability (ID) diagnosis before being diagnosed with ASD, earlier diagnosis of Hispanic and African American children may actually reflect age of ID diagnoses or special needs concerns.

The CSC variable also reflects parent report and may therefore be confounded by parent interpretation of a CSC. Of importance, the lack of a measure of CSC and insurance status preceding diagnosis limits the generalizability of our findings. Relatedly, the finding that health insurance type did not predict age of diagnosis must be interpreted with caution given that only 2 % of the sample was uninsured. Finally, the survey response rate of 23 % suggests that respondents may reflect a sample of convenience.

Statistical analyses of the model indicated a moderate effect size, which suggests that other factors contributed to age of diagnosis. Besides those variables studied, physician training has been identified as an important variable in ASD diagnosis (Fiscella et al. 2014; Fountain et al. 2011; Heidgerken et al. 2005). Differences in training of physicians serving primarily Caucasian versus African American populations may elucidate group differences, especially given different phenotypic expressions of ASD seen in the literature (Becerra et al. 2014; Sell et al. 2012). Implicit and explicit bias of physicians serving minority patients would also be worthwhile to study. Likewise, understanding how perceived discrimination plays into consultation delays would be an equally important factor to research. Lastly, more research is needed to clarify the intrapersonal factors involved in diagnosis seeking. Understanding which symptom led to referral (social, behavioral, intellectual), which person initiated the evaluation based on suspicion (parent, teacher or physician), and which tool and health professional confirmed the diagnosis would help to elucidate the diagnostic process and its predictors.

Determining deterrents to diagnosis, whether bias-based or not, is vital to reducing the gap between perception of ASD symptoms and confirmation of diagnosis. Timely diagnosis affords its recipients the specialized services that optimize outcomes (Fountain et al. 2011; Rogers and Vismara 2008). In addition, training physicians to recognize ASD symptoms in minority children and to practice nondiscriminatory referrals may help remedy racial disparities in the effect of having a CSC on age at diagnosis. Minimizing racial disparities may in turn decrease patient-based treatment delays associated with perceived discrimination and lower referral attendance to specialty care. Also, given that a considerable percent of children who are flagged do not complete follow-ups, health education of parents about the importance of specialty care must be emphasized.

One model that is increasingly popular and facilitates coordination of both preventive care and specialty referrals is the medical home (American Academy of Pediatrics 2005). Having a medical home has been shown to significantly improve care coordination and specialty care follow-through, and to in turn decrease unmet needs for children with special health care needs (Boudreau et al. 2014). Given recent provisions in the 2010 Patient Protection and Affordable Care Act that support implementation of the medical home model on a larger scale, we may be able to look forward to more regular health care utilization and better care coordination by families that have thus far been unable to access care (National Academy of State Health Policy 2010).