Introduction

Ever since the work of Darwin (1871), evolutionary biologists have focused on sex ratios as they are a key aspect of the natural history of sexually reproducing organisms and are particularly important for an understanding of social behaviour, breeding strategies and population dynamics (Hardy 2002; West 2009). Therefore, understanding how natural and sexual selection act on sex ratios is a significant component in studies on the evolutionary basis of animal behaviour. The core idea of frequency-dependent sex allocation was developed by Fisher (1930), who argued that the ratio of males to females at birth should not deviate from parity as long as the costs of producing males and females are identical. Numerous theoretical models have been developed following Fisher’s seminal argument that predicted skewed offspring sex ratios under certain environmental and genetic conditions (Hamilton 1967; Trivers and Willard 1973; Daan et al. 1996; reviewed by Trivers 1985; Hardy 2002; West 2009).

A shared prediction of several of these models is that the offspring sex ratio should deviate from unity as a result of temporal variation in costs and/or benefits of reproduction. For instance, bias is expected if the environment has sex-specific effects on offspring fitness (Trivers and Willard 1973; Bell et al. 2014; Bowers et al. 2015). Parents in poor environments might produce more of the sex that costs less (Myers 1978; Gomendio et al. 1990) or survives better (Heinsohn et al. 2011). As environmental factors often change seasonally and/or annually, offspring sex ratios are expected to be influenced by time of breeding (Pen et al. 1999; Bordier et al. 2014; Minias 2016).

Parental quality may also influence offspring sex ratios. The parental condition hypothesis predicts that parents in good condition should bias their allocation towards the sex that produces the greater increase in reproductive value for a given level of investment (Trivers and Willard 1973). The results from numerous studies are consistent with this hypothesis (Whittingham and Dunn 2000; Lu et al. 2013; Bowers et al. 2017), although the patterns are certainly not at all general (Leimar 1996; Hewison and Gaillard 1999). According to the attractive mate hypothesis (Burley 1981), if females get higher relative fitness returns from sons than daughters by mating with an attractive male, they bias the sex ratio of their offspring towards sons (Ellegren et al. 1996). For many sexually dimorphic species, variation in the ornamentation or body size of males influences offspring sex ratios (Sheldon et al. 1999; Romano et al. 2015; Booksmythe et al. 2017).

Sex ratio variation may emerge during early development. For instance, in sexually dimorphic species in which the males are larger, male offspring have a higher mortality rate than female offspring (Kalmbach and Benito 2007), presumably due to their higher sensitivity to starvation (Kalmbach et al. 2005). The reverse mortality pattern, i.e. higher female than male chick mortality, has also been reported for raptors, ducks, swifts and passerines (Arroyo 2002; Hipkiss et al. 2002; Bize et al. 2005; Råberg et al. 2005; Lehikoinen et al. 2008). Although the causes of these sex-dependent mortalities are rarely identified, the sex bias in juvenile mortalities may have knock-on effects on population viability with major conservation implications, especially for endangered birds (Donald 2007; Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2017; Ramula et al. 2018).

Here, we investigate sex allocation in a small shorebird, the Kentish Plover, Charadrius alexandrinus, which has a body mass of about 32–56 g (Wiersma et al. 2018). The Kentish Plover provides a good model system for understanding the evolution of mating systems, and has been used for ecological and evolutionary studies for the following main reasons. First, they have an unusually wide geographic distribution that includes Europe, North Africa and Asia (Wiersma et al. 2018), and they inhabit different types of habitat that include hot deserts, temperate coast and dunes, inland salt marshes and cold subalpine lakes. This diversity offers unique opportunities to test behavioural, ecological and physiological adaptation to different environments. Second, the Kentish Plover exhibits diverse mating systems and parental care strategies, and has been used as a model system of mating system evolution as either the male or the female parent may desert the family shortly and then re-mate, and, therefore, create a variable degree of polygamy in different populations (Lessells 1984; Székely and Lessells 1993; Fraga and Amat 1996; Székely 2014). Third, recent work showed that closely related plover populations exhibit different sex ratios, and that these sex ratio biases are related to the extent of parental cooperation (Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2018). These observations raise the possibility that different plover populations have divergent sex allocation patterns that lead to biased sex ratio that have knock-on effects on mating system and parental care. For example, a previous study of Kentish Plover in Turkey found that hatch date was associated with the hatchling sex ratio, as the hatchling sex ratio became progressively female biased over the breeding season (Székely et al. 2004), and consistent with theoretical expectations (Kokko and Jennions 2008; Székely et al. 2014b), the male parent looks after the young (Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2018).

Here, we focus on a population of Kentish Plover that breeds in Eastern China and investigate whether parental quality and/or hatch date predict the offspring sex ratio. In Eastern China, both parents incubate the eggs, although after chicks hatch, many females desert their brood during the first week, so that the males look after the chicks until they fledge or die (P. Que, unpublished data). As a male-skewed sex ratio could lead to brood desertion in female plovers (Székely et al. 1999), the offspring of the Chinese population may have a biased sex ratio. The specific aims of our study were to test: (1) whether hatchling sex ratios are biased; (2) whether sex ratios vary in relation to hatching date, brood age and breeding year; and (3) whether parental quality predicts hatchling sex ratio. In addition, by incubating eggs that were confiscated from illegal egg-collectors (see below), we also test (4) whether sex ratio shift may occur between laying and hatching of the eggs.

Methods

Study sites

We conducted fieldwork during April-July 2009 and 2012 at Nanpu Wetland (27,000 ha, 39°09′N, 118°09′E to 39°02′N, 118°19′E; Fig. 1S), and during April–July 2013–2016 at Daqinghe Wetland (9700 ha, 39°08′N, 118°46′E to 39°14′N, 118°52′E; Fig. 1S). Both field sites are important stopover and breeding sites for migratory waterbirds in the East Asian-Australasian Flyway and are located in Bohai Bay, Eastern China. Over 200,000 waterbirds stop or breed in these areas each year (Yang et al. 2011). These two sites are composed of large artificial salt pans used for salt processing and prawn aquaculture. Approximately 1800 pairs of Kentish Plover breed at Nanpu and 600 pairs at Daqinghe (Lei 2017). We treated Nanpu and Daqinghe as one study area in this study as they are less than 60 km apart and have similar breeding habitats.

Field methods

On finding a nest, we floated the eggs to estimate the stage of incubation, initiation date and hatch date (Liebezeit et al. 2007; Que et al. 2015). Nests were checked at least daily near the time of hatching (approximately the 22nd day after the start of incubation) to capture freshly hatched chicks in the nest scrape (Székely et al. 2008). As the Kentish Plover is not very sensitive to cars, we could hide inside a car to search through a spotting scope for chicks and brooded plovers that left the nest scrape (Székely et al. 2008). After successful capture, body mass, tarsus length and bill length were measured for each chick. Body mass was measured to the closest 0.01 g with an electronic scale (G&G MS501), while bill and tarsus lengths were measured to the closest 0.01 mm using Vernier callipers (Mitutoyo 500-762-10) following Redfern and Clark (2001). A blood sample (< 20 μl) was also taken from the left leg vein and stored below − 20 °C in a freezer.

Parents were caught using funnel traps or spring traps during incubation or brood care (Hall and Cavitt 2012). Adults were sexed according to the colour of the head and breast (Hayman et al. 1986), and for a subset, morphological sexing was confirmed by molecular sexing (see below). Body mass, tarsus length and bill lengths were measured following the above protocol, and flattened wing chord length was measured to the closest 1 mm with a wing ruler (Redfern and Clark 2001). Plovers were marked with a numbered aluminium ring from the National Bird Banding Centre of China, and a unique combination of one to three coloured plastic leg bands for visual identification. All handling methods were approved by the Hebei Province Forestry Bureau.

On 5 May 2015, two people illegally collected 138 Kentish Plover eggs for human consumption at Hangu, Tianjin (Fig. 1S). Local policemen and conservation volunteers apprehended these individuals and brought the eggs to a wildlife rescue centre based at Daqinghe Saltworks. The eggs were immediately put into an incubator (type Yikebeite RK-60); we estimated that less than 6 h elapsed from when the eggs were collected until they were put in the incubator. During these 6 h, the eggs were exposed to the ambient temperature of approximately 30 °C. We measured and floated these eggs to estimate the number of incubated days and hatching status using the protocol of Székely et al. (2008), and placed all of the eggs into an incubator at 37.6 °C with 80–85% humidity (Page et al. 1989). We considered an egg to have failed when it remained unhatched after 7 days of estimated hatch date. Out of 138 eggs, 104 eggs hatched and 34 failed to hatch. For the hatched eggs, we took blood samples from the chicks for molecular sexing. Developed embryos were found in all unhatched eggs; we took tissue samples from these embryos. As we did not know from how many clutches the eggs had been taken, we considered each egg as an individual datum. After the measurements and blood samples were taken, the chicks were reared and released back into the wild by the Daqinghe Wildlife Rescue Centre when they fledged and had gained adult weight.

Molecular sexing

DNA was extracted from the blood and tissue samples using TIANamp Genomic DNA Kits (Tiangen Biotech, Beijing) and primers 2550F/2718R (Fridolfsson and Ellegren 1999). Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplification was carried out in a total volume of 20 μl. The final reaction conditions were as follows: 0.5 μl of each primer, 4 μl of target DNA, 10 μl of 2 × EasyTaq PCR SuperMix and 5 μl of deionized water. Thermal cycling was carried out in a Biometra UnoII: initial denaturation for 5 min at 94 °C was followed by 35 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C, 45 s at 48 °C, 45 s at 72 °C, with a final extension for 7 min at 72 °C. As molecular sex determination is known to be prone to errors (Robertson and Gemmell 2006), we randomly chose 300 samples, included 40 of 104 hatched confiscated eggs and 20 of 34 unhatched confiscated eggs, and used another set of primers, 2602F/2669R (van der Velde et al. 2017), to test the result of primers 2550F/2718R. PCR amplification was carried out in a total volume of 20 μl: with 1 μl of each primer, 2 μl of target DNA, 10 μl of 2× EasyTaq PCR SuperMix and 6 μl of deionized water. The PCR program was as follows: initial denaturation for 1 min at 94 °C, 35 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C, 60 s at 61 °C, 60 s at 72 °C, and a final extension for 2 min at 72 °C. Electrophoresis of the PCR products was carried out on 2.5% agarose gel stained at 120 V. The products were then stained with SYBR and photographed under ultraviolet (UV) light. The male samples had one band, while the female samples had double bands.

The results of the sex determination experiment with two different primer pairs were identical, and the bands on the UV-transillumination were clearer and brighter than before. In addition, 24 chicks were tested blind with regards to the first set of results, and the second sexing provided identical results to the first one. Finally, we molecularly sexed 48 adults (19 males and 29 females) that had been sexed in the field using plumage characteristics. The results of molecular sexing matched the field-based sexing for all adults.

Statistical analyses

One male and two females produced two broods either within a year or in subsequent years, although only the first complete brood was included in the analysis for each adult. These complete broods were included in the statistical analyses. We created the following sets of data: (1) broods in which all the hatched chicks were captured in the nest, which we termed the offspring sex ratio at hatching, were used to analyse the relationship between offspring sex ratio and hatching date; (2) all broods that were either captured in the nest or off the nest, which were used to analyse the relationship between the offspring sex ratio and the chick age. The equation given by Székely and Cuthill (1999) was used to estimate the age of chicks which were captured off the nest. (3) Broods in which all the hatched chicks were captured in the nest, and of which both parents were captured during incubation, were used to analyse the relationship between the offspring sex ratio at hatching and parental body size and condition.

Binomial tests were used to determine whether offspring sex ratio bias occurred at hatching. χ2-tests were used to ascertain whether variations in the sex ratio occurred between years.

We calculated a scaled mass index (SMI) to quantify the body condition of the parents from the body mass and a linear body measurement (Peig and Green 2009). SMI is a good indicator of body condition as it accounts for the changing relationship between mass and length as body size changes (Peig and Green 2010), and it has been used to indicate the energy reserves and body components in birds (Bell et al. 2014; Cooper et al. 2015), including the Red-capped Plover (Charadrius ruficapillus) (Tan et al. 2015). As wing length was the structural measure most strongly correlated with body mass (Table S1), we used wing length as a measure of structural size. At first, we created a bivariate plot of mass and wing length to excluded any outliers that may distort the expected relationship. Then standardised major axis regression was carried out to calculate the slope. Thus, the SMI of body condition can be computed for each individual following the equation described by Peig and Green (2009).

To assess the effect of parental condition and time of breeding on the offspring sex ratio, we used generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) with binomial errors and a logit link. We fitted the brood sex ratio as the dependent variable, the brood size as weights, the brood identifier and year as random factors, and body size (mass and tarsus length) and the body condition (SMI) of both parents, hatching date, and brood age as independent variables (fixed effects). We created candidate models of all possible combinations including all considered variables for each data set. As a previous study suggested that hatching date has a non-linear relationship with brood sex ratio in the Snowy Plover (Charadrius nivosus) (Saalfeld et al. 2013), the quadratic effects of hatching date, and the interaction between year and hatching date were also considered.

Statistical analyses were carried out in R version 3.2.2 (R Development Core Team 2015) using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015) for fitting GLMMs and the MuMIn package (Barton 2015) for selected models. Akaike’s information criterion corrected for small sample size (AICc) was used to assess candidate models. The best model is the model with the lowest AICc score, and the models with a change in AICc of less than 2 are considered candidates (Burnham and Anderson 2002). We conducted model averaging to obtain robust estimates of the model parameters if there was more than one model with an AICc less than 2 (Burnham and Anderson 2002). Parameter estimates are represented as the mean ± SE, and we calculated two-tailed probabilities. Sex ratio is expressed as the proportion of males.

Results

Brood sex ratio

Over the 6 years of the study, blood samples from 1322 chicks were collected from 676 broods, for which the sex was determined in 1264 chicks (95.61%; Table 1). The sex ratio was not significantly biased at hatching (0.49 ± 0.023, χ2 = 0.33, p = 0.57, n = 440 chicks from 146 broods; Fig. 1).

Table 1 Number of broods and chicks of the Kentish Plover in this study
Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution of the number of male chicks in freshly hatched complete broods of Kentish Plover (n = 144 broods). The modal brood size is 3 at chick hatching. The observed distribution is not different from that predicted by the binomial distribution (χ2 goodness of fit, χ2 = 2.52, p = 0.47)

Brood sex ratio and timing of breeding

The sex ratios at hatching did not differ significantly among years (χ2 = 9.48, df = 5, p = 0.09, n = 146 broods; Fig. S2). Year, hatching date, and the interaction between year and hatching date were not included in the candidate model for brood sex ratio at hatching (Table S2). This suggested that breeding year and hatching date did not significantly vary with brood sex ratio at hatching. We have no evidence that the proportion of males changed significantly with brood age [β = 0.19 (− 0.08, 0.45) (± 95% CI), p = 0.16, n = 1252 chicks from 664 broods; Table 2].

Table 2 Sex ratio in Kentish Plover broods in relation to hatching date and brood age in Eastern China (n = 664 broods)

Brood sex ratio and parental body condition

The brood sex ratio at hatching was not significantly related to the body size of the female parent [body mass, β = − 0.21 (− 0.73, 0.30) (± 95% CI), p = 0.41, n = 249 chicks from 83; Table 3] or the male parent (body mass, β = 0.19 (− 0.33, 0.70) [± 95% CI], p = 0.47, n = 249 chicks from 83; Table 3); none of the candidate models included male or female tarsus length (n = 249 chicks from 83 broods; Table 3).

Table 3 Brood sex ratio at hatching of Kentish Plover with parental body size and condition (n = 83 broods)

Brood sex ratio at hatching was not significantly related to parental body condition (SMI) [female parent, β = − 0.17 (− 0.20, 0.71) [± 95% CI], p = 0.52; male parent, β = 0.17 (− 0.26, 0.59) (± 95% CI), p = 0.52, n = 249 chicks from 83; Table 4].

Table 4 Sex ratio of confiscated Kentish Plover eggs in relation to day of incubation at collection and hatching success (i.e. hatched or failed in the incubator; n = 117 eggs)

Sex ratio of the confiscated eggs

Of 104 hatched eggs, 49 were male (binomial test, p = 0.62), whereas of 34 unhatched eggs, nine were male (binomial test, p = 0.01). The sex ratio bias in unhatched eggs is significantly different from that in hatched eggs (9:25 cf. 49:55; Fisher exact test, p = 0.045). The overall sex ratio of 138 eggs was not significantly different from even (binomial test, p = 0.07). The sex ratio of confiscated eggs did not vary significantly during incubation [GLMMs, β = 0.04 (− 0.01, 0.09) (± 95% CI), p = 0.10, n = 117 eggs; Table 4].

Discussion

Our study produced four main findings. First, the chick sex ratio did not differ significantly from even at laying or at hatching, which is consistent with previous studies that found unbiased sex ratios in plovers (Székely et al. 2004; Saalfeld et al. 2013; Riordan et al. 2015; Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2017). However, the sample size was limited for inferring sex ratio at egg-laying, these data were from only 1 year, and the eggs were pooled for the analyses rather than accounting for clutch identity or parental identity. Nevertheless, these results suggest that the hatchling sex ratio is not strongly biased in Charadrius plovers, which is consistent with results for most birds that have been studied to date (Donald 2007; Székely et al. 2014a).

Our finding that there was no significant shift in sex ratio during egg development underpins that primary and secondary sex ratios are not different, and suggests that there is no sex-specific offspring mortality before hatching. Such data are quite rare for wild bird populations for ethical reasons, although we were lucky to have a dedicated conservationist in Bohai Bay willing to incubate confiscated eggs for us. We wish to note two findings here. First, the sex ratio was significantly biased towards males in unhatched eggs, suggesting that female embryos may be more sensitive to disturbance and/or fluctuation in incubation temperatures than males. Although female eggs might have been treated more harshly than male eggs for unknown reasons (e.g. female eggs may, for unknown reasons, have spent more time in the collector’s bag than male eggs), there is a possibility that male and female eggs have genuinely different sensitivity to perturbations. Second, to detect shifts in pre-birth offspring, large sample sizes are needed, and such samples are rarely available for ornithologists. For instance, Orzack et al. (2015) reported significant shifts in foetal abortion in humans, although this study used over 90,000 individuals.

Second, unlike previous studies in Kentish and Snowy Plovers (Székely et al. 2004; Saalfeld et al. 2013), the hatchling sex ratio did not show significant seasonal variation in Bohai Bay. This suggests that different plover populations may exhibit different seasonal trends in offspring sex ratios: in Turkey the proportion of males decreased over the breeding season (Székely et al. 2004), in Texas the seasonal pattern was quadratic as the proportion of males was high both earlier and later than in the middle of the breeding season (Saalfeld et al. 2013), whereas we found no significant trend in Eastern China. A seasonal trend in offspring sex ratio may vary among species (Smallwood and Smallwood 1998; McIntosh et al. 2003; Goławski et al. 2016; Minias 2016), and even among different populations of a species (e.g. Fiala 1981 vs. Weatherhead 1983; Howe 1977 vs. Maddox and Weatherhead 2009).

Third, in previous studies of Kentish and Piping Plovers (Charadrius melodus) the proportion of males increased with brood age (Székely et al. 2004; Saunders and Cuthbert 2015), which appears to result from female-biased chick mortality (Székely et al. 2004; Saunders and Cuthbert 2015). The lack of a significant association between brood sex ratio and brood age in Bohai Bay is consistent with our previous finding that mortality did not differ significantly between male and female chicks (Que 2015).

Fourth, we found no evidence for offspring sex ratio adjustment relative to parental body size. These results did not support the parental condition hypothesis or the attractiveness hypothesis. The underlying assumption of the parental condition hypothesis, the function indicating that investment to fitness returns differ for male and female offspring, is difficult to measure in most study systems. An in-depth investigation of the underlying assumptions is necessary for each study testing the parental condition hypothesis. Similarly, choosing appropriate traits that indeed relate to the fitness of male offspring and are selected by females is a major challenge for sex-allocation studies.

Recent theoretical and empirical studies are adding a new layer of interest to sex allocation by arguing that an important, yet neglected, aspect of mating system evolution is adult sex ratio (Kokko and Jennions 2008; Székely et al. 2014b; Schacht et al. 2017). Frequency-dependent selection of these social behaviours are expected, as the rarer sex in the population has more opportunities to choose a mate and/or abandon the partner than the more common sex. Phylogenetic analyses of birds including shorebirds are consistent with these propositions (Liker et al. 2013, 2014). Consistent with these phylogenetic comparative results, a recent demographic analysis of six closely related populations showed significantly different adult sex ratios in Charadrius plovers [Eberhardt-Phillips et al. (2018); note that the latter study did not include data from China]. Importantly, Eberhart-Phillips et al. (2018) also showed that a major factor creating biased adult sex ratios is sex-dependent juvenile survival. Whilst we found no significant difference in juvenile survival in Bohai Bay, our brood observations concentrated on small chicks shortly after hatching, so we do not know whether sex-specific mortality may occur in subsequent chick development. Therefore, studies are needed to investigate the transitions in sex ratios from conception till adulthood.

In conclusion, using 440 chicks from a well-studied shorebird species, the Kentish Plover, we found no sex bias at hatching in China. Therefore, it seems unlikely that the male-biased adult sex ratios reported for Kentish and Snowy Plover populations (Kosztolányi et al. 2011; Stenzel et al. 2011; Carmona-Isunza et al. 2017; Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2018) are due to biased secondary (hatching) sex ratios, consistent with birds in general (Székely et al. 2014a). We call for further studies to understand the causes of adult sex ratio variation by testing sex differences in maturation and/or in mortalities of chicks and adults. Such studies are needed not only to clarify one of the key areas of avian evolutionary ecology, i.e. breeding system evolution, but may also have conservation implications for the management of endangered bird populations with biased sex ratios (Donald 2007; Eberhart-Phillips et al. 2017).