Introduction

Predation is one of the most important selective forces in evolution, shaping several morphological characteristics and animal behavior (Lima and Dill 1990; Sansom et al. 2009; Preisser and Orrock 2012). Failing to avoid predation may result in death for the animal; thus, the ability to perceive and respond to a threat is under high selective pressure throughout evolutionary history (Lima and Dill 1990; Kats and Dill 1998).

Arthropods possess several defense mechanisms to avoid predation including crypsis, mimicry, thanatosis, autotomization of appendices, and fleeing, and aggressive behaviors like stinging (Tullberg et al. 2005; Fleming et al. 2007; Gnaspini and Hara 2007; Pomini et al. 2010; Coelho et al. 2017). Sexual dimorphism and the developmental stage are multi-integrated components affecting the defensive actions (Williams et al. 2001; Uma and Weiss 2012; Miller et al. 2016). For example, Centruroides vittatus (Say, 1821) males and females differ in morphology and behavior. Males sprint faster than females, while females with their larger, heavier bodies move slower and are more likely to sting and deliver repetitive stings more rapidly when threatened (Carlson and Rowe 2009; Carlson et al. 2014).

Many arthropods also exhibit ontogeny-related defensive behavior (Breed et al. 1990; Jeanne et al. 1992; Judd 2000; Haight 2008; Ramirez et al. 2010). For example, in the wasp Polybia occidentalis Olivier, 1791, the probability of a young worker engaging in defensive behavior is low but increases with age (Jeanne et al. 1992). Dangles et al. (2007) found that juveniles of the cricket Nemobius sylvestris (Bosc, 1792) exhibit a higher incidence of escape performances than older instars, suggesting that these variations may be related to the lower predatory risk perceived by large adults than by juveniles.

Modulation of antipredatory behavior according to diel rhythms has also been described in many arthropod species (McIntosh and Townsend 1994; Jones et al. 2011; Effertz and von Elert 2014; Watts et al. 2014). Watts et al. (2014) found that the orb-weaving spider Cyclosa turbinate (Walckenaer, 1841) exhibits thanatosis more frequently and for more extended periods during daytime when confronted with a predator stimulus in an experimental setup. Similarly, the orb-weaving spider Larinioides cornutus (Clerck, 1757) holds a huddled posture longer in the light than in the dark (Jones et al. 2011).

Being both a predator and prey, scorpions offer a robust opportunity to examine trade-offs associated with defensive behavior. To avoid predation, scorpions have evolved different defense mechanisms such as retreating to a burrow or other hiding place (Polis 1990), using pincers (chelae), venomous telsons (stingers), or both (Warburg 1998; van der Meijden et al. 2013). Morphology is also a key feature influencing scorpion defensive behavior. Many species (e.g., Tityus sp., Centruroides sp., and Isometrus sp.) from the family of Buthidae C. L. Koch, 1837 that possesses less robust pedipalp pincers react more often with stinging compared to species with more robust pedipalp pincers such as Scorpionidae Latreille, 1802, Diplocentridae Karsch, 1880, Liochelidae Fet and Bechly, 2001, or Iuridae Thorell, 1876 (Polis 1990; Warburg 1998; van der Meijden et al. 2013). Additionally, van der Meijden et al. (2013) found that species with strong pincers used these more often as a defense mechanism compared to species with weak pincers that rather used stinging.

In this study, we analyzed the defensive behavior of Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 by simulating a risk predation situation and demonstrated possible differences in the behavior response associated with sex, age, and diel period. Tityus pusillus, the litter-dwelling scorpion, is a sexually dimorphic species with males having more robust pincers and enlarged metasomas compared to females (Lira et al. 2018a). We addressed the following questions: (1) Do differences in the age of T. pusillus influence defensive behavior? (2) Do morphological differences, such as more robust pincers in males, caused by sexual dimorphism, influence the defensive performance in this species? (3) Does T. pusillus exhibit alternative defensive behavior according to the diel period, such as being more responsive during the nighttime?

Materials and methods

Studied species

Tityus pusillus is a sedentary, small-sized scorpion (30-35 mm) that occurs in the Atlantic Forest and Caatinga in Northeastern Brazil (Porto et al. 2010; Lira et al. 2018a). The species is found primarily in leaf litter layers (Lira et al. 2013, 2018b; Santos et al. 2018) and is sensitive to microhabitat structure changes (Lira et al. 2015; Dionisio-da-Silva et al. 2018). Tityus pusillus is a protandric species that reach adulthood after 4–5 molts, with females being able to give birth to 8 to 12 live offspring in 85 ± 12 days (Albuquerque and Lira 2016; Lira et al. 2018a).

In this study, 248 T. pusillus individuals (68 males, 90 females, and 90 juveniles in the second instar phase), collected in a remnant Atlantic forest in the Moreno municipality, Pernambuco state, northeast Brazil, were used. All animals were collected during the dry season in February 2014; during this season, T. pusillus exhibit an increase in foraging activity (Dionisio-da-Silva et al. 2018). In the laboratory, the animals were placed in individual plastic terraria (14 cm × 10 cm × 8 cm) and supplied with cardboard as a shelter and wetted cotton wool as a water source. They were left in observation for 30 days before the start of the experiments. Laboratory conditions were maintained at 24 °C ± 2 °C and 80% ± 5% relative humidity, and a 12:12 h light:dark photoperiod. The scorpions were fed weekly with cockroach nymphs of the species Nauphoeta cinerea (Oliver, 1789). The test animals were subjected to a fast 15 days before the experiments, and no gravid females (with embryos visible through the ventral mesosoma) were used.

Simulated predator experiments

Two sets of experiments using a simulated predator stimulus were performed to verify T. pusillus anti-predator response and to assess differences in behavior caused by age (adult/juvenile), sex (male/female), and diel period (night/day). All the procedures were performed by two researchers (AFAL and FMFA).

In the first trial based on Pomini et al. (2010), 123 scorpions (33 males, 45 females, and 45 juveniles) were used and predator attacks were simulated by holding the telson with forceps for 10 s and by dropping the animals from a height of 25 cm on a plastic tray (47 × 32 × 9 cm) containing 3 mm of litter layer. Between the trials, the litter layer was changed and the plastic tray was washed with 70% ethanol. In situations where thanatosis (freezing posture) was observed, the time individuals took to start moving again was recorded for up to 15 min. Behavioral assessments were performed at night (19:00–21:00) under red light (Machan 1968) and repeated after 15 days during the day between 13:00 and 14:00 to verify if diel period influenced the intensity of response to predation. All scorpions were fed before the second round of experiments.

In the second trial, a different group of scorpions (35 males, 45 females, and 45 juveniles in the second instar phase) was transferred to individual 1000 ml plastic terrariums (9 cm high and 14 cm in diameter) and allowed to acclimate for 15 min. Each scorpion had a separate terrarium in which the trials were carried out. In the second trial, each of the pincers was first restrained laterally in a random order for 5 s (per pincer) using large rubber-tipped tweezers, followed by a similar restriction (dorso-ventrally) of the prosoma (van der Meijden et al. 2013). Behavioral assessments were performed at night and during the day as described for the first trial. Scorpions were arbitrarily assigned to the day or the night group to ensure the randomness of the dataset. After 15 days, the groups were inverted. Voucher specimens were deposited in the Arachnological Collection of Universidade Federal de Pernambuco.

Statistical analyses

Age and sex differences in frequencies on defensive behavior (thanatosis or fleeing) were assessed using a Chi square test for the first trial. For the second trial, the frequencies of responses from the pincer and prosoma restrictions were assessed through a Chi square for independence. Differences in thanatosis time of adults vs. juveniles, males vs. females, and daytime vs. nighttime were assessed by the Mann–Whitney pairwise test with p values being adjusted using the Bonferroni’s correction. Before these analyses, the normality and variance of the data were assessed by the Shapiro–Wilk and the Levene tests, respectively. All statistical analyses were performed with the PAST 3.22 software (Hammer et al. 2001).

Results

In response to a simulated predator attack, T. pusillus exhibited five defensive behaviors (Table 1). Scorpions responded with thanatosis or fleeing when their telsons were pressed and released (Fig. 1) and with stinging, standing still, fleeing, or tail wagging when their pincers or prosomas were immobilized (Tables 2, 3). However, the frequency of these behaviors was influenced by sex, age (adult/juvenile stage), and diel period (Tables 2, 3).

Table 1 Defensive behaviors performed by Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 during trials
Fig. 1
figure 1

Defensive behavior (%) of Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 in response to a predation attack in simulated experiments by restricting the telson in daytime and nighttime periods. a Adults vs. juveniles; b males vs. females. Gray = thanatosis and black = fleeing

Table 2 Defensive behavior (%) of Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 juveniles (N2) and adults in response to restriction of different body parts and diel period in simulated experiments of predation attack
Table 3 Defensive behavior (%) of male and female Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 in response to restrictions of different body parts and diel period

After pressing the telsons with forceps and dropping the animals from a height in the first set of experiments, both juvenile and adult T. pusillus exhibited defensive behavior characterized by fleeing or thanatosis. However, a clear differential response related to age, sex, and diel period was observed. Juveniles and females exhibited a preference for fleeing. The flight response significantly increased among juveniles (χ2 = 4.52, df = 1, p = 0.03) if the simulated attack occurred during the nocturnal trials, while thanatosis was the main response of males independent of the diel period (Fig. 1a). Juveniles displayed significantly more fleeing behavior than adults in both daytime (χ2 = 3.59, df = 1, p = 0.05) and nighttime (χ2 = 13.9, df = 1, p < 0.01). Thanatosis was performed by 30% of the juveniles, while a fleeing response was performed by 60% of the juveniles during the diurnal trial. In nocturnal trials, thanatosis was performed seven times less by the juveniles compared to fleeing (Fig. 1a). Adult individuals showed no differences (χ2 = 0.01, df = 1, p = 0.73) in the frequency of thanatosis and fleeing, independent of the period (Fig. 1a).

The time spent in thanatosis (Table S1) (mean ± standard deviation) by adults during the day (758 ± 263 s) was not significantly different (U = 349, adjusted p = 0.14) from the time spent in thanatosis during the night (484.71 ± 412 s) (Fig. 2). Similarly, juveniles did not show a diel period effect in terms of the time spent in thanatosis (U = 13.5, adjusted p = 1) (Fig. 2). Compared to adults, juveniles spent significantly less time in thanatosis in daytime (U = 56.0, adjusted p < 0.01), but not in nighttime (U = 34.0, adjusted p = 1) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Thanatosis time (in seconds) performed (mean ± standard deviation) by Tityus pusillus Pocock, 1893 adults and juveniles in daytime and nighttime periods after simulated predator attacks by holding the telson followed by dropping the animals

During diurnal trials, males showed a higher preference for thanatosis (χ2 = 7.65, df = 1, p = 0.05) compared to females (Fig. 1b). However, the duration of the thanatosis response of males and females was similar (males: 723.63 ± 291.91 s; females: 822.25 ± 194.97 s; U = 105, adjusted p = 1). Likewise, no significant difference between males and females was recorded for time spent in thanatosis during the nocturnal trials, although the time spent in that state decreased for both sexes (males: 379.78 ± 412.44 s; females: 638.07 ± 374.75 s; U = 87.5, adjusted p = 1).

In the second trial, four types of defense strategies were registered: standing still, fleeing, tail wagging, and stinging. The type of strategy used was mostly influenced by which body part was restricted in both adults and juveniles. When the prosomas were pinched, the primary defense mechanism was to sting while when the pincers were immobilized, the individuals chose to either show no reactions or to flee (Table 2). Stinging behavior was not influenced by diel period (daytime χ2(3,2) = 1.85, df = 2, p = 0.39; nighttime χ2(3,2) = 0.67, df = 2, p = 0.71) in both adults and juveniles. However, in response to pincer restriction, standing still was more frequently observed than stinging. This behavior was predominantly performed by adults during both diel periods (daytime χ2(4,2) = 15.4, df = 3, p < 0.01; nighttime χ2(4,2) = 16.6, df = 3, p < 0.01).

Tail wagging was only performed by adults and was independent of body part restriction (prosoma or pincers) and diel period (Table 2). Differences in the response of pincer restriction were observed between sexes, but only in diurnal trials (χ2(4,2) = 54.4, df = 3, p < 0.01) and not in nocturnal trials (χ2(4,2) = 4.96, df = 3, p = 0.17). In diurnal trials, males performed primarily two reactions: fleeing or stinging, while females exhibited no reaction or tail wagging (Table 3). Contrarily, males and females performed mainly stinging in response to prosoma restriction, with no differences between daytime (χ2(3,2) = 4.84, df = 2, p = 0.08) or nighttime (χ2(3,2) = 1.08, df = 2, p = 0.58) (Table 3).

Discussion

This study described the defensive mechanisms used by the litter-dwelling scorpion Tityus pusillus to simulate predation situations. Our results indicate that this species exhibits behavioral plasticity in defensive strategies, with stinging, thanatosis, and fleeing as the main defensive behavioral reactions. Nevertheless, these responses can be modulated according to factors such as the body structure immobilized by a predator, sex, diel period, and developmental stage.

In response to threats, juveniles of T. pusillus would rather flee than to engage in combat, while adults are more actively defensive. Similar results were described in other arachnids such as the cobweb spider Parasteatoda tepidariorum C. L. Koch, 1841 (Uma and Weiss 2012), in which juvenile spiders fled in response to attacks by the predator, the mud-dauber wasp Chalybion californicum (Saussure, 1867), while adult spiders fought in response to the wasp attacks (Uma and Weiss 2012). In the black widow spider Latrodectus mactans (Fabricius, 1775), an ontogenetic shift in female aggressiveness has also been positively associated with developmental stage, with adults being more aggressive than juveniles (Troupe 2009). A possible explanation for the differential response to predation between juveniles and adults is the relative body size that often determines how an animal interacts with its surroundings (Werner and Gilliam 1984; Polis and McCormick 1987; Polis et al. 1989). Young animals and small-sized individuals are more easily preyed upon by other larger animals. Therefore, it is plausible that these animals flee rather than face the predators. Although scorpions possess a venomous stinger as a defensive weapon at all stages of the life cycle, they do not deviate from this trend (Polis 1990). Known as generalist predators capturing a wide diversity of preys (Polis 1990), juvenile and smaller sized scorpions are also subject to predation, resulting in a high mortality rate (e.g., Polis and McCormick 1987; Moreno-González and Hazzi 2012; Lira et al. 2016, 2017a, b).

Tityus pusillus also shows sex-related differences in defensive behavior with males performing thanatosis and stinging more often and females performing fleeing, tail wagging, and stinging more often. Morphological differences between T. pusillus sexes such as males possessing larger metasomal segments and more robust pedipalp pincers and females possessing larger prosomas and mesosomas (Lira et al. 2018a) may explain the differences in defensive behavior. Similarly, differences in defensive behavior of the scorpion species Centruroides vittatus may be associated with sexual dimorphism (Carlson et al. 2014). In this species, females are more aggressive than males, possibly because the females have larger bodies compared to males. This observation suggests that in this species the females are the combative sex to compensate for their locomotive restriction for reproduction while males possess longer legs for sprinting to evade predators and to find mates (Carlson et al. 2014). Another explanation for the differences between male and female T. pusillus could be related to the life strategies of each sex, as researched by Coelho et al. (2017). Additionally, only T. pusillus adults exhibit unusual ‘tail wagging’ behavior, which is commonly considered as a reproductive dance (e.g., Melville et al. 2003; Gaffin and Brownell 2010; Taylor et al. 2012) and not as a defensive behavior. Tail wagging consists of metasomal movements that occur before stinging and are more common in Tityus pusillus females than males. In a non-reproductive context, this behavior has been described for juveniles of Tityus uruguayensis Borelli, 1901 when sharing the same prey (Toscano-Gadea and Costa 2006). Moving the metasomal before stinging may be considered as a warning of the aggressiveness level of the scorpion against potential predators or competitors.

Differences in behavioral strategies in response to the observed diel period in T. pusillus adults suggest that this scorpion species shows behavioral plasticity according to environmental pressure. Diurnal predators (e.g., lizards and snakes) typically hunt for prey through visual stimulus (Vitt and Cooper Jr. 1986; Husak et al. 2006); thus, the variegated coloration related to thanatosis behavior exhibited by T. pusillus makes these scorpions difficult to find in leaf litter. In addition, nocturnal predators (e.g., spider and scorpions) can use other mechanisms for prey detection such as chemical cues and substrate vibration (Brownell and Farley 1979; Persons and Rypstra 2000; Mineo and Del Claro 2006). For these types of predators, prolonged thanatosis is not advantageous, while fleeing to lower leaf litter layers or stinging, as exhibited by T. pusillus, is an effective mechanism to avoid predation. Contrarily, there were no differences in the fleeing response for T. pusillus juveniles in the different diel periods. The lack of differential fleeing behavior could be explained by their smaller size, allowing them to hide in places difficult to access by predators.

Our findings show that the litter-dwelling scorpion, Tityus pusillus, exhibit defensive behavioral plasticity influenced by developmental stage, sex, and diel period. However, differences found in our study between adults and juveniles should be interpreted with caution because of the effect of the perceived size of the stimulus compared to the scorpion body size. Finally, this is the first study revealing the ontogenic differences in defensive mechanisms of scorpions. Also, we observed that males and females perform different behavioral reactions, possibly because of sexual dimorphism or different life strategies.