Introduction

Prolonged sitting and physical inactivity are two predominant risk factors that contribute to obesity, cardiovascular diseases (CVD) (e.g. high blood pressure), type 2 diabetes and impaired cognitive function (Hamilton et al. 2007; Bankoski et al. 2011; Magnon et al. 2018). Due to computer-based work and high reliance of technology, office-based workers spend approximately 65–80% of working hours sitting (Clemes et al. 2014; Waters et al. 2016). Although standing and fidgeting seem to provide applicable approaches to elevate physical activity, the efficacy of using these kinds of low-energy demanding activity are likely to be rather modest (Betts et al. 2019; Koepp et al. 2016). Thus, alternatives and/or more effective strategies to reduce prolonged sitting behaviour and increase physical activity for office workers whilst working are urgently needed.

Replacing sitting time with regular short bouts of walking has been shown as a promising approach to ameliorate detrimental impact of prolonged sitting (e.g. to reduce postprandial glucose and insulin concentrations) and increase physical activity levels in the laboratory (Chen et al. 2018; Loh et al. 2020). However, human behaviours are usually well-controlled within the laboratory setting (i.e. predetermined energy consumption and physical activity) that may impede the application of exercise intervention in the real-world scenario if individuals subsequently show compensatory changes in behaviour (e.g. reduced spontaneous physical activity and/or increases in energy intake) in the (free-living) hours following the trial. In prescribed exercise interventions, the energy deficit elicited can in some cases be less-than-expected, which may be due to the compensatory decreases in physical activity and/or increases in energy intake (Silva et al. 2018; Turner et al. 2010; Melanson et al. 2013; Flack et al. 2018). To date, only two studies have examined breaking prolonged sitting and its influence on human behaviour and only eating behaviour was assessed (Mete et al. 2018; Bailey et al. 2016). These studies found that energy intake was not affected by breaking up sitting (Mete et al. 2018; Bailey et al. 2016), but limited food choices and pre-selected meals in the controlled laboratory period might weaken the ecological relevance to the public. Indeed, studies have demonstrated that built environmental in the free-living condition (public transportation, alternations of work-related activity, high energy density food and more palatable food choices) might lead to the changes in eating and physical activity (Lam et al. 2021). At present, it is still unknown whether predetermined (un)interrupted sitting will induce compensational changes in nutritional intake and physical activity levels once people are in the free-living environment. This has important implications for energy balance and general health.

In addition to the negative effects on the energy balance and metabolic health, prolonged uninterrupted sitting can also impair cognitive function through several mechanisms, including reduced cerebral blood flow (Carter et al. 2018), elevated mental fatigue and lower energy status (Bergouignan et al. 2016; Wennberg et al. 2016), and postprandial hyperglycaemia (Wheeler et al. 2017). Cognitive performance covers several important mental skills, such as working memory, cognitive flexibility and inhibition (i.e. avoiding distractions) that have each been shown to greatly influence work productivity. A recent systematic review has indicated that frequent sitting breaks seems to be able to reverse the negative effects on cognition during prolonged sitting, but the favourable impacts disappeared when relative slow walking speed (e.g. 3.2 km/h) and less frequent sitting breaks (e.g. hourly breaks) were involved (Chueh et al. 2022). Exercise/physical activity intensity plays a moderating role in the regulation cognitive performance (Chang et al. 2012). Therefore, in the present study, walking speed and frequency were both increased, compared to lower intensity and less frequent sitting breaks that have been used previously (Chueh et al. 2022), to examine the effects of breaking up sitting on cognition in the context of an office environment.

Taken together, this study aimed to investigate effects of breaking up prolonged sitting with intermittent brisk walking in healthy individuals on (1) post-trial human behaviours including energy intake and physical activity under free-living conditions and (2) cognitive performance in a simulated workplace environment. We hypothesised that breaking up sitting would reduce post-trial physical activity levels due to prescribed intermittent walking in the laboratory and that better cognitive performance would be observed in the breaking up sitting condition.

Methods

Study participants

Studies have shown that office-based workers, with the average age of 37 years, spend 65% of working hours sitting (Clemes et al. 2014). Accordingly, twenty-four young and middle-aged healthy participants (male = 13) age between 20 and 45 years were recruited in the Taipei city via local advertisements in the current study. Participants were confirmed to be without current and any history of brain injury, neurological disorder, cardiometabolic related diseases, diabetes or cancers and were weight stable (not self-reported weight change ± 3 kg at least 3 months) with a body mass index (BMI) ranging between 18.5 and 27 kg/m2. Once consented to take part, a Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q) was completed to ensure the participants were able to walk on a treadmill without any safety concerns. Smokers, post-menopausal female, and volunteers who used any medications that could influence metabolic and inflammatory responses, were excluded. The participants’ characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Participant characteristics (Mean ± SD)

Experimental design

Participants completed two separate main trials (SIT, uninterrupted prolonged sitting; ACTIVE, interrupted prolonged sitting every 20 min with 2 min intermittent walking) at least a 24-h apart within 7 days in a randomised, crossover design (randomisation was performed by YC-Chen using an online programme, randomizer.org). A pedometer (3D TriSport, China) (Walhin et al. 2018) was worn at least 24 h prior to the first main trial for a continuous period of 7 days. Participants were restricted to any vigorous-intensity exercise, alcohol and caffeine consumption 24 h prior to both SIT and ACTIVE trials. Dinner intake was recorded on the day before the first trial and participants were asked to replicate it the day prior to the second trial. Female participants completed two main trials between 3 and 11 days in their menstrual cycle. The study protocol was approved by Tri-Service General Hospital Institute Review Board (IRB reference number: 2-108-05-151) and is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (ID: NCT05911490). All participants provided written informed consent before taking part.

Studies have shown that prescribed exercise might reduce non-prescribed physical activity (Ridgers et al. 2014; Goran and Poehlman 1992). Therefore, we determined sample size based on the effect of breaking up sitting on energy expenditure in the laboratory. Our previous study demonstrated that breaking up sitting with intermittent walking, compared with prolonged sitting, significantly increased energy expenditure by 216 kcal over a 5.5-h period in 11 participants (Chen et al. 2018). We chose to double the sample size in the present study to increase confidence in observing potential changes in post-trial spontaneous physical activity, along with generating a more representative sample that also enables consideration of the variation of physical activity in different individuals. Twenty-four participants were recruited to allow for expected dropout.

SIT and ACTIVE trials

Participants were required to fast for at least 12-h and minimised their physical activity level before arriving at the exercise physiology laboratory in the National Taiwan Normal University between 0800 and 0900 a.m. on the main trial days. Once anthropometric measurements [including height (Stadiometer JENIX DS-102, Jen An Technology Co., Ltd), weight (InBody 720, Biospace Co. Ltd, Seoul Korea), waist and hip circumference and blood pressure (OMRON HEM-7320, OMRON Co., Ltd., Japan)] completed, participants rested on the chair for 10 min, followed by 10 min expired air sample collections using an automatic gas analysis system (Vmax29, Sensor Medics the Corp., Yorba Linda, CA) to determine resting metabolic rate (RMR) and substrate oxidation (Betts et al. 2011; Compher et al. 2006). The amount of oxygen utilisation (V̇O2) and carbon dioxide production (V̇CO2) were used to calculate carbohydrate and lipid oxidation rates to estimate resting and intermittent walking energy expenditure (Jeukendrup and Wallis 2005; Frayn 1983).

In both SIT and ACTIVE trials, breakfast was consumed after RMR collection, while lunch was provided 3 h afterwards. The test meal was prescribed according to participants’ total body mass (contained 8.8 kcal energy per kilogramme body mass) and the percentage of energy from macronutrients was 77% carbohydrate, 16% fat and 7% protein. The composition of the meal was selected to reflect typical breakfasts in the morning (O'Neil et al. 2014). To avoid the possible interactive effects of energy intake on the major outcomes in this study, the same meal was consumed for lunch. The test meal consisted of oatmeal (Quaker; Taiwan) and fructose (Fong Leng, Taiwan), guava, mango and orange mixed fruit juice (Kung Chuan, Taiwan) and digestive biscuits (McVitie’s, Great Britain). The oatmeal with water and fructose were provided firstly, while juice and biscuits were provided thereafter. Both breakfast and lunch (identical) were consumed within a 10-min period and equal amount of energy was consumed during the SIT and the ACTIVE trials (breakfast and lunch energy intake were 1142 ± 67 kcal, 95% CI [1075, 1209]).

Trial days: laboratory data collection

The experimental protocol was based on our previous publication (Chen et al. 2018) and is illustrated in Fig. 1. In the SIT trial, participants were required to remain seated throughout the whole period. In the ACTIVE trial, after breakfast consumption, participants walked 2 min at 6.4 km/h speed every 20 min on a treadmill (h/p/cosmos mercury® med, Germany) for the following 3 h. After finishing lunch, participants continued to walk in the same pattern for the following 2 h. Apart from walking, participants sat on a chair for the remaining period. In total, participants performed 15 two-min bouts of walking (i.e. totally 30 min over 320 min). Heart rate (Polar M430, Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland) and RPE (Borg 6–20) were collected in the last 30 s of each 2-min bout of walking during the ACTIVE trial. Moreover, two 2-min of expired air samples were collected to estimate walking energy expenditure and substrate utilisation in the ACTIVE trial. In both SIT and ACTIVE trials, cognitive performance was assessed using computerised cognitive tests at the beginning and the end of the trials. Visual analogue scales (VAS) were recorded hourly and blood pressure was measured 3 times at baseline, before lunch and at the end of the trials. Finger pricking whole blood glucose concentrations (Bayer Contour Plus, Germany) were measured in a regular pattern (7 times) during the trials.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental protocol

Participants were only allowed to read, use a laptop, or watch television but were otherwise asked to keep as still as possible (e.g. instructions to avoid fidgeting) while sitting in both SIT and ACTIVE trials. Temperature and humidity were recorded at 0-, 190- and 320-min during trials and there were no differences between different times and trials (both, p > 0.05, data not shown).

Trial days—free-living environment data collection

After data collection was completed in the laboratory, in both trials participants were asked to keep wearing the same pedometer and to record any food and liquid consumption using a weighed food diary for the rest of the day (until midnight, approximately 8.5 h duration) under free-living conditions. No restrictions were placed on free-living energy intake and physical activity outside the laboratory, and participants did not know this was the one of main research outcomes. Participants were required to weigh all the amount of food and liquid consumed with a given digital weight scale up to 0.1 g (I-2000, China).

Cognitive tests

Cognitive performance was determined using Task Switching and Flanker tasks employing Unity software (SF, USA) and familiarisation tests were provided prior to the main trials. Both tasks took approximately 5 min, for the total duration was about 10 min. Task switching test was based on the design of Hung et al. (Hung et al. 2016). In the Task Switching task, a white numeric digit (digits 1–9, excluding 5) was presented in the centre of a computer screen on a black background. Participants had to identify whether the number surrounded by a solid-line rectangle, was smaller (1, 2, 3, 4) or bigger (6, 7, 8, 9) than 5 (i.e. all tasks were categorised as “A” type) and the number surrounded by a dashed square was odd (1, 3, 7, 9) or even (2, 4, 6, 8) (i.e. all tasks were categorised as “B” type) to press the left (number is smaller 5 or odd) or the right (number is larger than 5 or even) button using the index fingers of their left or right hand as fast and as accurately as possible. While the task was combined with an alternative run switching paradigm (i.e. ABBBA…) and repeated twice. The following task was the modified Eriksen Flanker task (Eriksen and Eriksen 1974). Participants were instructed to identify the direction of the middle arrow in either congruent (> >  >  > >) or incongruent (> >  <  > >) stimuli by pressing the left (left-pointing arrow) or the right (right-pointing arrow) button using the index fingers of their left or right hand as fast and as accurately as possible, and repeated the same paradigm for the second round.

Self-perceived sensations assessment

Subjective sensations were measured hourly (6 times in total) using 100-mm VAS for 6 variables including appetite (hunger and satiety), fatigue (fatigue and energy) or mood (tension and relaxation) paired with the opposing sensation (e.g. ‘extremely hungry’ and ‘not hungry at all’). Participants were asked to draw a vertical dash on each line that best matched their feeling at that time. Each score was determined by measuring the distance from the left side of the line (0-mm) to the right (100-mm).

Human behaviours (energy intake and spontaneous physical activity)

Total calorie and macronutrient consumption (carbohydrate, fat and protein) in the free-living environment were carefully analysed and calculated through MyFitnessPal (Teixeira et al. 2018). The number of steps were recorded (from home to laboratory, in the laboratory and in free-living environment) over 24-h period and physical activity energy expenditure (PAEE) was assumed as step counts with stride length of 0.825 m for male and 0.745 m for female (Auvinet et al. 2002) based on an oxygen cost of 0.125 ml/kg/min (Dill 1965).

Statistical analysis

Participant characteristics were presented as means ± standard deviation (SD). Outcomes of variables and variance bars on figures were presented as means with 95% confidence interval (CI). Time series data were analysed using a two-way repeated measures analysis of variance ANOVA (trial [SIT versus ACTIVE] × time) irrespective of minor deviations from a normality of distribution (Maxwell and Delaney 1990) but with Greenhouse–Geisser corrections utilised for intra-individual contrasts where ε < 0.75, and the Huynh–Feldt correction for less severe asphericity (Atkinson 2002) using SPSS version 23 (IBM, Armonk, NY). Where a trial × time interaction was found, post-hoc Holm–Bonferroni corrections were used to characterise differences between trials (Atkinson 2002). Incremental area under the curve (iAUC) for glucose was calculated using the trapezoid method (Narang et al. 2020) and analysed using a paired t-test. Cognitive tests both in accuracy and reaction time were calculated using rate of change ([post-trial—baseline/ baseline] × 100%) and analysed using paired t-test. Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

Results

Physiological responses during the ACTIVE trials in the laboratory

In the ACTIVE trial, during the accumulated 30 min of walking, average heart rate reached 112 (95% CI 107, 118) bpm with an RPE (Borg 6–20 scale) of 10 (95% CI 9.7, 10.7). Estimated 30 min walking energy expenditure was 149 (95% CI 138, 160) kcal and averaged RER was 0.87 (95% CI 0.85, 0.9) via expired air collection.

Steps counts and estimated physical activity energy expenditure on trial days

No differences between trials were shown in step counts and estimated PAEE from home to laboratory (p = 0.892 and p = 0.914, respectively) and in the free-living environment between the end of the trial and midnight (p = 0.416 and p = 0.393, respectively) (Table 2). The step counts and estimated PAEE in the laboratory were higher in ACTIVE compared with SIT (both, p < 0.001), resulting in greater total steps [11,680 (95% CI 10740, 12620) versus 6049 (95% CI 4845, 7253) steps] and estimated PAEE [364 (95% CI 326, 402) versus 186 (95% CI 148, 225) kcal] in the ACTIVE trial compared to the SIT trial (both, p < 0.001) (Table 2).

Table 2 24-h step counts and estimated PAEE between SIT and ACTIVE trials

Post-trial eating behaviour in the SIT and ACTIVE trials

No differences between trials were found in the total energy intake (SIT: 1021, 95% CI [860, 1182] kcal versus ACTIVE: 1132, 95% CI [953, 1311] kcal) and macronutrient consumption (45% versus 46% in carbohydrate, 20% versus 19% in protein and 35% versus 35% in fat between SIT and ACTIVE, respectively) in the post-trial free-living environment (all, p > 0.05) (n = 21, due to 3 participants not completing records).

Blood glucose concentrations

Blood glucose concentrations did not differ at baseline between trials (p = 0.561). There was an interaction effect (time x trial) in blood glucose concentrations between trials (p = 0.038, Fig. 2A). The glucose iAUC value was lower in the ACTIVE trial compared to the SIT trial [7389 (95% CI 5801, 8978) mg/320 min/dL versus 9703 (95% CI 7790,11616) mg/320 min/dL, p = 0.007, Fig. 2B].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Finger-prick blood glucose concentrations (A) and glucose iAUC (B) in the SIT and the ACTIVE trials. Values are presented as means with 95% CI. The shaded box denotes meal time

Visual analogue scale (VAS) for appetite and mood status

All VAS values did not differ at baseline between trials (all, p > 0.063). There was no interaction effect (trial x time) in any of VAS values (all, p > 0.05, Fig. 3A–F). Time effects were showed in both hunger and satiety (both, p < 0.001, Fig. 3A, B). Trial effects were observed indicating greater energy and lower fatigue in the ACTIVE trial compared to the SIT trial (p < 0.001 & p = 0.039, respectively, Fig. 3C, D).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Self-perceived hunger (A), satiety (B), energy (C), fatigue (D), tension (E) and relaxation (F). Values are presented as means with 95% CI. The shaded box denotes meal time

Cognitive function

Neither Task Switching, nor Flanker Task differed at baseline between trials (all, p > 0.308). The rate of percentage changes (%) in accuracy for Task Switching [1.9 (95% CI −2.4, 6.2) versus 1.7 (95% CI −0.8, 4.1)] and Flanker [2.5 (95% CI −1.4, 6.5) versus 1.3 (95% CI −0.3, 2.9)] did not differ between SIT and ACTIVE (all, p > 0.05, Fig. 4A). The rate of percentage changes (%) in reaction time between SIT and ACTIVE trials showed no difference for Task Switching [−7.3 [95% CI −14, −1.1) versus −12 (95% CI −18, −6.5)] and Flanker Task [−1.1 (95% CI −3.7, 1.5) versus 0.6 (95 CI −1.5, 2.7)] (all, p > 0.05, Fig. 4B).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Cognitive function in accuracy rate of change (A) and reaction time rate of change (B). Values are presented as means with 95% CI. N = 23 due to technical issue in one female participant

Blood pressure

There were no differences observed in systolic and diastolic pressure between the SIT and the ACTIVE trials at baseline, 190 min [99 (95% CI 94, 104) versus 99 (95% CI 93, 104) mmHg; 65 (95% CI 62, 68) versus 68 (95% CI 65, 72) mmHg] and 320 min [101 (95% CI 95, 106) versus 101 (95% CI 96, 106) mmHg; 64 (95% CI 61, 67) versus 66 (95% CI 63, 69) mmHg] (all, p > 0.05).

Discussions

In the present study, we investigated the effects of breaking up sitting on human behaviours (energy intake and spontaneous physical activity) and cognitive performance. In the post-trial free-living environment, we found that there were no differences in eating and physical activity behaviours between the SIT and the ACTIVE trials. Therefore, the predetermined breaking up of prolonged sitting with intermittent walking in the ACTIVE trial, elicited greater physical activity levels and created negative energy balance over the 24-h monitoring period, compared to the SIT trial. In the context of office-based conditions, breaking up sitting attenuated postprandial glycaemic iAUC and induced greater self-perceived level of energy, but there were no differences in cognitive performance (accuracy or reaction time) between trials.

In prescribed exercise interventions, the energy deficit (i.e. weight management) elicited in some occasions can be less-than-expected, which may be due to the compensatory behaviours, such as a decrease in physical activity or an increase in energy intake, or the combination of both (Silva et al. 2018; Turner et al. 2010; Melanson et al. 2013; Flack et al. 2018). In the present study, a total of 30 min short bouts of brisk walking did not lead to obvious post-trial behavioural compensation in either energy intake or in physical activity, suggesting that breaking sitting can be an applicable and practical approach to enhance physical activity levels and create negative energy balance for office-based workers over a 24-h period.

The volumes and/or the intensity of previous bouts of exercise and/or physical activity plays a vital role in regulation of the subsequent volitional physical activity (Rowland 1998; Garland et al. 2011). Indeed, studies have shown that prescribed exercise and/or physical activity might spontaneously reduce non-prescribed physical activity (Ridgers et al. 2014; Goran and Poehlman 1992). In the current study, approximately 150 kcal energy was expended accompanied by relatively low self-rated walking intensity (RPE rated at 10 in Borg 6–20 scale) in the ACTIVE trial. Therefore, the volumes and/or the intensity of our breaking sitting protocol may not reach the threshold of inducing behavioural compensation (i.e. decrease in physical activity). In addition, office-based workers do not appear to be more physically active outside the office to compensate for high durations of sedentary behaviour at work (Clemes et al. 2014; Waters et al. 2016). Importantly, our research design (2 min walking every 20 min over 5.5 h) increased step counts by approximately 5,000 steps, resulting in greater physical activity over the 24-h period in the ACTIVE trial. There is a dose–response effect of daily walking steps for health (Jayedi et al. 2022; Saint-Maurice et al. 2020) and each 1000 daily step count increase from baseline has been shown to reduce the risk all-cause mortality (6–36%) and CVD (5–21%) (Hall et al. 2020). Therefore, breaking up prolonged sitting with intermittent walking might have the potential to reduce the risks of CVD related diseases if individuals apply this strategy repeatedly. One limitation that is important to mention is that we do not know the intensity of physical activity and non-exercise activity thermogenesis such as fidgeting, standing and posture changes in all activities of daily living can substantially influence energy expenditure. More sophisticated tools such as accelerometers to precisely capture the intensity of physical activity as well as non-exercise activity thermogenesis are warranted in future investigations. On the other hand, physical activity assessed via pedometer is an easy metric of measurement that help generally quantifying daily activity and which has been well-recognised to be strongly associated with the risk of obesity, CVD and type 2 diabetes in the clinical research (Jayedi et al. 2022; Saint-Maurice et al. 2020).

In terms of eating behaviour, interestingly, similar to ad libitum energy intake assessed in the laboratory (Bailey et al. 2016; Mete et al. 2018), we did not find a significant difference in the eating behaviour in the post-trial free-living environment between the SIT and the ACTIVE trials. Exercise and/or physical activity may also substantially affect subsequent energy intake, in part due to exercise induced energy deficit and/or glycogen depletion and/or changes in appetite (Gonzalez et al. 2019). Given that our participants had breakfast prior to the first bout of walking (i.e. walking in the fed state), alongside the very low energy cost during each bout of walking, insufficient energy storage and glycogen depletion are unlikely to happen. Moreover, in line with previous results (Mete et al. 2018; Bailey et al. 2016), there were no differences in subjective feelings of hunger and satiety between SIT and ACTIVE trials in our study. Taken together, those might be the most likely potential reasons why we did not observe differences of total energy intake in the current study in free-living conditions. One of the strengths in our study was the use of weighed food diaries which allowed participants to choose their food intake without limitations compared with a controlled eating behaviour assessment in the laboratory.

Previous studies have shown that sitting breaks did not acutely improve cognition, which may in part be due to the low intensity and/or infrequent physical activity (Chueh et al. 2022). In this study, we had increased both walking speed and frequency compared to previous studies, but we still did not find an acute effect of breaking sitting on cognition. The underlying properties of study population may influence the effects of breaking up prolonged sitting on cognitive function. One study revealed that cognitive performance could be improved via 3 min walking every 30 min at speed of ~ 6 km/h (ranged between 5.0 and 8.3 km/h) with a total of 27 min physical activity in young sedentary females over 5-h trial (Chrismas et al. 2019). However, another investigation which applied 5 min cycling every 30 min (30 min cycling in total) in young overweight and obese people suggested there was no significant benefit on the cognitive performance (Wanders et al. 2021). Our study design was very similar (2 min walking every 20 min at 6.4 km/h with 30 min walking over 5.5 h period) and we did not find positive effects on cognition in the healthy active young adults. Considering active individuals generally have better cognitive function than inactive people (Falck et al. 2017; Erickson et al. 2019), it is maybe not surprising that breaking sitting with walking had limited ability to further enhance their cognitive function as observed in our data. Moreover, a wide range of walking speed between 5.0 and 8.3 km/h was used (Chrismas et al. 2019), which could be a confounding factor impacting the results. Different cognitive tests used between studies might also lead to conflicting findings despite the overall similar experimental design between studies.

Mood status (Boksem et al. 2005) and glycaemic control have been suggested to influence cognitive function (Wheeler et al. 2017). Consistent with previous studies (Wennberg et al. 2016; Bergouignan et al. 2016; Wanders et al. 2021), we also found that greater self-perceived level of energy and lower postprandial glucose concentrations were present in the ACTIVE trial, but these beneficial effects did not acutely translate into better cognitive performance. It is also possible that this approach (regular breaking sitting) would have to be continuously adhered to over several weeks or months for any positive effects to be observed and this requires further long-term investigation. Notably, interventions with exercise programmes sometimes might be detrimental for the cognition. Breaking up sitting via intermittent fidgeting (Stoner et al. 2019) and acute bouts of high-intensity exercise (Chang et al. 2012) has been observed to impair cognitive functions. Although we did not observe any favourable effects on cognition via intermittent brisk walking, it is important to note that cognitive performance during demanding the tasks were not compromised either.

Finally, postprandial hyperglycaemia is associated with increased potential of developing CVD even for the normoglycaemic individuals (Levitan et al. 2004), and replacing sitting with physical activity may reduce all-cause and CVD mortality risk (Stamatakis et al. 2019). In accordance with previous findings (Chen et al. 2018), we also showed that regular short bouts of intermittent walking reduced postprandial glucose iAUC by approximately 31%. Therefore, our results suggest that breaking prolonged sitting with walking can be a useful strategy to reduce the likelihood for developing to CVD for the office-based workers.

There are potential limitations in the current study. The frequent short bouts of intermittent walking is one of the strengths in our study; however, oxygen uptake might not reach a steady state during a 2-min walking bout. Nevertheless, this would not impact our conclusions on cognition or post-laboratory free-living energy intake and spontaneous physical activity. Moreover, due to safety considerations for the participants, speed of the treadmill was accelerated slowly from 0 to 6.4 km/h (followed by a full 2-min walking at 6.4 km/h); this inevitably means that participants accumulated a small number of additional steps in the laboratory.

Conclusions

Predetermined sitting breaks did not lead to post-trial compensatory changes of human behaviours (i.e. decrease in physical activity and/or increase in energy intake) in the free-living environment. This suggests that breaking up prolonged sitting with short bouts of intermittent walking can be an applicable and effective strategy to enhance 24-h physical activity levels for office-based workers. Moreover, breaking prolonged sitting reduced postprandial glycaemic concentrations and enhanced self-rated energy status but these positive effects did not acutely translate into the improvements in cognitive function in young and middle-aged healthy individuals.