Introduction

Predation is one of the main evolutionary forces in animals (Endler 1986; Lima and Dill 1990). The hunting efficiency of predators usually coevolves with the antipredatory behaviours of the prey (Greene 1988; Vermeij 1994). Nonetheless, the invasion of alien predators imposes new predator-prey interactions among animals that do not share a common evolutionary history (Strauss et al. 2006; Sih et al. 2010; Simberloff et al. 2013). In this scenario, naïve native prey may be unable to recognize predators or respond to them appropriately (Banks and Dickman 2007; Kovacs et al. 2012; Cooper et al. 2014). In addition, sometimes, the alien predator has coevolved with similar prey in its original habitat, hence becoming a highly effective hunter of the naïve prey (Sih et al. 2010), which may result in the extinction of the naïve prey (e.g. Blackburn et al. 2004). However, there are many cases in which naïve prey may recognize and avoid novel predators despite not having a shared evolutionary history (e.g. Strauss et al. 2006; Anson and Dickman 2013; Nunes et al. 2014). Such an ability may be innate and hence fixed by evolution (e.g. Losos et al. 2006; Nunes et al. 2014), or it may be a trait that is caused by predator exposure and subsequent learning to avoid novel predators by the naïve prey (Kelley and Magurran 2003; Griffin 2004; Ferrari et al. 2012). Learning is well documented in fish and has been reported to occur rapidly, only some days after the first visual encounter with the novel predator, and even faster antipredator responses have been recorded with using predator chemical cues (usually by means of chemical alarm signals of conspecifics or heterospecifics; Chivers et al. 1995; Brown et al. 1997).

Many animals are able to recognize predators by chemoreception, which allows potential prey to avoid risky encounters and, often, the costs of fleeing (Kats and Dill 1998; Apfelbach et al. 2005; Cooper et al. 2009). Reptiles have highly developed mechanisms of chemoreception (Schwenk 1995), and several species of lizards have been reported to use them to identify and react to native predators (Thoen et al. 1986; Dial et al. 1989; Webb et al. 2009; Mencía et al. 2016). However, long-term isolations from predators may result in that prey may have lost the ability to recognize predators (Blumstein 2002; Blumstein and Daniel 2005; Cox and Lima 2006).

The Mediterranean island of Ibiza has never been inhabited by snakes (Kotsackis 1981; Alcover 2010) until their recent introduction via olive trees that were used for ornamental gardening in 2003 (Álvarez et al. 2010; Silva-Rocha et al. 2015). Hence, the Ibiza wall lizard, Podarcis pityusensis, first encountered snakes around one decade ago. Three invasive snake species (the horseshoe whip snake, Hemorrhois hippocrepis, the ladder snake, Rhinechis scalaris, and the Montpellier snake, Malpolon mosnpessulanus) are now present on the main island of Ibiza, while coastal islets are still free from snakes (Silva-Rocha et al. 2015). Thus, Ibiza wall lizards from coastal islets have never encountered snakes. We took advantage of this unique situation in order to test if recognition and response to alien predators can be acquired in a naïve prey that lacks a common evolutionary history with their potential predators. To do this, we conducted experiments on chemical recognition of alien snakes by Ibiza wall lizards. We exposed lizards from Ibiza (which have been in contact with snake predators for about one decade) and lizards from the snake-free islet of Sal Rossa to the scents of two of the alien snakes: the horseshoe whip snake and the ladder snake.

In order to study the reaction of insular lizards to the chemical cues of the recent invader species of snakes, we conducted laboratory experiments with different odour treatments. If lizards recognize the chemical cues of the snakes, we would expect that they show significant differences in their behaviours to harmless odours vs. odours from potential predators. Thus, one would expect lizards to react with antipredatory behaviours if they recognize the odour as a threat, whereas no such behaviours would be displayed when encountering harmless odours.

If the ability to recognize the scents of predatory snakes is not innate, we would expect that lizards from Sal Rossa, which have never encountered snake scents before, would not display antipredatory behaviours. These lizards would act normally and similarly to controls and to the scent of snakes. If lizards from Ibiza, which coexist with both types of snake during the last decade, are able to recognize the snake’s scent as a threat, we would expect that they react to the scent with antipredatory behaviour but react normally to the controls. Finally, it is also possible that lizards from Ibiza discriminate and react only to the scent of the predatory snake, the horseshoe whip snake, and react normally to controls and to the scent of the ladder snake.

Materials and methods

Study system

The Ibiza wall lizard is a medium-sized lacertid lizard that inhabits the main islands of Ibiza and Formentera and 42 surrounding islets (Salvador 1984, 2014; Salvador and Pérez-Mellado 1984). On Ibiza, the wall lizards are exposed to several predators: mammals, such as genets (Genetta genetta) and feral cats (Felis silvestris), as well as birds such as kestrels (Falco tinnunculus) and barn owls (Tyto alba; Alcover 1984; Salvador 2014). As mentioned previously, a decade ago, three species of snakes have invaded Ibiza and hence constitute three additional potential predators. Sal Rossa is an islet (<0.5 Ha) that is close to the Southeastern coast of Ibiza, where the only potential predators are birds.

The horseshoe whip snake inhabits the South of the Iberian Peninsula and the North of Tunisia, Algeria and Morocco (Carranza et al. 2005; Silva-Rocha et al. 2015). Adult horseshoe whip snakes are active foraging snakes and feed mainly on mammals and reptiles such as lizards, but their diet also includes birds and invertebrates (Pleguezuelos and Moreno 1990; Pleguezuelos and Fahd 2004; Pleguezuelos and Feriche 2014). The ladder snake inhabits the Iberian Peninsula, the Southeast of France and the Northeast of Italy (Pleguezuelos 1998). Adult ladder snakes are active foragers which feed on mammals and birds and do not depredate lizards (Pleguezuelos 1998; Pleguezuelos et al. 2007). Both species have been recently introduced to Ibiza through ornamental gardening, and they have stable reproductive populations in Ibiza (Pinya and Carretero 2011; Silva-Rocha et al. 2015).

Experimental design

During June of 2014, we conducted three experiments: (1) ‘Ibiza horseshoe whip snake’, to test if lizards from Ibiza respond to the scent of adult horseshoe whip snakes, (2) ‘Ibiza ladder snake’, to test if lizards from Ibiza respond to the scent of adult ladder snakes and (3) ‘Sal Rossa’, to test if lizards from Sal Rossa recognize and respond to the scents of adult horseshoe whip snakes and adult ladder snakes. We used three different sets of 15 adult male lizards, one set for each experiment: 15 adult males from Ibiza for the experiment Ibiza horseshoe whip snake, another 15 males from Ibiza for the experiment Ibiza ladder snake and 15 males from the islet of Sal Rossa for the experiment Sal Rossa. All lizards were captured by noosing and immediately placed into individual cloth bags and transported to the laboratory. The body length of studied lizards is 65.95 ± 5.36 mm (mean snout-vent length (SVL) ± SD), and the mean weight is 7.68 ± 1.64 g. On Ibiza, we captured the lizards in the municipality of Santa Eulària des Riu, within the area of the highest density of the invader snakes (Álvarez et al. 2010). There, we also captured one adult male horseshoe whip snake (SVL = 872 mm) and one adult male ladder snake (SVL = 750 mm). Due to the difficulty in finding snakes, only a single snake was used. Snakes were transported in a different vehicle than lizards in order to avoid any odour mixture.

The experiments were conducted in the research facilities of Sa Casilla (Sant Josep de Sa Talaia, Ibiza). Lizards were kept in individual terraria (40 × 25 × 30 cm) with a substrate of artificial grass and fed daily with crickets and Tenebrio molitor larvae and provided with water ad libitum. Snakes were housed in a different room and kept in individual terraria (50 × 30 × 30 cm) with a substrate of artificial grass and water ad libitum.

Our experimental design was based on previous studies (e.g. Thoen et al. 1986; Van Damme and Quick 2001) and was similar to the one used in Mencía et al. (2016). The experiments consisted of registering the behaviour of lizards in terraria subjected to different chemical cues (odours), i.e. odours from an adult horseshoe whip snake, an adult ladder snake and two controls: an odourless control and a pungent control (cologne) without biological significance for lizards (Cooper et al. 2003). The type of treatments varied in the three experiments. For the experiment Ibiza horseshoe whip snake, we used three different treatments: ‘odourless control’, ‘pungent’ and ‘horseshoe whip snake’. For the experiment Ibiza ladder snake, we used three different experimental terraria: odourless control, pungent and ‘ladder snake’. Finally, for the experiment Sal Rossa, we used four different experimental terraria: odourless control, pungent, horseshoe whip snake and ladder snake.

We placed an absorbent paper on the floor in each of four different experimental terraria (60 × 40 × 40 cm), except for the odourless control. We then allowed three different odours; i.e. horseshoe whip snake, ladder snake and cologne impregnate the absorbent paper. The snakes were placed into the terrarium 24 h before the beginning of the experiment and were placed inside them once again during 40 min between trials. The occlusive plastic cover of the terrarium was closed to avoid odour loss. The snakes were removed from the terrarium 2 min before each experiment trial. We used the absorbent paper in multiple trials with different lizards, cleaning the terrarium and replacing it with new paper each night. Thus, it is possible that lizard alarm odours could be left in the paper, confounding the effect of the chemical cues of the snakes. However, in order to minimize such an effect, we conducted the trials in a random order (i.e. lizards were introduced randomly into the different treatments) and the data was analyzed with repeated measure tests (values of each behavioural variable were tested within individuals).

Each lizard was subjected once to each treatment, resulting in 45 trials (15 lizards × 3 treatments) for each experiment of lizards from Ibiza and 60 trials for the experiment of the lizards from Sal Rossa (15 lizards × 4 treatments). Each lizard was tested once a day within their normal activity period (0800 to 1700 GMT). The experimental room was dark, and only the terrarium was illuminated by a 75-W bulb 50 cm above it, providing homogeneous lighting. We maintained a homogeneous constant temperature of 30 °C in the experimental room in order to avoid possible variations in the behaviour of lizards due to temperature. We drew six equal sectors on the transparent surface of each terrarium in order to count the number of times that lizards moved among sectors. Each trial began by introducing the lizard into the experimental terrarium, closing the terrarium with the occlusive transparent cover in order to avoid scent loss and registering lizard behaviour with a digital recorder for 10 min. Two observers were placed in front of the terrarium, opposite each other: one observer recorded the behavioural variables with binoculars and the other recorded the number of movements and changes among the sectors of the terrarium. Snakes were not fed for the entire duration of all three experiments in order to avoid the potential influence in the lizard’s response to the snakes’ chemical cues due to their diet. It was not possible to record data blindly because terraria were clearly labelled to avoid mistakes. Additionally, the snakes were re-introduced into their terraria after trials. In any case, bias should be low since lizards’ behaviours registered by the two observers. All animals remained healthy throughout the study period. Once we finished each of the three experiments, we released all lizards to their capture sites.

Behavioural variables

Antipredatory vs. normal behavioural patterns are well defined in the lacertid lizard Iberolacerta galani (Mencía et al. 2016). Antipredatory responses include slow movements, jerky movements as tail waving and foot shakes (Thoen et al. 1986; Van Damme et al. 1995; Mencía et al. 2016). By moving less than normal and/or moving in a slow motion, an individual is less likely to be visually detected by the predator (Labra and Niemeyer 2004). Tail waving is another clear antipredatory behaviour related to caudal autotomy of lacertid lizards (Arnold 1984). On the other hand, normal exploratory behaviours of lizards, expected under neutral odours, include walking normally, rubbing the snout of the walls of the terrarium, scratching the walls of the terrarium and/or raising the head (Mencía et al. 2016).

Accordingly, we recorded 16 behavioural variables: (1) ‘walk latency’: time until the first ‘walk’ movement; (2) walk: the lizard walks normally, as it would move in the wild; (3) ‘change among sectors’: the lizard moves between the six predefined sectors of the experimental terrarium; (4) ‘slow’: the lizard walks slowly and with stalking or scattered movements (Thoen et al. 1986; Mencía et al. 2016); (5) tongue flick (TF) latency, i.e. time until the first TF; (6) ‘TF’: number of TFs; (7) ‘snout’: the lizard taps the wall of the terrarium with the snout; (8) ‘rubbing’: the lizard rubs its head against the walls of the terrarium; (9) ‘stand and scratching’: the lizard stands up against the wall of the terrarium and scratches it with its forelegs, as if it were trying to escape; (10) ‘head bob’: the lizard shakes its head up and down; (11) ‘head raise’: the lizard raises the head with its forelimbs straightened; (12) ‘tail waving’: the lizard waves its tail about in a horizontal plane; (13) ‘foot shake’: the lizard moves its forelimbs rapidly up and down; (14) ‘walk time’: total amount of time that the lizard moves normally; (15) ‘slow time’: total amount of time that the lizard moves in a slow motion; and (16) ‘no move’: total amount of time that the lizard stays immobile. The variables were quantified as frequencies, except for walk latency, ‘TF latency’, walk time, slow time and no move, which were quantified in seconds. We started to record the behaviour of each lizard 5 s after placing it in the centre of the experimental terrarium.

Data analysis

We conducted all analyses on R, version 3.1.3 (R Core Team 2015). Because neither the original nor the log-transformed data met the requirements of parametric statistics, we analyzed the data with non-parametric tests. For each experiment, we used the repeated measures of the Friedman test to assess possible differences in the behavioural variables among treatments. When the result of the Friedman test was significant, we performed post hoc multiple comparisons for the Friedman test in order to locate the differences between treatments (Giraudoux 2012).

Results

Experiment Ibiza horseshoe whip snake

All the 15 lizards from Ibiza detected and reacted to the scent of the adult horseshoe whip snake. The values of 11 of the 16 studied behavioural variables were significantly different in the Friedman test of horseshoe whip snake compared to the odourless control and ‘pungent control’ (Table 1). The post hoc tests of Friedman identified the significant differences between the horseshoe whip snake treatment and the odourless control treatment and between the horseshoe whip snake treatment and the pungent treatment (Table 2). For example, the frequency of normal lizard movements was significantly lower containing the scent of the horseshoe whip snake compared to the two controls (Fig. 1). Other behavioural variables, such as the frequency of movements in a slow motion and the frequency of tail waving, only occurred when lizards where exposed the scent of the horseshoe whip snake.

Table 1 Mean (range) values of the 16 behavioural variables and results of the Friedman test (df = 2) in the three treatments (i.e. control no odour, control pungent odour and odour from an adult horseshoe whip snake) in 15 male Podarcis pityusensis from Ibiza
Table 2 Friedman’s post hoc paired comparisons of the Friedman test for the behavioural variables recorded between the two control treatments (no odour and pungent) and the horseshoe whip snake in 15 Podarcis pityusensis of from Ibiza
Fig. 1
figure 1

Boxplots (the line in the middle of the box depicts the median, the box extends from the 25th to the 75th percentiles, and the whiskers depict the 10th and 90th percentiles) of the number of normal movements (variable ‘walk’) of Podarcis pityusensis lizards for each experimental condition recorded during the three experiments

Experiment Ibiza ladder snake

None of the 15 lizards from Ibiza reacted to the chemical cues of the adult ladder snake. All behavioural variables, except for TF latency, showed similar values in the Friedman test for the three treatments: ladder snake, odourless control and pungent (Table 3).

Table 3 Mean (range) values of the 16 behavioural variables and results of the Friedman test (df = 2) in the three treatments (i.e. control no odour, control pungent odour and odour) from an adult ladder snake in 15 male Podarcis pityusensis from Ibiza

Experiment Sal Rossa

None of the 15 lizards from Sal Rossa reacted to the scent of the adult horseshoe whip snake or the adult ladder snake. All behavioural variables showed similar values in the Friedman test for the four treatments: horseshoe whip snake, ladder snake, pungent and odourless control (Table 4).

Table 4 Mean (range) values of the 16 behavioural variables and results of the Friedman test (df = 3) in the four treatments (i.e. control no odour, control pungent odour, odour from an adult horseshoe whip snake and odour) from an adult ladder snake in 15 male Podarcis pityusensis from Sal Rossa

Discussion

The results from the present study show that Ibiza wall lizards from the main island of Ibiza, after approximately one decade of coexistence, recognized and reacted to the scent of the adult horseshoe whip snake by displaying antipredatory behaviours, while ignoring the scent of the adult ladder snake. Importantly, Ibiza wall lizards from Sal Rossa did not recognize or responded to any of the snakes.

Prior to the arrival of humans, birds were the only predator of the Ibiza wall lizard (Alcover 2010). About 5000 years ago, humans arrived and introduced mammalian predators such as genets and feral cats (Brown et al. 2008). However, until 2003, Ibiza has been as snake-free island (Alcover 2010; Silva-Rocha et al. 2015). The present study shows that after less than 15 years of coexistence, Ibiza wall lizards are able of recognizing and displaying antipredator behaviours, such as tail waving and slow motion movements, to the chemical cues of the adult horseshoe whip snake (which frequently feed on lizards), while they ignore the chemical cues of the adult ladder snake. These antipredatory responses are similar to those exhibited by other European lacertid lizards, which have coexisted with predatory snakes for thousands of years (Thoen et al. 1986; Van Damme and Quick 2001; Mencía et al. 2016). However, lizards from the snake-free islet of Sal Rossa ignored the scent of both snakes, behaving similarly to the controls (odourless and pungent odours). Our results show that Ibiza wall lizards have acquired the ability to recognize and respond to the scent of the horseshoe whip snake during this brief period of coexistence. Neophobia, i.e. a general avoidance of novel stimuli, has been shown to be associated with high predation risk (Brown et al. 2013). However, if neophobia was underpinning the lizards’ antipredatory behaviours, they should have responded similarly to the odours of both the snakes and not only to the scent of the horseshoe whip snake, hence making neophobia an unlikely mechanism to strong antipredator response to the adult horseshoe whip snake observed in the lizards from the island of Ibiza. Thus, there are two possible explanations to the rapid adaptation of these lizards to the novel predator: (1) rapid evolution of the observed antipredatory behaviour and/or (2) learning to respond to the scent of the horseshoe whip snake.

Rapid evolution requires a strong predation pressure and background genetic variability on which to act (Nunes et al. 2014). Both requirements seem to apply here. Horseshoe whip snakes prey on similar lacertid lizards in their native habitats of the Iberian Peninsula (Pleguezuelos and Moreno 1990), so they are most likely highly effective at capturing Ibiza wall lizards. As the generation time of Ibiza wall lizards is 1 year, such an evolutionary response would have evolved during the last ten generations of coexistence with horseshoe whip snakes. Losos et al. (2006) reported strong and directional selective pressures elicited by predators within one generation of lizards. Additionally, Nunes et al. (2014) reported that the innate antipredator recognition of tadpoles of the Iberian green frog (Pelophylax perezi) had evolved in only 20 years of predator coexistence. Furthermore, Freeman and Byers (2006) demonstrated that mussels changed in morphology after one decade after the introduction of a novel predator. It is therefore possible that the Ibiza wall lizards have evolved a fixed antipredatory behaviour in the presence of cues from the horseshoe whip snake. However, only a subsequent experiment using captive hatched neonate lizards will be able to elucidate whether the antipredatory behaviours recorded are an innate response to this novel predator.

On the other hand, several species have been shown to rely on learning to recognize and respond to novel predators (Ferrari et al. 2015). For example, introduction of northern pike (Esox lucius) into a pond previously only inhabited by fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) resulted in that the minnows displayed antipredatory behaviours to pike odours only 3 days after the introduction (Brown et al. 1997). Learning only requires one successful encounter with the predator, enabling the surviving naïve prey to rapidly recognize chemical and/or visual cues of the predator (Griffin 2004; Ferrari et al. 2010; Brown et al. 2011; Crane and Ferrari 2013). Moreover, wall lizards (Podarcis muralis) have been shown to combine both chemical and visual cues to detect snake predators (Amo et al. 2004); it is therefore possible that visual cues may have reinforced the Ibiza wall lizards’ antipredatory learning response to horseshoe whip snakes.

Ibiza wall lizards have been subjected to a continuous high predation pressure from several predatory birds and human-introduced mammals over many thousands of years (Pérez-Mellado et al. 1997; Cooper and Pérez-Mellado 2012) as revealed by their high autotomic capacity (Pérez-Mellado et al. 1997) and long flight initiation distance (Cooper and Pérez-Mellado 2012). We therefore propose that previous high-level predation has further enabled the lizards to maintain the ability to learn how to rapidly recognize and respond to new predators. This ability could potentially save Ibiza wall lizards from possible significant population decline or extinction caused by the alien snake invaders. However, as mentioned previously, further experiments have to be conducted in order to elucidate which of the two antipredatory mechanisms, i.e. innate or learned, being the major underpinning of the strong antipredator responses displayed by the wall lizards from the island of Ibiza to a novel snake predator, the horseshoe whip snake.