Introduction

Social cognition is an important aspect of human emotional competence and is crucial for adequate social interaction. Social cognition is altered in stressful situations through activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and consecutive increased cortisol secretion (Sandi and Haller 2015).

One important aspect of social cognition is selective attention after emotional priming (attentional bias), favoring the cognitive processing of emotional information (MacLeod et al. 1986). Some studies have focused on the role of cortisol on attentional bias toward emotional cues suggesting that cortisol is associated with an attentional bias toward negative stimuli (Putman and Roelofs 2011; Roelofs et al. 2007; Tsumura and Shimada 2012; van Honk et al. 1998). However, McHugh et al. (2010) found an attenuated attentional bias toward threatening stimuli after stress induction.

The ability to recognize emotions in facial expressions is another important subdomain of social cognition. Deckers et al. (2015) found improved emotion recognition after psychosocial stress (Trier Social Stress Test) in patients with borderline personality disorder and in healthy participants. Duesenberg et al. (2016) have investigated the role of the glucocorticoid receptor agonist hydrocortisone on emotion recognition in healthy subjects but did not find a hydrocortisone effect on emotion recognition.

In the brain, cortisol binds at two receptors, the mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) and the glucocorticoid receptor (GR). Whereas the GR is abundant throughout the brain, the MR is mainly distributed within limbic structures, especially in the hippocampus, the lateral septum, and the amygdala (de Kloet 2014). Previously, GR function attracted most research attention, but more recently, studies have also begun to emphasize the role of the MR (Joëls et al. 2008). For example, animal studies have shown that the MR is important for behavioral regulation in emotionally charged situations. The MR has been linked to fear memory (Brinks et al. 2009), to retrieval of emotional information and fear response selection (Zhou et al. 2011), to reactivity in unfamiliar situations (de Kloet et al. 1999), to the extinction of fear-motivated behavior (Ter Horst et al. 2012), and to reduced anxiety (Lai et al. 2007; Mitra et al. 2009; Rozeboom et al. 2007). In humans, emotional memory is impaired by MR blockage (Rimmele et al. 2013) and by MR depletion via metyprapone (Rimmele et al. 2015). In addition, Wingenfeld et al. (2014) have shown that the MR agonist fludrocortisone improves emotional empathy in patients with borderline personality disorder and in healthy women. However, no study up to now has examined the role of MR stimulation on selective attention toward emotional cues and on emotion recognition.

Given the above findings, we decided to examine the effects of fludrocortisone on attentional bias and emotion recognition. As cortisol—endogenously released through psychosocial stress induction or exogenously by hydrocortisone administration—stimulates both receptors, the GR and the MR, the aim of the current study is to investigate the effects of selective MR stimulation on important aspects of social cognition, namely selective attention and emotion recognition. In accordance with research stressing the role of the MR for “fight, fright or flight” behavioral adaptation with regard to selective attention (de Kloet 2014), we hypothesize an attentional bias toward negative stimuli after fludrocortisone intake. This hypothesis is supported by the work of Tsumura and Shimada (2012) and Roelofs et al. (2007) who found an increased attentional bias toward negative stimuli in the early phase of stress response, when MR activity is most pronounced. We do not hypothesize an attentional bias toward positive stimuli, since happy facial expression does not imply hazard, threat, peril, or distress.

Since, in a previous study, we found improved emotional empathy after fludrocortisone administration in healthy participants and controls (Wingenfeld et al. 2014), it was promising to examine whether the general ability to recognize the emotions from facial expressions is improved after fludrocortisone intake. The recognition of emotions from facial expression is a necessary prerequisite for empathy. Given the results on the positive effects of MR stimulation on emotional empathy (Wingenfeld et al. 2014), we further hypothesize that fludrocortisone administration will facilitate emotion recognition.

Since there are heterogeneous results on sex differences with regard to selective attention (Breitberg et al. 2013; Carr et al. 2016; McHugh et al. 2010) and emotion recognition (Duesenberg et al. 2016; Hoffmann et al. 2010; Montagne et al. 2005; Thompson and Voyer 2014), it was promising to examine potential sex differences in an explorative manner. There are previous findings showing that women are more reactive to emotional stimuli than men (Bradley et al. 2001; Cahill 2006). Moreover, there is increasing evidence for sex-dependent effects of the MR (ter Heegde et al. 2015). Therefore, we hypothesize that there are sex differences and that they have an influence as moderator between the association of MR stimulation and selective attention as well as emotion recognition.

Methods and materials

Participants

We recruited 40 healthy women and 40 healthy men. All participants had a university-entrance diploma (German Abitur), were currently enrolled at university, and between 18 and 30 years of age. The mean age was 23.9 years (SD = 3.3). They were recruited through local advertisements such as postings at hospitals, universities, other public places, and on Web sites. Past research has indicated that the sample size of 80 participants is of adequate power for our purpose (Duesenberg et al. 2016). We have conducted post hoc power analysis to confirm this assumption and underpin our stopping rule of data collection.

All participants were medication-free. Exclusion criteria comprised central nervous system diseases, severe somatic diseases, previous traumatic brain injury, malignant tumors, HIV infection, symptomatic cardiac arrhythmias as well as myocardial infarction, hypertension, autoimmune diseases, metabolic or endocrine disease like type I and type II diabetes mellitus, ingestion of oral and inhalative glucocorticoids (all cortisol containing compounds), current infections, allergies, and pregnancy. All potential participants were screened for previous or current axis I disorders according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—DSM-IV (Wittchen and Fydrich 1997), and were excluded if any psychiatric disorder was present. Due to the influence of the female cycle on the HPA-axis, all women not using oral contraceptives were tested in the luteal phase (Gaffey et al. 2014; Kirschbaum et al. 1999). The Ethics Committee of the German Psychology Association (DGPs) approved this study, and it is in line with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Participants signed a written informed consent before participation.

Procedure

This is a double-blind, placebo-controlled randomized controlled study. In this between-group design, participants received either 4 pills of 0.1 mg fludrocortisone (Florinef®) or 4 identical looking placebo pills. Due to the circadian rhythm of cortisol (Edwards et al. 2001; Kirschbaum and Hellhammer 1989), fludrocortisone or placebo was administered in the afternoon at 1 p.m. The testing started 2 h after drug intake (3 p.m.) at maximum plasma concentrations of fludrocortisone (Ribot et al. 2013). The testing was conducted in a quiet surrounding. The participants followed rules of conduct that prompted participants not to do sports, not to smoke, eat or drink alcoholic or caffeinated beverages within 1 h prior to testing (12 p.m.–1 p.m.). During the 2 h after drug intake until the testing, the participants were sitting in a quite room, where they could only drink water and read.

Salivary cortisol was collected five times using Salivettes® (blue cap, Sarstedt, Germany): 0 (baseline—immediately before fludrocortisone or placebo intake), 90, 120, 180, and 210 min. Salivary cortisol samples were retained at −80 °C (−112 °F) until biochemical analysis. Systolic and diastolic blood pressure as well as heart rate were measured at the same time points. Furthermore, the current psychological state of participants was quantified immediately before testing with the short version of the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (MDBF), i.e., Mehrdimensionaler Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (Steyer et al. 1997). The MDBF is divided into three subscales: mood, vigilance-fatigue, and composure-restlessness. The total score for each subscale of the MDBF ranges between 4 and 20 and every subscale has 4 items. Moreover, we used the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) to measure potential depressive symptoms during the past 2 weeks (Beck et al. 1996).

Hormonal assessment

Cortisol levels were analyzed using an adapted homogenous time-resolved fluorescence resonance energy transfer (HTR-FRET)-based competitive immunoassay. The analysis was conducted in the Neurobiology Laboratory of the Department of Psychiatry, Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin. Intra-assay coefficients of variation were below 8 %; inter-assay coefficients of variation were below 10 %. All samples and standards were measured in duplicate, and the limit of detection was 0.2 nmol/L. For a detailed description of the method that was used for hormonal assessment, see Duesenberg et al. (2016).

Assessment of social cognition

We confined the assessment of social cognition to its emotional aspects as captured by the emotional dot-probe task and the facial emotion recognition task.

Emotional dot-probe

The emotional dot-probe is a computer-based, emotional version of the visual dot-probe paradigm. It is a measure of selective attention toward emotional cues and takes for about 15 min (MacLeod et al. 1986). We used a set of human faces from the FACES database (Ebner et al. 2010), comprising 20 persons (10 female and 10 male) illustrating happy, sad, and neutral facial expressions. As stimuli, two pictures of human faces are quickly presented on a computer screen (for 500 ms). One picture set always consists of two facial expressions of the same person, either paired as neutral-sad, neutral-happy, or neutral-neutral. Afterward, either the left or the right picture is replaced by a vertical bar as the cue (for 1100 ms). The participants are instructed to press one of two keys (left vs right) as quickly as possible in reaction to the position of the cue. The attentional capture is reflected in the response latency. It is assumed that participants react quicker when the cue replaces the picture at which they antecedently concentrated their attention on. The condition in which the cue replaces the location on the screen where the emotional stimulus was placed is called “congruence” (Fig. 1). The condition in which the probe replaces the picture on the opposite side (neutral facial expression) is called “incongruence” (Fig. 1). The neutral-neutral condition was used as control condition to measure the baseline reaction time.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the emotional dot-probe task—on the left side: procedure of a congruent trial, i.e., the probe follows at the location of the emotional facial expression (happy face). On the right side: procedure of an incongruent trial, i.e., the probe follows at the opposite side (neutral face) of the emotional facial expression

Overall, each participant completes 200 trials, i.e., 40 trials in the neutral condition, 40 trials in the positive congruent condition, 40 trials in the negative congruent condition, 40 trials in the positive incongruent condition, and 40 trials in the negative incongruent condition. The position of the facial pictures is counterbalanced between left and right position, and the order between all trials is quasi-randomized.

The “attentional bias index” can be determined by calculating the average reaction time for incongruence minus congruence (MacLeod and Mathews 1988; Tsumura and Shimada 2012):

$$ \begin{array}{l}\mathrm{Attentional}\ \mathrm{bias}\ \mathrm{index}=\frac{1}{2}\left[\left(\mathrm{incongruence}\ \mathrm{right}\hbox{--} \mathrm{congruence}\ \mathrm{right}\right)+\right.\left(\mathrm{incongruence}\ \mathrm{left}\hbox{--} \right.\hfill \\ {}\left.\mathrm{congruence}\ \mathrm{left}\Big)\right].\hfill \end{array} $$

A positive attentional bias index score can be interpreted as an attentional bias toward the emotional stimulus (i.e., orientation toward the happy or sad faces), and a negative score as an attentional bias toward the neutral stimulus (i.e., avoiding the sad or happy faces).

Facial emotion recognition task

The facial emotion recognition task is a computer-based test, which measures the ability to recognize facially expressed emotions. Pictures of human faces were presented for 1 s, expressing an emotion (sad, neutral, or angry) followed by a gray screen (for 4 s). The participants are asked to classify the emotion by selecting one of three possible answers (sadness, neutral, or anger) by pressing the respective key on the keyboard (arrow keys). The two basic emotions, anger and sadness, were used as emotion recognition task, and the neutral stimuli were used as control trial. Facial expressions of the emotion anger and sadness were morphed into four emotional intensities (20, 30, 40, and 80 %) based on the full (100 %) facial expression of the emotion (Fig. 2). For each emotional valence (sadness and anger), 24 trials per intensity (20, 30, 40, and 80 %) and 24 control trials (neutral–0 % emotion) were shown. In sum, each participant was asked to classify 216 pictures of facial expressions in randomized order. Both correct answers and errors (false classification of emotion) were recorded. The facial expressions (6 male and 6 female faces) were taken from the NIMSTIM scale (Tottenham et al. 2009). The facial emotion recognition task lasted for about 15 min.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Examples from the facial emotion recognition task—the examples of facially expressed emotions (anger and sadness) were presented in four intensities ranging from low task difficulty (80 %) to increasingly higher task difficulties (40, 30, and 20 %)

Statistical analyses

SPSS version 22.0 was used for all statistical analyses. χ 2 tests were used for categorical data and Student’s t tests for continuous data in order to analyze demographic differences between the treatment groups (fludrocortisone vs placebo).

Moreover, we used a 2 × 2 ANOVA to analyze the emotional dot-probe with valence (attentional bias index for sad faces and for happy faces) as within-subject factor and treatment (fludrocortisone vs placebo) as between-subject factor.

We used a 2 × 2 × 4 ANOVA to analyze the facial emotion recognition task with intensity (20, 30, 40, and 80 %) and valence (anger vs sadness) as within-subject factors and treatment (fludrocortisone vs placebo) as between-subject factor.

RmANOVAs with time (0, 90, 120, 180, and 210 min after drug intake) as within-subject factor and treatment (fludrocortisone vs. placebo) as between-subject factor were used to analyze salivary cortisol, blood pressure, and heart rate.

Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied when indicated. Post hoc tests (ANOVA and t tests) were performed in event of a significant interaction. For ANOVA, partial η 2 was used as effect size and for t tests, Cohen’s d, respectively.

Results

Demographic characteristics

There were no significant differences in the treatment groups (fludrocortisone vs placebo) regarding age, body mass index, smoking, and psychological state immediately prior to testing. Female participants who took fludrocortisone and female participants who took placebo did not differ with respect to the intake of oral contraceptives. Demographic characteristics of our sample are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic variables

Salivary cortisol secretion during testing

In order to minimize the influence of potential outliers, we excluded participant’s salivary cortisol values from statistical analyses if they deviated ±2 SD from the mean for more than 50 % of the time (i.e., at least in 3 of 5 salivary cortisol values). Therefore, salivary cortisol values from 4 participants (3 from the fludrocortisone group and 1 from the placebo group) were excluded. Due to significant baseline differences between the fludrocortisone and placebo group (t(74) = −2.55, p = .013; d = 0.6), we performed a baseline correction by subtracting the baseline value at 0 min from the subsequent values at 90, 120, 180, and 210 min after drug/placebo intake. RmANOVA with time (0, 90, 120, 180, and 210 min after drug intake) as within-subject factor and treatment as between-subject factor revealed no significant main effect of treatment. There was a significant main effect of time (F(4, 71) = 28.31, p < .001; η p 2 = .29) and a significant time × treatment interaction (F(4, 71) = 9.71, p < .001; η p 2 = .12). Post hoc tests revealed significant differences between the cortisol values of the placebo and the fludrocortisone group at 180 min (t(71) = 2.03, p = .047) and 210 min (t(72) = 2.95, p = .004) after drug intake. Cortisol delta values (i.e., baseline value at 0 min minus the last value 210 min after drug intake) were calculated, and we found significant differences between the placebo and the fludrocortisone group (t(73) = −2.96, p = .004; d = 0.7), indicating greater cortisol suppression after fludrocortisone intake compared to placebo (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The course of salivary cortisol levels—shown are salivary cortisol values (nmol/l) after fludrocortisone and placebo intake after baseline correction. Error bars represent standard errors. The duration of the facial emotion recognition task (FER) and the emotional dot-probe task (EDP) are marked. Both lasted for about 15 min

Blood pressure and heart rate

We found no significant treatment effect on systolic and diastolic blood pressure. There was a significant main effect of time on systolic (F(4, 78) = 13.70, p < .001; η p 2 = .15) and diastolic blood pressure (F(4, 78) = 5.68, p < .001; η p 2 = .07), indicating a decrease over time. However, we found no significant time × treatment interaction in systolic and diastolic blood pressure.

Regarding the heart rate, there was no significant main effect of treatment. We found a significant effect of time (F(4, 78) = 21.54, p < .001, η p 2 = .22), indicating a decrease in heart rate over time. We found no significant time × treatment interaction.

The data of systolic and diastolic blood pressure as well as heart rate were summarized in Table 2.

Table 2 Blood pressure, heart rate, and salivary cortisol

Emotional dot-probe task—attentional bias toward emotional cues

We excluded reaction times from statistical analyses that were less than 100 ms (anticipation error) and greater than 1500 ms (lack of concentration) in order to minimize the influence of outliers. We only analyzed response latencies from correct responses. Loss of data was less than 2 %.

2 × 2 ANOVA revealed a significant treatment effect (F(1, 78) = 4.18, p = .04, η p 2 = .05). There was no significant main effect of valence. However, we found a significant interaction of treatment × valence (F(1, 78) = 6.20, p = .02, η p 2 = .07).

To disentangle the interaction of valence × treatment, we performed separate ANOVAs for attentional bias indices for sad and happy faces, respectively. Univariate ANOVA revealed a significant treatment effect (F(1, 76) = 9.39, p = .003; η p 2 = .11) for sad faces, indicating an attentional bias away from sad faces after placebo intake and a shift in selective attention toward sad faces compared to placebo (see Fig. 4). There was no significant treatment effect for facial expressions of happiness. To examine whether the attentional bias scores differ significantly from zero, we used one sample t test and found an attentional bias away from sad faces in the placebo group (t(39) = −2.9, p = .006), but not toward sad faces in the fludrocortisone group (t(39) = 1.579, p = .12). Respectively, for happy faces, there were no significant differences after placebo intake (t(39) = .267, p = .791), nor after fludrocortisone intake (t(39) = .443, p = .660).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Attentional bias index for the valences sadness and happiness—the attentional bias away from sad faces in the placebo group and the shift in selective attention toward sad faces compared to placebo is shown on the left. The lack of a significant attentional bias toward happy faces is shown on the right. Error bars represent standard errors

Furthermore, we examined whether the attentional bias index differs between sad and happy faces in both groups and found significant differences between the attentional bias index of sad and happy faces in the placebo group (t(39) = −2.38, p = .022), but not in the fludrocortisone group (t(39) = 1.11, p = .27).

There was no significant difference between the placebo and the fludrocortisone group regarding the baseline reaction time, measured by the neutral-neutral condition (t(78) = −.244, p = .808).

There were no differences between women and men in the emotional dot-probe task.

Correlation between salivary cortisol and attentional bias to emotional cues

There was no significant correlation between cortisol suppression after fludrocortisone intake and attentional bias index for sad faces (r = −.117, p = .472) or for happy faces (r = .027, p = .869).

Moreover, there were no significant correlations between the raw data for cortisol at each time point and the attentional bias index for sad faces. The same was true for the attentional bias index for happy faces.

Facial emotion recognition task

The 2 × 2 × 4 ANOVA revealed no significant treatment effect. There was a significant main effect of valence (F(1, 78) = 55.82, p < .001; η p 2 = .42), i.e., the number of correct answers was lower in anger than in sadness. As expected, there was a significant main effect of intensity (F(3, 234) = 1501.12, p < .001; η p 2 = .95); the higher the intensity of emotional expression, the higher the accuracy of recognition.

There was a significant interaction of treatment × intensity (F(3, 234) = 3.15, p = .025; η p 2 = .04). However, post hoc t test revealed no significant differences between the placebo and the fludrocortisone group.

Furthermore, the 2 × 2 × 4 ANOVA revealed a significant valence × intensity interaction (F(3, 234) = 12.20, p < .001; η p 2 = .14). Post hoc t test revealed significant differences between anger and sadness, i.e., the number of correct answers was lower in anger than in sadness when the intensity was 20 % (t(79) = −.7.58, p < .001; d = 1.69), 30 % (t(79) = −6.72, p < .001; d = 1.50), or 40 % (t(79) = −6.23, p < .001; d = 1.39), but not when the intensity was 80 %. All other interactions were not significant (p > .05).

Women and men did not differ in the facial emotion recognition task.

Discussion

We examined the effect of the MR agonist fludrocortisone on attentional bias toward emotional cues and on emotion recognition in young healthy subjects. As predicted, fludrocortisone administration increased selective attention toward negative emotional cues. By contrast, fludrocortisone did not affect attentional bias toward happy faces and did not improve overall emotion recognition accuracy in our healthy, young, high-educated sample.

Emotional dot-probe task

Our results of significantly increased selective attention toward negative emotional cues after MR stimulation are in line with Tsumura and Shimada (2012), who found evidence for an attentional bias toward sadness- and depression-related words immediately after a psychosocial stressor, which may well reflect the early and rapid effects of MR activation (Joëls et al. 2012; Oitzl et al. 2010; Tsumura and Shimada 2012). Moreover, Roelofs et al. (2007) have found that psychosocial stress-induced cortisol release is associated with an increased attentional bias toward angry faces, but not toward happy faces by using the masked emotional Stroop task. Again, this effect was measured shortly after stress induction and may, therefore, also be linked to the rapid non-genomic effects of MR occupation (Joëls et al. 2012; Oitzl et al. 2010). In contrast, McHugh et al. (2010) report decreased attentional bias toward threatening stimuli 30 min following a psychosocial stressor. This may further indicate that the attentional bias is to be found in the early phase (within seconds to minutes) of stress response, where the activation of the noradrenergic system and the rapid (selective) MR effects play a crucial role. Since the MR agonist fludrocortisone leads to a shift in selective attention toward sad faces compared to placebo, our findings substantiate the interpretation of Tsumura and Shimada (2012) that rapid MR stimulation may play a critical role on increased selective attention toward negative emotional cues. These results are consistent with the well-established hypothesis that emotional information processing is improved in the early phase (within seconds to minutes) of cortisol response (de Kloet 2014; Oitzl et al. 2010).

The MR seems to modulate selective attention in a valence-specific way. Especially those studies investigating the early effects of stress revealed an enhanced selective attention to negative stimuli, which was also the case in the current study. The emotional valence of sadness, unlike happiness, is closely linked to fear, threat, and anger as being an expression of aversive and negative, psychosocial stress-associated stimuli. This is consistent with evidence that the MR is crucial for stress-associated emotional arousal and adaptive behavior (Brinks et al. 2007), as well as for the appraisal of new, unknown situations (De Kloet 2013) and for the rapid selection of response strategies (Vogel et al. 2016). This is biologically plausible, because the hippocampus, the lateral septum, and the amygdala, where the MR is highly expressed (de Kloet 2014), are closely related to the processing of emotional information (Groeneweg et al. 2012; Joëls et al. 2011), and because the lateral septum and the amygdala are associated with effective stress coping (Singewald et al. 2011) and emotion (Rolls 2015; Sheehan et al. 2004). The attentional bias away from sad faces, which was found in our placebo group, is in line with previous studies on healthy subjects (e.g., Joormann and Gotlib 2007).

Facial emotion recognition task

Fludrocortisone did not improve overall emotion recognition accuracy in our study. A post hoc power analysis with α = 0.05 and η p 2 = 0.04 revealed a sufficient power of 0.99, i.e., only 54 participants were needed to detect the interaction of treatment × intensity that we found in our study (Faul et al. 2007). Therefore, the hypothesis that an inadequate test power is the reason of the non-significant treatment effect appears unlikely.

However, we did find that it was more difficult to recognize anger correctly than sadness, if the emotion was not very salient and task difficulty was high, i.e., for emotional intensities of 20, 30, and 40 %, but not for 80 %. As one would expect, the recognition accuracy was better when the emotion was vivid and well-marked, i.e., task difficulty was low (80 % emotional intensity) and the recognition accuracy was worse when task difficulty was high (20, 30, and 40 % emotional intensity).

In a recent study, Deckers et al. (2015) found that healthy controls were significantly better in emotion recognition following the Trier Social Stress Test compared to baseline recognition accuracy before psychosocial stress. In contrast, fludrocortisone intake did not have this effect in our sample of young healthy subjects. Therefore, it seems unlikely that MR activation alone explains the findings of Deckers et al. (2015). Since the results of Duesenberg et al. (2016) suggest that GR is also not directly modulating emotion recognition, it is necessary to examine the role of other hormones, for example adrenaline or noradrenaline, that are released in response to stress and might alter emotion recognition. However, Deckers et al. (2015) had no non-stress control group, and they used a repeated measurement design. Therefore, a practice effect cannot be excluded as an explanation of their results.

Sex differences

Contrary to our hypothesis, we failed to find a significant main effect of sex for the emotional dot-probe task and the facial emotion recognition task. These results do not fit to previous studies that found differences between men and women in the facial emotion recognition task (Duesenberg et al. 2016; Hoffmann et al. 2010; Montagne et al. 2005; Thompson and Voyer 2014). Regarding selective attention, there are only few and more heterogeneous results. Most of the studies did not report any sex effects (Roelofs et al. 2007; Tsumura and Shimada 2012), or examined only men (Putman et al. 2007, 2010; van Honk et al. 2000). In contrast to our results, Breitberg et al. (2013) found a dose-dependent reduction of an attentional bias toward happy faces in women, but not in men. Furthermore, McHugh et al. (2010) found a higher association between acute changes in cortisol levels after a psychological stressor and changes in a negative attentional bias in women than in men. However, comparable with Taylor et al. (2011) and van Honk et al. (1998), we found no significant sex differences.

Salivary cortisol secretion

We used fludrocortisone, because it is known to activate the MR even better than physiological aldosterone and because the affinity of the MR to fludrocortisone is about 15 times higher than the affinity of the GR (Agarwal et al. 1977; Otte et al. 2003). As expected, we found a decrease of salivary cortisol levels over time after fludrocortisone intake compared to placebo intake in our sample (baseline corrected salivary cortisol values). This fits very well to the known inhibitory effect of MR stimulation on the HPA-axis, even in non-stress conditions (Buckley et al. 2007; Karamouzis et al. 2013; Otte et al. 2003). Moreover, there were significantly higher absolute cortisol levels (raw data) in the fludrocortisone group compared to the placebo group at 0, +90, and +120 min, but not at +180 and +210 min. In spite of this drop of salivary cortisol levels taking place between +120 and +180 min, there is a limitation of our study in this regard. Since testing took place between +150 to +180 min, it would have been best to measure salivary cortisol also at +150 min. This would have made it even clearer that absolute cortisol levels had no influence on our outcome variables with regard to the time frame between +150 to +180 min. Nonetheless, we found no significant correlations between the absolute data of cortisol at each time point and the attentional bias index.

Strength and weaknesses

Our study has several strengths: it is a well-controlled study and it is the first study that examines the effects of fludrocortisone on attentional bias and emotion recognition. Moreover, we balanced female and male participants at the outset in order to analyze potential sex effects and we had a relatively large sample size. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were clearly stated, and since the participants had to pursue rules of conduct, potential confounding effects could be minimized.

One limitation of our study is the restriction on anger and sadness as the sole emotional valences in the facial emotion recognition task and likewise happiness and sadness in the emotional dot-probe. It might have been revealing to include different emotions. Moreover, we did not measure plasma concentration of fludrocortisone.

Since we only included young and well-educated participants, our results cannot be generalized to other populations. Furthermore, fludrocortisone has at least some GR potency, although its affinity to the MR is about 15 times higher than its affinity to the GR (Agarwal et al. 1977). Therefore, a potential influence of GR activity cannot be completely excluded.

Future studies may further examine the role of mineralocorticoid receptor stimulation on attentional bias toward emotional cues and may also include additional emotions like fear and anger. Findings have demonstrated that genetic variances, haplotypes, and polymorphisms of the MR gene (NR3C2) moderate aspects of affective processing like amygdala reactivity after emotional childhood neglect (Bogdan et al. 2012) and elevated optimism as a protective factor for depression in women (Klok et al. 2011). Even more relevant, Vogel et al. (2014) have linked variances of NR3C2 with memory bias for negative emotion. Therefore, it might be very interesting to systematically explore the differential role of NR3C2 variances on affective processing, especially in reactivity-, withdrawal-, and social threat-related emotions like sadness, anger, fear, or disgust and to examine attentional bias in particular.

Due to our homogeneous sample characteristics, it would be enlightening to compare the results of these young, healthy, and high-educated subjects with other samples as well as in clinical samples, e.g., in depression. It is also important to investigate whether the observed fludrocortisone-related effect on attentional bias toward sad emotional cues is dose-dependent and to assess its exact timing.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first study examining the effects of MR stimulation on the emotional dot-probe task and the facial emotion recognition task. We found a significant shift in selective attention toward sad faces compared to placebo, but no selective attention toward happy faces after fludrocortisone intake. This extends the existing literature that MR function plays a crucial role in quick and automatic behavioral adaption in response to emotional-charged situations that typically arise from stressful social interaction.