Introduction

Brief episodes of social stress can escalate drug self-administration in humans and rats (Boyson et al. 2014; Covington and Miczek 2005; Sinha 2009). A large percentage of adolescent humans (10–30 %) are victims of social stress (Nansel et al. 2001; Newman et al. 2005), which is associated with increased illicit substance use (Hoffmann et al. 2000; Tharp-Taylor et al. 2009). Our recent study modeled this connection between stress and drug self-administration by applying social defeat stress to adolescent male rats from postnatal day (P) 35–44 and then measuring several features cocaine self-administration in early adulthood (P65–85). After brief episodes of social defeat during mid-adolescence, rats acquired intravenous cocaine self-administration faster, attained a greater number of cocaine infusions under a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement (PR), and escalated cocaine intake during a 24-h continuous access binge later on in adulthood (Burke and Miczek 2015). Past studies also implicate the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system in adolescent social defeat cross-sensitization to psychostimulants (Burke et al. 2013; Burke and Miczek 2014; Burke et al. 2010; Burke et al. 2011). These effects were only observed when the rats were allowed social experiences during adolescence. The neural mechanisms underlying the cross-sensitization between adolescent stress and adult drug-seeking behavior remain largely elusive but may involve neuropeptides such as corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) (Brielmaier et al. 2012; Nader et al. 2012).

CRF not only plays a neuroendocrine regulatory role in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) stress response but also is involved in stress reactivity through action in extrahypothalamic brain sites (Heinrichs et al. 1992) and in drug reward (Koob and Zorrilla 2010). Our recent work implicates the CRF system in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in neuroadaptations that are induced during episodes of adult social defeat to subsequent cocaine self-administration, cocaine-stimulated motor activity, and cocaine-stimulated dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens shell (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). The corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 receptor (CRF-R1) antagonist CP395376 is a highly selective antagonist for CRF-R1 based on previous in vivo studies (Chen et al. 2008; Schulz et al. 1996). Microinjections of CP395376 prior to each social defeat dose-dependently prevented the subsequent escalation of cocaine intake during a 24-h binge in singly housed adult rats (Boyson et al. 2014).

The extrahypothalamic CRF system has been investigated mostly in the extended amygdala during adolescent development (Eghbal-Ahmadi et al. 1998; Viau et al. 2005; Weathington and Cooke 2012) but not in the adolescent VTA. Stressors such as exposure to fox odor or placement on an elevated platform, during adolescence decreases CRF-R1 gene expression in subregions of the hippocampus and amygdala, and antagonism of CRF-R1 following adolescent stress attenuates behavioral indices of long-term anxiety states (Veenit et al. 2014) and also depressive-like behavior (Bourke et al. 2014). Systemic administration of CRF-R1 antagonists during adolescence prevents stress sensitization to conditioned place preference for psychostimulants (Brielmaier et al. 2012; Nader et al. 2012). The current study aims to determine whether microinjection of the CRF-R1 antagonist CP359376 into the VTA prior to each episode of adolescent social defeat will block putative neuroadaptations from occurring that promote escalated cocaine self-administration in early adulthood. Furthermore, we investigated the acute effect of CP359376 on social defeat behavior and searched for adolescent behavioral predictors of adult cocaine taking.

Experimental methods

Long-Evans rats from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA) were housed in a vivarium that was maintained at 21 ± 1 °C with 35–40 % humidity on a reverse light/dark cycle (lights on 2000 to 0800 h). Non-littermate male rats (N = 64, 5 separate 1-day shipments over 7 months) arrived between P20 and P22 and were housed in pairs in standard polycarbonate cages (45 × 24 × 20 cm). Food (LabDiet 5001 rodent chow) and tap water were available ad libitum. Reliably aggressive adult male rats (500–700 g; N = 20) were housed with females in large stainless steel cages (71 × 46 × 46 cm) in a separate room, but within the same vivarium (Miczek 1979). Facilities and procedures were approved by the Tufts Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in adherence with the guidelines established by the National Institutes of Health (National Research Council 2011).

Intracranial surgery

Between P28 and P30 rats were anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (6 mg/kg) and surgically implanted with bilateral stainless steel cannulae (23 ga, 9 mm length, Plastics One) aimed 1 mm above the VTA (anterior-posterior: −5.2 mm from bregma; medial-lateral: ±1.8 mm from bregma; dorsal-ventral: −7.1 mm from skull) at a 10° angle. Protection and patency of the cannulae were maintained by inserting obdurators between microinjections. Rats were allowed to recover until P35 before the first microinjection.

Microinjections and social defeat

All rats were microinjected with CP376395 (500 ng/side) or artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) into the VTA as previously reported for adult rats (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). After infusion, injectors were left in place for one additional minute to prevent backflow and allow for diffusion; then the obdurators were replaced and the rats were returned to their home cages (Fig. 1). Twenty minutes after the start of the microinjection, rats were placed into a resident’s cage. Before the first microinjections, all resident males had multiple encounters with non-experimental adolescent intruders and the six most consistently aggressive residents were selected based on short latency to attack, frequent aggressive postures, and consistent attack bites. Adolescents were defeated four times over the course of 10 days, following the intermittent episodic social defeat design of previous experiments (Boyson et al. 2014; Burke and Miczek 2015; Covington and Miczek 2001; Miczek et al. 2011). The interaction was terminated 5 min after the first attack bite or earlier if the intruder displayed a submissive supine posture for >8 s or if 13 attack bites occurred. The intruder was then placed in a protective mesh cage (20 × 30 × 20 cm), which was placed inside the resident’s home cage for 10 min. Intruders were exposed to a different resident during each episode of defeat. Control rats remained undisturbed during the defeats, and cage-mates always had the same treatment, either both defeated or both control.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Summary of experimental procedures and timeline

Behavioral analysis

Rat behavior was measured using The Observer XT v9.0.436 (Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands). A trained observer (intra-observer reliability r > 0.93) measured previously described social defeat behaviors (Buwalda et al. 2013; Koolhaas et al. 1980; Miczek 1979; Plyusnina et al. 2011). Since the duration of the fights (latency to submission) varied, all Observer-coded data were expressed as a percent of total time or number per minute. Resident behaviors quantified were as follows: attack bite, leg attack/lunging, dragging intruder, frontal threat, lateral sideways threat, allogrooming, and sniffing. The term “attack” refers to attack bites, leg attacks, and dragging. Intruder behaviors quantified were as follows: non-social exploration (rearing and sniffing), walking, supine posture, freezing posture, stretch attend posture, walking slowly, tail rattle, upright defensive posture, follow the resident, approach resident, crawl under the resident, and social investigation (sniffing resident). Freezing and stretch attend were combined and referred to as freezing. Social investigation, following, and crawling under were defined as social exploration. Immobility was calculated by subtracting percent of time engaged in behaviors requiring mobility (e.g., following) from the percent of time not walking. A time lag of +2 s from the attack bite was used to determine the probability of intruder behaviors in response to an attack bite.

Cocaine self-administration

Rats were anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (6 mg/kg). An indwelling catheter (Silastic silicone tubing, Dow Corning, ID 0.63 mm, OD 1.17 mm) was implanted into the right jugular vein as previously described (Burke and Miczek 2015; Covington and Miczek 2001) on P57–59. Following 5–6 days of recovery, the rats were transported to a custom-built chamber (30 × 30.5 × 24.5 cm) within a sound- and light-attenuating enclosure.

All self-administration procedures and equipment were closely similar to those used in previous experiments (Boyson et al. 2014; Burke and Miczek 2015; Fig. 1). Med PC IV (Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT) controlled experimental events and recorded operant responding.

During acquisition, rats were allowed to self-administer cocaine (0.75 mg/kg per infusion) according to a fixed ratio (FR) one schedule with the active and inactive levers present once per day. The session was terminated after 5 h or 30 infusions. On the second and third day of acquisition the experimenter simulated a response using the computer, which resulted in a single priming infusion of cocaine to increase motor activity. Rats were considered to have acquired the task when they obtained 15 infusions for two out of the three sessions. If rats did not achieve this requirement within three sessions (15 h of access to cocaine), the experimenter shaped the rat’s behavior. No behavioral shaping was used during measurement of dependent variables. After acquisition, the FR was gradually increased to five and after two stable days of FR5 responding the motivation to self-administer cocaine was studied according to a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement (PR).

Rats were given access to cocaine on a PR that required an increasing number of responses to be reinforced by an infusion of cocaine (0.3 mg/kg/infusion). A lower unit dose of cocaine was used for this phase (0.3 mg/kg/infusion) based on previous studies suggesting that reliably augmented self-administration following adult social defeat is observed at this lower dose (Covington and Miczek 2001; 2005). The PR session was followed by one or more FR5 maintenance sessions until responding stabilized again. The following day, rats were given access to cocaine (0.3 mg/kg/infusion) for 24 h on an FR5 schedule.

Histology

After the 24-h binge, rats were deeply anesthetized and perfused with saline and 4 % PFA, and their brains were fixed in PFA, then sucrose. Sixty-micrometer sections were mounted on slides and stained with cresyl violet to identify exact injection sites using light microscopy and a rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson 2007; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Representative photo of cresyl violet stained midbrain illustrating bilateral cannula placement 1 mm above the VTA and injector tracks entering the VTA. Each figure corresponds to coronal sections of the rat brain −5.2 to −6.04 mm from the bregma. Shown are symbols representing injection sites for control aCSF (black circle, N = 13), control CP376395 (gray circle, N = 12), stress aCSF (black square, N = 9), and stress CP376395 (gray square, N = 11) treatment groups

Drugs

CP376395 (Tocris Bioscience) was dissolved in aCSF. The chosen dose had proven to be most effective in similar studies in adult rats (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). Cocaine hydrochloride was obtained from the Research Technology Branch of the National Institute on Drug Abuse (Rockville, MD) and dissolved in sterile 0.9 % saline.

Statistical analysis

Behaviors during social defeat encounters were analyzed with a repeated measures two-way ANOVA with the factors day (first or fourth defeat) x drug (aCSF or CP376395). Two-way ANOVA (drug x treatment [control or social defeat] was used to analyze the cocaine self-administration data and body weights. The percent increase in body weight was calculated by examining the difference between the average weight from P51–56 (pre-surgery) and the average weight on P31–35 (pre-stress) for each individual. The Kaplan-Meier survival analysis followed by the Mantel-Cox log rank tests was used to analyze the percent of rats reaching the criteria for acquisition of cocaine self-administration. Separate linear regression analyses identified significant correlations between adolescent behavior and adult cocaine taking. Separate Grubbs’ tests were applied to cocaine self-administration data (except acquisition) and behavioral data during social defeat to identify outliers (Grubbs 1969). The outcome removed subjects from the binge (N = 1), PR (N = 1), maintenance (N = 0), and from several behavioral data sets (N = 1–3). Survival analysis and log ranks were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics v21 (IBM Corp.). Sigma Plot v11 (Systat Software Inc.) was used for the remainder of statistical tests. The alpha level was always 0.05 for statistical significance.

Results

Histology

Intra-VTA placements were evenly distributed across the anterior-posterior dimension of the VTA (Fig. 2). There were too few subjects with placements outside of the VTA (N = 6) to allow statistical comparison, and there were no misses in one treatment group.

Social defeat behavior

Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs yielded several significant effects of day of adolescent defeat stress and drug infusion during the first (P35) and fourth (P44) social defeat (Table 1). Both vehicle- and CP376395-infused rats were quicker to freeze after repeated defeats as evidenced by the main effect of day for latency to freeze. The CP376395 infused rats exhibited a longer latency to submission. The drug x day interaction for social exploration and following behavior as well as non-social exploration behavior was significant, whereby aCSF-infused rats increased social exploration but decreased non-social exploration from P35 to P44. These effects were absent in CP376395-infused rats. CP376395-infused rats walked less and spent more time immobile compared to aCSF-infused rats as indicated by main effects of drug. There was a significant main effect of day for the probability of displaying the supine posture when attacked, suggesting that rats were more likely to adopt the supine posture when attacked on P44 compared to P35.

Table 1 Mean (± SEM) social defeat behaviors of intruders infused with aCSF or CP376395 prior to first defeat (P35) and fourth defeat (P44)

The main effect of drug on latency to attack was significant, suggesting that the residents were quicker to attack aCSF-infused rats than to attack CP376395-infused rats. There were also significant main effects of drug on the number of attacks per minute and a drug x day interaction. Residents attacked P44 intruders at a higher rate than P35 intruders only if the intruder was infused with aCSF prior to the social interaction. The residents also attacked aCSF-infused intruders at a higher rate than CP376395 intruders on P44. However, the residents spent more time sniffing the CP376395 intruders as evidenced by a main effect of drug. There was a significant drug by day interaction on the latency to sniff the intruder, revealing that residents were slower to sniff CP376395 intruders on P35 compared to P44.

Body weight gain

The percent increase in body weight gain was 57.7 ± 0.6 for aCSF control, 58.6 ± 0.8 for CP376395 control, 57.1 ± 0.6 for aCSF stress, and 59.4 ± 1.0 for CP376395 stress. The main effect of drug on body weight gain reached significance (F(1.45) = 3.98, p ≤ 0.05) indicating that rats infused with the CRF-R1 antagonist gained slightly more body weight.

Cocaine self-administration

Rats infused with intra-VTA aCSF prior to each adolescent social defeat acquired the i.v. cocaine self-administration task in early adulthood significantly faster than rats infused with CP376395 prior to defeat (x 2 = 4.2, p < 0.05; Table 2). There was a significant main effect of drug on stable responding for i.v. cocaine at FR5, referred to as “maintenance” in Table 2 (F(1.36) = 7.30, p ≤ 0.01), indicating that adult rats infused with aCSF prior to adolescent defeat responded at a higher rate for i.v. cocaine than rats infused with CP376395. For PR (Table 2), a significant effect of drug (F(1.34) = 8.27, p < 0.01) suggested that CP376395 infusion prior to adolescent defeat attenuated the number cocaine infusions achieved during the PR test compared to aCSF-infused rats. There was a significant drug x stress treatment interaction for total cocaine infusions obtained during the 24-h binge (F(1.34) = 5.51, p < 0.05; Fig. 3, right panel). Post hoc comparisons indicated that within rats infused with aCSF during adolescence, the socially defeated adolescent rats consumed more cocaine than non-defeated controls during the 24-h binge in adulthood (p ≤ 0.05). Furthermore, among the socially defeated rats, infusion of CP376395 prior to each adolescent defeat blocked the escalated cocaine consumption during the 24-h binge observed in aCSF-infused rats (p < 0.01).

Table 2 Adult cocaine self-administration following social defeat stress during adolescence
Fig. 3
figure 3

Number of cocaine infusions (mean ± SEM) accumulated in 2 h bins (left) and during the entire 24-h binge (right) by non-stressed controls infused with aCSF (N = 11), non-stressed controls infused with CP376395 (N = 9), stressed rats infused with aCSF (N = 9), and stressed rats infused with CP376395 (N = 10). Artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF); CP376395 (CP). *Significantly different from control aCSF group. #Significantly different from stressed CP376395 group

The latency to exhibit the supine posture on the last day of adolescent social defeat (P44) was negatively correlated with total cocaine infusions during the 24-h binge for the aCSF-defeated group (p ≤ 0.001, r 2 = 0.89) and the CP376795-defeated group (p < 0.004, r 2 = 0.71) as assessed in early adulthood (Fig. 4, top). The latency to display the supine posture was also negatively correlated with cocaine infusions obtained under PR for aCSF-defeated group (p = 0.023, r 2 = 0.68) and the CP376795-defeated group (p = 0.004, r 2 = 0.67; Fig. 4, bottom). When aCSF and CP376395-defeated groups were combined, latency to display the supine posture was still negatively correlated with cocaine infusions during the binge (p = 0.002, r 2 = 0.52), PR (p < 0.001, r 2 = 0.66), and also with responses per minute during maintenance (p = 0.018, r 2 = 0.32).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Top, latency to adopt the supine posture during the fourth defeat on P44 was negatively correlated with the number of cocaine infusions obtained during the 24-h binge for rats infused with aCSF prior to social defeat stress and those infused with CP376395. Bottom, latency to supine on P44 also was inversely correlated with cocaine infusions obtained during progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement

Discussion

The present experiment confirmed that social defeat stress during adolescence escalated adult cocaine self-administration during the 24-h binge (Burke and Miczek 2015). This effect was prevented by CRF-R1 antagonism in the VTA prior to each adolescent social defeat episode, suggesting that VTA CRF-R1 receptors are essential for adolescent social defeat to initiate a neuroadaptive process that escalates later drug self-administration in adulthood. Furthermore, the latency to assume the supine posture after repeated social defeat episodes was highly predictive of adult cocaine taking both after vehicle and CP376395 infusion into the VTA prior to the social defeat, indicating that this salient element of submissive behavior during adolescence is somehow related to adult drug taking.

Adaptive changes in both social and non-social behaviors over the course of repeated social defeats characterized vehicle-infused control rats, while these effects were absent in rats infused with CP376395. Notably, rats infused with CP376395 prior to each social defeat walked less and were more immobile during interactions with the aggressive adult. These results suggest that CRF-R1s in the VTA are important for locomotion behavior under high stress conditions and for adaptive behaviors over repeated exposures to social stress. Meanwhile, the residents were less aggressive toward these CP376395-injected rats on P44, but more likely to socially investigate them, suggesting that the lack of adaptive changes in CP376395-infused rats impacted resident social behavior during the fourth social defeat (P44).

Both CP154526 and CP376395 CRF-R1 antagonists infused into the VTA prior to social defeat in adulthood prevent escalation of cocaine self-administration (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). Systemic and intracerebroventricular infusion of both these CRF-R1 antagonists causes transient reductions in alcohol drinking (Hwa et al. 2016), and CP376395 reduces stress-induced escalation of alcohol drinking (Hwa et al. 2015). CP154526 has high affinity (Ki < 10) for CRF-R1 (Schulz et al. 1996) and is a prototypic CRF-R1 antagonist. CP376395 is more selective for CRF-R1 (IC50 = 5) and more water-soluble than CP154526 (Chen et al. 2008). The high dose of CP376395 (500 ng/side) was chosen based on previous studies (Blacktop et al. 2011; Holly et al. 2015) and because it most consistently blocked neural and behavioral cross-sensitization in our previous study of adult rats (Boyson et al. 2014).

The investigation of behavior during social defeat 20 min after intra-VTA CRF-R1 antagonist infusion was previously unexplored. There was evidence for habituation in the non-social exploration behavior in aCSF-treated rats from P35 to P44 confirming our previous study (Burke and Miczek 2015). In addition, the aCSF-treated rats attended to the resident more on P44 compared to P35. However, the CP376395-treated rats did not adapt to repeated social defeat encounter as the aCSF-treated rats did, suggesting a potential role of VTA CRF-R1 in behavioral adaptation during adolescent stress. Confrontation with an aggressive adult opponent activates the HPA stress response in adolescent rats (Watt et al. 2009). The HPA axis is initiated by hypothalamic CRF release, which eventually causes the synthesis of corticosterone in the adrenals and systemic corticosterone exerts negative feedback on further CRF release (Herman et al. 2003). Our finding that intra-VTA CP376395 reduces motor activity agrees with the attenuating effect of systemic administration of the CRF-R1 antagonist antalarmin on i.c.v. CRF-induced motor activity (Zorrilla et al. 2002). Low motor activity of adolescent rats injected with CP376395 could explain the reduced rates of attacks and the increased latency to attack toward these less active intruders, since reduced intruder locomotion is thought to reduce resident aggressive behavior (Buwalda et al. 2012; Paul et al. 2011). As the residents spent less time attacking the CP376395-infused intruders they spent more time sniffing them, suggesting residents were no less interested in the drug-treated intruders, just less aggressive toward them. The behavior of the intruder rat has been shown to influence resident aggression (Blanchard and Blanchard 1977; Miczek 1979; Burke and Miczek 2015). For example, lower levels of motor activity during the social defeat test are thought to reduce further aggression when the intruder is an adolescent (van den Berg et al. 1999) and when it is an adult (Buwalda et al. 2012; Nocjar et al. 2012; Paul et al. 2011). It is not possible to determine whether the increase in aggression from P35 to P44 is a result of the conditioned behavior of the intruder or the increased probability of attacking a larger and older adolescent intruder undergoing sexual maturation (Burke and Miczek 2014).

The latency to display the supine posture after repeated social defeats predicted greater risk for escalated cocaine taking about 5 weeks later in early adulthood. The probability of the intruder exhibiting the supine posture increased over the course of repeated defeats, indicating increased submissive behavior during repeated aggressive encounters. In agreement with our previous study (Burke and Miczek 2015), rats were quicker to freeze from P35 to P44 when confronted with the resident. Attack-induced freezing during adolescent social defeat predicted adult cocaine taking in our previous experiment (Burke and Miczek 2015), yet this correlation was absent in the present study. Discrepancies between the current and past results may be the result of additional handling during the microinjection procedure, the change in duration of supine posture required for fight termination (4 s in the previous study to 8 s in the current study), or the exclusion of the single housing factor. Overall, adaptive behaviors during adolescence that prevent further attacks such as decreased latency to supine in this study and increased attack-induced freezing in our previous study (Burke and Miczek 2014) have interesting relevance to the escalation of cocaine taking in adulthood.

Rats infused with aCSF prior to social defeat self-administered more cocaine during the 24-h unlimited access binge as previously observed (Burke and Miczek 2015). Pretreatment with bilateral CP376395 into the VTA prior to each adolescent social defeat blocked escalation of cocaine self-administration during the binge in adulthood. This result is similar to the prevention of escalated cocaine taking during the 24-h binge by CRF-R1 antagonism in the VTA of adult rats prior to social defeat (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). The major methodological differences between the current study and Boyson et al. (2014) are that the intruders were adolescents and that they were housed in pairs. This agreement between studies with adolescent and adult rats is concordant with the observations that CRF-R1 mRNA in the rat mesocorticolimbic system peaks in the brain prior to adolescence between P12 and P21 (Avishai-Eliner et al. 1996; Insel et al. 1988). In the VTA, CRF-R1 and CRF-R2 expression remains constant during the stages of adolescence into adulthood (Lukkes et al. 2016). Thus, the role of CRF-R1 in the VTA in escalation of cocaine self-administration following social defeat stress is probably not age-dependent.

One potential neural mechanism for social defeat episodes during adolescence to escalate cocaine self-administration in adulthood involves the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Both cocaine and intermittent social defeat stress during adulthood increase extracellular dopamine in the NAc shell in rats with a history of intermittent social defeat (Miczek et al. 2011), which is attenuated by intra-VTA CP376395 prior to each defeat (Boyson et al. 2014). While adolescent social defeat does not increase extracellular dopamine in the NAc core stimulated by amphetamine in adulthood (Burke et al. 2013), it remains to be determined whether social defeat during adolescence increases subsequent extracellular NAc shell dopamine in response to cocaine. The current results for the 24-h binge are consistent with other findings suggesting that CRF receptors in the VTA are involved in escalated cocaine self-administration (Specio et al. 2008), stress-induced reinstatement (Blacktop et al. 2011; Chen et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2005), and the induction of neuroadaptive changes that take place during social stress to escalate later cocaine self-administration (Boyson et al. 2014; Boyson et al. 2011). Other studies suggest that CRF-R1 antagonism prevents adolescent non-social stress from increasing preference for contexts paired with experimenter-administered nicotine and cocaine (Brielmaier et al. 2012; Nader et al. 2012). The current results are the first evidence of a role of CRF-R1 for adolescent social stress to induce neuroadaptive changes that escalate voluntary drug self-administration. These novel results also implicate the adolescent VTA CRF system in the propensity for escalated cocaine taking after adolescent adverse experience.

Not only salient rewarding events (Schultz 2002) but also stressful experiences, such as social defeat of adult rats, increase tonic and phasic dopamine signaling in the mesocorticoaccumbal system (Anstrom et al. 2009; Holly et al. 2015; Tidey and Miczek 1996), which presumably sensitizes the dopamine system to subsequent psychostimulant challenges. Stressful events also result in the release of CRF into the VTA (Wang et al. 2005), and VTA dopamine neurons increase their firing rate through activation of CRF-R1 receptors (Wanat et al. 2008). Thus, CRF-R1 antagonism prior to social defeat may prevent this increase in dopamine firing rate of VTA neurons, which in turn reduces cross-sensitization to cocaine, and attenuates cocaine self-administration during the 24-h binge in the CP376395 stressed group. Recent data investigating adult rat dopamine release during social defeat did not detect attenuated extracellular dopamine by the CRF-R1 antagonist (Holly et al. 2015). Because the dopamine system is undergoing substantial maturation during the mid-adolescent period (P34–46) when adolescent social defeat occurs (reviewed in Burke and Miczek 2014), it is necessary to investigate tonic and rapid phasic firing of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system during social defeat stress and CRF receptor manipulation during adolescence to completely rule out the above hypothesis. Future studies need to investigate multiple doses of the CRF-R1 antagonist and determine the role of CRF type 2 receptors that are implicated in adult social defeat studies (Boyson et al. 2014; Holly et al. 2015) and other aspects of the CRF system (e.g., binding protein) for a role in adolescent social defeat behavioral adaptations over repeated stressful episodes.

We found that CRF-R1 within the VTA plays an important role in the link between adolescent social stress and adult-escalated cocaine taking during unlimited access. Presumably, by blocking CRF-R1 in the VTA, we are preventing social stress-induced neuroadaptations that would normally increase the risk for cocaine abuse. The CRF-R1s in the VTA are essential for normal acclimation to repeated social defeats during adolescence and also for motor behavior under stressful conditions. The impact of CRF-R1 on intruder behavior may impede aggressiveness of the resident, but does not reduce social exploration. Further studies are required to determine whether other drugs that alter intruder behavior similarly to the pharmacological manipulation in this study can also reduce cocaine taking in adulthood. Finally, individuals that were quicker to submission after repeated aggressive confrontations during adolescence were more likely to uncontrollably self-administer cocaine when given unlimited access in adulthood, suggesting that greater severity of social stress equates to greater risk for drug abuse. Although limited to this animal model, these findings implicate a potential neural mechanism explaining how victimization during adolescence increases risk for substance abuse and addiction in humans.