Keywords

1 Introduction

On the website of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (https://www.ninds.nih.gov/Disorders/All-Disorders/Tardive-Dyskinesia-Information-Page), tardive dyskinesia (TD) is classified as an extrapyramidal syndrome arising from long-term use of neuroleptic agents (i.e., antipsychotics). TD is characterized by repetitive involuntary purposeless movements of specific muscle groups, which may include fingers, arms, legs, and trunk but generally presents as oral dyskinesia (typically, vacuous chewing movements, VCMs) which may include lip smacking and tongue protrusion. A person with TD may appear to be chewing gum.

TD arises in humans, nonhuman primates (Blanchet et al., 2012), and rodents by chronic (exclusive) treatment with a dopamine (DA) D2 receptor (R) antagonist (Saifee & Edwards, 2011) – the class of agents oft used for treating schizophrenia. The incidence rate of TD is approximately 5% per treatment year with a “classical” (first-generation) antipsychotic drug (e.g., haloperidol) (Kane et al., 1985) and two to threefold less so when atypical antipsychotic agents (e.g., clozapine or quetiapine) are used (Tarsy et al., 2011). TD tends to be long lived, sometimes lifelong, and is usually refractory to treatment. TD remains a serious problem today. First-line “treatment” of TD is withdrawal of the antipsychotic, if possible. Vesicular monoamine transport inhibitors such as deutetrabenazine and valbenazine are viable treatment options (Bhidayasiri et al., 2018; Keepers et al., 2020; Ricciardi et al., 2019), although a variety of other drugs is often attempted.

2 Multineuronal Associations with Tardive Dyskinesia

Although TD arises from prolonged administration of a DA D2 R antagonist, it is recognized that many different neuronal phenotypes may have a role in the development or persistence of TD (Casey, 1987; Egan et al., 1995; Gong et al., 1992; Gunne & Häggström, 1983; Jeste & Caligiuri, 1993; Knable et al., 1994; Kostrzewa, 1995; Waddington, 1990). Cholinergic (Kostrzewa & Neely, 1993; Rupniak et al., 1983, 1985; Salamone et al., 1990), GABA-ergic [GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid] (Gunne et al., 1982; Lloyd et al., 1985; Mithani et al., 1987; Tamminga et al., 1979), and serotoninergic (Gong et al., 1992; Gong & Kostrzewa, 1992) involvements have been implicated in TD. Nevertheless, the focus of this chapter will remain on dopaminergic systems and the uncovered important role of serotoninergic systems in TD.

3 Oral Dyskinesia Arising in Rodents from Acute Dopamine Receptor Agonist or Antagonist Treatments

Acute administration of a DA D2 R antagonist to rats or mice results in the onset of oral dyskinesias, which are prominent over the following hour or more. The short-lived effect of the D2 R antagonist is dose-related. Similarly, acute administration of a DA D1 R agonist is associated with a short-lived and dose-related induction of oral dyskinesia. These correlated findings give credence to the possibility that oral dyskinesia may be related to an imbalance in D1 R/D2 R activation by the endogenous neurotransmitter, DA (Rosengarten et al., 1983). Moreover, when a D2 R antagonist is administered chronically, as per animal modeling of TD, or when psychiatric patients are treated long term with an antipsychotic drug, one must consider the long-term effect of such treatment on the overall balance of D1 R/D2 R activation – and the effect this might have on the origin of TD.

3.1 Experimental Induction of Oral Dyskinesia in Rodents by Acute Dopamine D1 R Agonist Treatment or D2 R Antagonist Treatment

Antipsychotics and D2 R antagonists in general induce the occurrence of spontaneous oral activity – the so-called oral dyskinesias in humans and the so-called VCMs in rodents (Clow et al., 1979; Iversen et al., 1980; Waddington & Gamble, 1980). Similarly, DA D1 R agonists induce oral activity (Arnt et al., 1987; Koshikawa et al., 1987; Levin et al., 1989; Molloy & Waddington, 1988; Murray & Waddington, 1989; Rosengarten et al., 1983; Rosengarten, Schweitzer, Egawa, & Friedhoff, 1986a; Rosengarten, Schweitzer, & Friedhoff, 1986b; Rupniak et al., 1985), and it is the presumed balance in D1R/D2R activation that is considered to be important for this behavioral response (Rosengarten et al., 1983). For example, in rodents, there is an increase in oral activity when there is an increase in D1R/D2R activation, as occurs with D2R antagonists, also after D1R agonist treatment (Johansson et al., 1987; Molloy & Waddington, 1987, 1988; Rosengarten et al., 1983), and in rat strains with reduced numbers of D2R (Rosengarten et al. 1986a, b).

3.2 Effect of Neonatal Dopaminergic Denervation on Induction of Oral Dyskinesia in Rodents by Dopamine D1R Agonists or D2R Antagonists

When rats are lesioned perinatally with the neurotoxin 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA; desipramine pretreatment), repeated D1 R agonist treatments “prime” DA D1 R. Initial D1 R agonist treatments have no marked effect, but repeated D1 R agonist treatments permanently sensitize D1 R, so that exaggerated behavioral responses are observed following acute D1 R agonist treatment. This process is known as homologous priming of D1 R, and the resulting D1 R supersensitization is lifelong (Breese et al. (1985, 1987). Also, repeated D2 R agonist treatments of 6-OHDA-lesioned rats can induce the D1 R supersensitization, a process known as heterologous priming of D1 R (Criswell et al., 1989).

When rats so-lesioned perinatally with 6-OHDA were challenged with a single dose of a D2 R antagonist, there was an increase in perioral movements when compared to the effect in nonlesioned control rats (Kostrzewa & Hamdi, 1991). In an analogous manner for rats that were perinatally 6-OHDA lesioned, the first adulthood challenge dose of a D1 R agonist surprisingly produced an increased number of perioral movements in comparison to nonlesioned control rats (Kostrzewa & Gong, 1991). Thus, in contrast to the latent D1 R supersensitivity for locomotory and stereotyped activities that required unmasking by repeated D1 R agonist treatments (Breese et al., 1985, 1987), there was overt supersensitization of D1 R for D1 R agonist induction of perioral activity. Moreover, in rats that were perinatally 6-OHDA-lesioned, perioral activity was induced in adulthood by a D1 R agonist dose that was 100–1,000 times lower than was needed for induction of perioral activity in intact control rats (Kostrzewa & Gong, 1991). The D1 R supersensitivity for this effect developed if the nigroneostriatal dopaminergic fiber lesioning by 6-OHDA occurred within the first week postbirth (Kostrzewa et al., 1993) and if neostriatal DA depletion was 98.5 % or more (Gong et al., 1993). This D1 R supersensitization was also lifelong. Moreover, the effect was unaccompanied by an increase in the Bmax for D1 R (i.e., relative number of D1 R) or change in the Kd (i.e., relative affinity of the receptor for an agonist) (Gong et al., 1994).

Perinatal 6-OHDA treatment which produces near total destruction of nigroneostriatal dopaminergic neurons (Doucet et al., 1986) leaves the ventral tegmental nucleus largely intact and able to provide dopaminergic innervation to limbic regions (Fernandes Xavier et al., 1994; Snyder et al., 1986). Thus, the important element of the above studies is that a lesion of dopaminergic innervation to the neostriatum resulted in overt D1 R supersensitization for the induction of oral activity (Kostrzewa, 1995; Kostrzewa et al., 1998, 2003, 2008).

4 Oral Dyskinesia Arising in Rodents from Acute Serotonin Agonist Treatment

When perinatally 6-OHDA-lesioned rats were challenged in adulthood with the serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) 5-HT2 R agonist m-chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP), the initial mCPP dose produced an enhanced increase in the number of perioral movements in comparison to that of intact control rats (Gong & Kostrzewa, 1992). This finding and others indicate that 5-HT2 R is supersensitized by 6-OHDA lesioning (El Mansari et al., 1994). The effect was not replicated by 5-HT1A R agonist 8-OH-DPAT [(±)8-hydroxydipropylaminotetralin] or 5-HT1B R agonist CGS-12066B (7-trifluoromethyl-4(4-methyl-1-piperazinyl)-pyrrolo[1,2-alquinoxaline]); nor was the effect attenuated by 5-HT1A/1B R antagonist pindolol, nor by the predominate 5-HT2A R antagonist ketanserin, nor by 5-HT3 R antagonist MDL 72222 (3-tropanyl-3,4-dichlorobenzoate) (Gong et al., 1993). However, the selective 5-HT2 R antagonist mianserin blocked the effect of mCPP, indicating that 5-HT2 R, perhaps 5-HT2C R specifically, represents the subtype that is overtly supersensitized by perinatal 6-OHDA lesioning. It is thus considered that supersensitized 5-HT2C R accounts for enhanced perioral movements following an adulthood 5-HT-agonist challenge dose in the group of rats perinatally 6-OHDA-lesioned rats (Gong et al., 1993).

In fact, the number of perioral movements after mCPP treatment was greater than that following D1 R agonist treatment (Gong et al., 1992). Mianserin pretreatment, in addition to blocking the effect of the 5-HT2 R agonist mCPP, blocked the effect of a D1 R agonist on perioral activity (Gong et al., 1992), and the dorsal striatum was one site for this interaction (Plech et al., 1995; Salamone et al., 1990). By contrast, a D1 R antagonist did not block the effect of mCPP (Gong et al., 1992). These findings indicate that DAR supersensitivity for perioral activity is mediated via a serotoninergic system and notably at the 5-HT2 R (Gong et al., 1993).

5-HT fiber hyperinnervation of neostriatum and the forebrain is prominent in rats lesioned perinatally with 6-OHDA (Berger et al., 1985; Snyder et al., 1986; Mrini et al., 1995; see Kostrzewa et al., 1998), and this is accompanied by an approximate 30–60% increase in the number of 5-HT1A R, 5-HT1B R, 5-HT1nonAB R, 5-HT2A R, and 5-HT2C R (Radja et al., 1993). Therefore, to further explore the effect of 5-HT innervation on perioral activity, the neurotoxin 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine (5,7-DHT) was co-administered with 6-OHDA to perinatal rats which were then studied as adults. In rats co-lesioned with 6-OHDA and 5,7-DHT, the supersensitized perioral response to a D1 R agonist was eliminated, indicating that 5-HT fibers indeed mediate the D1 R agonist effect on perioral movements (Brus et al., 1994).

5 Abatement of Oral Dyskinesia in a Rodent Model of Tardive Dyskinesia

Although acute effects of D1 R agonists on perioral activity are attenuated by acute D1 R antagonist treatment, a 5-HT2 R antagonist is equally effective (Gong et al., 1994). In the rodent model of TD, produced by chronic administration (approximating one year) of a D2 R antagonist, acute treatment during the haloperidol-withdrawal phase with a D1 R antagonist does not reduce the incidence of perioral movements (Kostrzewa et al., 2007); nor does a D2 R antagonist (spiperone), muscarinic receptor antagonist (scopolamine), histamine H1 R antagonist (cyproheptadine), opioid mu receptor agonist (morphine) or mu receptor antagonist (naloxone), adenosine A2A R antagonist (theophylline), alpha-adrenoceptor antagonist (phentolamine, phenoxybenzamine), beta-adrenoceptor antagonist (propranolol); neither a 5-HT1A R agonist (pindolol) nor 5-HT2A R antagonist (ketanserin). However, agents with 5-HT2C R antagonist activity were all effective in acutely reducing the number of perioral movements in the rodent haloperidol-withdrawn model of TD. Thus, mianserin, mesulergine, ritanserin, and clozapine each acutely reduced spontaneous oral activity during 8 months of the haloperidol-withdrawn phase in the rodent model of TD (Kostrzewa et al., 2007).

Despite the neural reorganization that occurs consequent to months-long continuous administration of a D2 R antagonist, it appears that it is an action mediated by serotoninergic 5-HT2 R that is most consequential in reducing spontaneous perioral movements (Gong et al., 1992). This is somewhat surprising, because 5-HT innervation in the brain is considered to be an afferent input, not an efferent output system. 5-HT innervation in the brain arises from the brainstem in the dorsal and medial raphe nuclei (see Kostrzewa et al., 1998). Also, from studies in rodents, it is recognized that perinatal 6-OHDA destruction of dopaminergic nerves results in supersensitization of DA D1 R as well as 5-HT2 R (Brus et al., 1994; Gong et al., 1992). Therefore, in TD it may be that supersensitized 5-HT2 R has the major influence on the induction of spontaneous perioral movements.

6 Tardive Dyskinesia Arising in Rodents from Chronic Dopamine Receptor Antagonist Treatment

Waddington and colleagues (Waddington et al., 1983; Waddington & Gamble, 1980) produced the first rodent model of TD by administering haloperidol in drinking water to rats for 6 consecutive months. An increase in spontaneous perioral movements developed only at 4 months, thus fulfilling the characteristic “tardive” element of TD (i.e., tardive signifies an onset of symptoms after chronic drug treatment). The increase in spontaneous oral activity persisted for as long as haloperidol continued to be administered. When haloperidol was withdrawn, spontaneous perioral movements waned and reverted to normal over a period of a month (see also Waddington, 1990). Ultimately, Ellison and See (1989) showed that the form and periodicity of oral dyskinesias in haloperidol-treated rats was similar in form to those of humans with TD. A disadvantage to this model of TD was that the high level of oral activity in rats dissipated over a period of 1 month, back to control level, after haloperidol was withdrawn. This was unlike the persistence of perioral activity in people with TD.

Gunne et al. (1982) and Glassman and Glassman (1980) had observed that there is an increase in neuroleptic-evoked perioral activity in rats following damage to the brain. In the interest of developing a somewhat more novel model and possibly permanent model of TD, it occurred that DA denervation, as occurs with 6-OHDA lesioning, may be an important element in both the prominence and persistence of oral dyskinesia in rodents. Accordingly, rats with 6-OHDA were perinatally lesioned, then haloperidol was added to the drinking water when these rats were 2 months of age. In these largely DA-denervated rats, an increase in spontaneous oral activity occurred at 3 months, one full month earlier than in intact rats receiving haloperidol via drinking water. Also, the number of perioral movements induced in 6-OHDA-lesioned rats by chronic haloperidol was twice the level for chronic haloperidol-exposed intact rats. At 1 year the neostriatal Bmax for the D2 R antagonist [3H]raclopride was increased ∼25 % in 6-OHDA-lesioned rats and ∼65 % in haloperidol-treated rats were either nonlesioned or additionally 6-OHDA-lesioned. Notably, when 6-OHDA-lesioned rats were withdrawn from haloperidol at 1 year, increased perioral activity did not revert to control levels. The high level of perioral activity observed during the haloperidol phase continued to persist in the group during the withdrawal phase until the study was terminated 8 months later. Further, it is notable that there was no increase in the neostriatal D2 R (i.e., Bmax for [3H]raclopride binding to striatal homogenates) during the haloperidol-withdrawal phase in which there was a persistent high level of perioral activity (Huang et al., 1997). Andersson et al. (1990) have shown that humans with TD do not have an increase in D2 R in the brain.

7 Neural Mechanisms Attending Animal Modeling of Tardive Dyskinesia: Relevance to Human Tardive Dyskinesia

The multiple animal modeling studies relating to TD indicate the following neural mechanism relating to TD. The balance of DA D1 R/D2 R activation appears to be of critical importance. Both D1 R agonists and D2 R antagonists promote oral activity. However, acute effects do not represent a modeling of TD.

TD is, by definition, a “tardive” effect, one that develops over a period of months during prolonged or chronic treatments, specifically with a D2 R antagonist. TD appears to be induced by prolonged imbalance of D1 R/D2 R activation, consequent to a tip in the balance towards D1 R overactivation, and possibly in neostriatum or other regions of the forebrain. The nigroneostriatal dopaminergic tract appears to relate most closely to TD. A remodeling of neural circuitry in the brain, over a period of months, represents an adaptive mechanism to the imbalance of D1 R/D2 R activation, which ultimately results in induction of spontaneous perioral movements. The neural adjustments and reorganization involved in the process are still a mystery.

Nevertheless, prolonged overactivation of DA D1 R appears to be the genesis for TD. Furthermore, it is apparent that damage to brain circuitry can accelerate the appearance of TD and impact on its permanence. From the most recent experimental studies, it is obvious that damage to dopaminergic innervation to neostriatum promotes earlier onset of TD and a fostering of permanence. From studies by Gunne et al. (1988), it is possible that damage to other neural circuits in the brain can have similar influence on TD.

8 Conclusion

Because chronic D2 R antagonist treatment of both humans and rodents produces TD, it is reasonable to assume that similar neural adaptive mechanisms occur in the brain of both rats and humans. Also, because nonspecific injury in the brain (see Gunne & Häggström, 1983) as well as nigroneostriatal dopaminergic damage confer permanence to the TD produced by prolonged DA D2 R antagonist treatment, it is possible that in those patients who develop lifelong TD, there is residual brain damage or dopaminergic fiber degeneration that impacts of the D2 R antagonist treatment. It is speculated that even a mild brain injury could have occurred earlier in life (i.e., head trauma) or perhaps dopaminergic innervation of the brain declines with aging. The latter event is thought to occur and thereby account for the origin of Parkinson’s disease in aged humans (Kish et al., 1992).

Moreover, an increase in the number of nigroneostriatal DA D2 R is not correlated with high levels of perioral movements, as demonstrated by Huang et al. (1997). Studies by Breese et al. (1985) and Gong et al. (1994) established that DA D1 R supersensitivity is not necessarily accompanied by an increase in the number of D1 R. Also, DA D2 R supersensitivity is not correlated with an increase in nigroneostriatal DA D2 R number in rodents (Kostrzewa & Brus, 1991) nor in humans with TD (Andersson et al., 1990). Therefore, the process of receptor supersensitization is not reliant on changes in DA receptor number or affinity. Regardless of underlying factors for development of TD, the animal experimental findings implicate 5-HT2 R antagonists as reasonable agents for the treatment of TD (Kostrzewa et al., 2007). It is proposed that the development of new agents for TD should re-focus on the serotoninergic 5-HT2C receptor.

9 Cross-References