Keywords

1 Introduction

Kissing bugs, in particular Rhodnius prolixus , exhibit the double interest of being considered as a classical model in insect physiology and also a vector of a major health problem in the New World, that is, Chagas disease. Understanding the behaviour of kissing bugs, as well as its underlying physiological mechanisms, is crucial for better understanding their role as vectors of Trypanosoma cruzi and may be adapting strategies for better controlling the transmission of Chagas disease.

Not far in the past, it was sometimes laborious persuading certain scientific and non-scientific fora about the necessity of a deep biological knowledge for designing and adapting efficient control campaigns. The reason was a fair point: the urgency for stopping disease transmission masked the necessity of scrutinising fundamental aspects. Control campaigns have been relatively successful and important progress has been achieved in eradicating bugs from vast regions. Yet, some problems, such as the development of resistance to control actions and resilient house reinfestation, still require digging deeper in biological aspects; not in the search for a ‘silver bullet’, but for making control as effective as possible.

In recent years, much effort has been devoted to the study of triatomine behaviour. This interest is not only inspired by the need to control bugs but also because these insects constitute an excellent model for the study of fundamental aspects of haematophagy and general questions on insect physiology, which have been pondered since the time of V.B. Wigglesworth. The present document presents an analytical review of our knowledge about triatomine behaviour and suggests possible future research topics regarding particular aspects for which a better understanding would be beneficial. Besides, the author is convinced that triatomines, and in particular Rhodnius prolixus , still constitute excellent model systems for unravelling fundamental biological mechanisms. Recently, the genome of Rhodnius prolixus has been sequenced (Mesquita et al. 2015), representing a major breakthrough in our comprehension of the genetic bases of the adaptations of insects to haematophagy and the interaction of blood-sucking insects with parasites and hosts. This achievement opened multiple research lines on lesser understood biological aspects and gave place to novel hypotheses, many of which wait for a functional validation. The timing for this review seems appropriate to open novel research possibilities in functional genetics of insect behaviour.

Most of our current knowledge on the behaviour of kissing bugs comes from the exhaustive study of a group of species considered as major vectors of Chagas disease , in particular Rhodnius prolixus and Triatoma infestans, but also Panstrongylus megistus, Triatoma brasiliensis, Triatoma dimidiata and few others. Yet, given the diversity of species, habitats and hosts associations present in the group, we can expect differences in the behavioural adaptations across triatomine species, in particular those inhabiting isolated ecotopes or challenging to rear in the laboratory. As a consequence, the information presented here should not be taken as valid for every species of the subfamily, but as representative of well-established strategies and mechanisms, serving as a rational basis for the analysis of behavioural traits and adaptations of other species to their specific way of life. An extensive revision of the behaviour of triatomines has been published some years ago (Lazzari et al. 2013); this chapter presents a more synthetic and updated view.

2 Host Search and Feeding Behaviour

Triatomines are documented as nocturnal insects that during daylight hours remain hidden and virtually immobile (i.e. akinesis) in dark places. During the first hours after dusk, bugs awake and begin exploring the surroundings, looking for distinctive cues revealing the presence of warm-blooded vertebrates. Host detection is achieved via air currents carrying distinctive odours, water vapour and heat. For this, triatomines exhibit not only a high level of sensitivity, but also the capacity of integrating multimodal information. Conversely, inhibitory mechanisms keep the insects away from hosts when obtaining a blood meal that is not absolutely necessary (Guerenstein and Lazzari 2009).

2.1 Orienting Cues

When insects are motivated to search for a resource, as a hungry bug leaving its refuge to search for food, the sensory cues associated with that resource are not necessarily apparent. Most insects do not sit and stay until something appears, nor move randomly looking for signs . Instead, they orient themselves relative to air currents potentially transporting odours of interest. The way insects use air currents varies according to the stability of their direction (Sabelis and Schippers 1984; Bau and Cardé 2015). Even though inside a nest or house one cannot expect intense winds, air always moves (e.g. convective currents). Upon detection of host-associated cues, appetitive search and long-range orientation are triggered (Barrozo et al. 2017). It should be noted that, when detected at a distance, olfactory cues do not provide directional information by themselves, but they can trigger instead positive anemotaxis, that is, upwind displacement (Guerenstein and Guerin 2001; Guerenstein and Lazzari 2009; Núñez 1982; Taneja and Guerin 1995). When detected at a close range, chemical cues can stimulate the insects to follow the concentration gradient and attract them. As in virtually every blood-sucking insect, carbon dioxide modifies the behaviour of triatomines (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004a; Guerenstein and Hildebrand 2008). Triatomine responses to carbon dioxide are modulated along the day by an endogenous circadian rhythm, making insects responsive when feeding motivation is also higher during the first hours of the scotophase (Barrozo et al. 2004a; Bodin et al. 2008; Lorenzo and Lazzari 1998). The responsiveness of triatomines to carbon dioxide is also dependent upon their physiological condition. Long starvation periods do not seem to strengthen the response (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004a), but recent feeding switches the effect of carbon dioxide from attraction to repellence (Bodin et al. 2009b). Moulting and reproduction (Bodin et al. 2009a, b) also affect the responses of bugs to carbon dioxide. In contrast to other haematophagous insects, carbon dioxide is not an essential component for the attraction of triatomines, although air currents loaded with relatively low quantities of carbon dioxide are able to evoke attraction by themselves (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004a). Some host odorants , such as nonanal, increase bug activity (Guerenstein and Guerin 2001) and others that are found in human sweat (Cork and Park 1996) such as isobutyric acid and 1-octen-3-ol induce their attraction (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004b; Guerenstein and Guerin 2001). Finally, ammonia , which is present in the sweat and urine of vertebrates, is able to evoke both activation and attraction in Triatoma infestans (Taneja and Guerin 1997), antennal responses to this compound being higher with increasing starvation (Reisenman 2014).

In nature, kissing bugs are confronted with mixtures of odours rather than with isolated volatiles. The components of such mixtures often interact in a synergistic form. In the case of triatomines, the response threshold for pure carbon dioxide is beyond 300 ppm above atmospheric concentration (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004a). Interestingly, pure L-lactic acid and short-chain fatty acids that are present in human sweat and on the skin (Bernier et al. 2000; Cork and Park 1996) are completely ineffective for triggering anemotaxis over a wide range of concentrations (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004a, b). Nevertheless, when sub-threshold amounts of carbon dioxide are combined with L-lactic acid and fatty acids in appropriate proportions, the attractiveness of the mixture becomes similar to that of a living host (Barrozo and Lazzari 2004b). Recently obtained experimental evidence supports the idea that microorganisms associated with human skin produce volatiles that are attractive to Rhodnius prolixus (Ortiz and Molina 2010; Tabares et al. 2018). These findings add a novel dimension to the analysis of how Chagas disease vectors recognize and localize their hosts.

Even though both vision and vibro-reception are well-developed triatomine senses, so far there is no evidence suggesting a role in host orientation. Mechanoreception is involved in long-range orientation, but not in relation with hosts, but with the detection of air currents guiding upwind displacement. The most important host-associated physical cue perceived by triatomine bugs is heat . Even if the heat emitted by host bodies is widely used as an orienting cue for many blood-sucking insects, its role remains relatively unknown for the vast majority (Lazzari 2009, 2019). In contrast, the thermal sense of triatomines has been the object of detailed studies. The sensitivity of triatomines to heat is extremely high, such that the insect can detect differences of thermal energy in the order of a few μWatt/cm2 (Lazzari and Núñez 1989b; Lorenzo et al. 1999a, b; Lazzari 2009). It has been calculated that Triatoma infestans is capable of detecting the heat emitted by a human face from a distance of approximately 2 m and by a mammal the size of a dog from several meters (Lorenzo et al. 1999a, b).

Triatomines remain the only group of blood-sucking insects whose ability to perceive host-emitted infrared radiation has been demonstrated (Lazzari and Núñez 1989b; Schmitz et al. 2000). This ability has important implications for successful host finding because infrared radiation propagation is not disrupted by air currents (which do disrupt conductive and convective heat transfer) or by the relative position of the insect with respect to the thermal source (which does influence convective heat transfer) (Lazzari 2009, 2019).

A remarkable characteristic of Triatoma infestans is the ability to detect the temperature and distance of heat sources, independently of their sizes, and based on thermal information alone. This seems to be possible due to their capacity to perceive radiant heat (Lazzari 2009; Lazzari and Núñez 1989b). A hypothetical model on how these bugs distinguish the temperature, size, and distance of objects based on thermo-receptive inputs and active antennal movements has been proposed by Lazzari (2009). It provides an explanation for the ability of triatomines to discriminate distant (or small) burning objects from closer (or large) tepid ones, even if the amount of thermal energy reaching their antennae is of the same magnitude.

The extreme thermal sensitivity of triatomines, together with their ability to discriminate the temperature of distant objects, raises the question about whether the absolute temperature of a heat source and the difference between the temperature of objects and their background are in fact used by these insects to recognize their hosts. This question is related to another, more practical one: are triatomines able to perceive objects at the temperature of a host when the ambient temperature is higher? Experiments were performed for testing the response of bugs to an object presented at different temperatures, in a chamber providing a background temperature that could also be modified (Fresquet and Lazzari 2011). Bugs responded by trying to bite objects showing temperatures between 30 °C and 40 °C, but only if the surrounding environment was colder than the objects themselves. Therefore, their ability to measure the temperature of objects is only effective when a positive difference exists between object and air temperature.

It should be emphasized that, in a natural context, triatomines are not exposed to single cues such as specific odours or heat , but to multiple cue combinations detected by different sensory modalities (mechanical, chemical, thermal and hydric). In the context of food search , multimodal convergence may increase the ability of bugs to find a host. For instance, it has been shown that water vapour , which constitutes a close-range orientation cue by itself, also increases bug responsiveness to heat (Barrozo et al. 2003). This can be due to the convergence of different sensory inputs into the insect brain or to a physical phenomenon (i.e. moist air transports more heat than dry air) increasing sensory stimulation.

2.2 Orientation Mechanisms

Sensory information can be used in different ways for locating a resource. In some cases, the source of stimuli is the resource itself, as the colour of a flower, but in others, external cues become relevant. For instance, the sun, the moon and the polarization pattern of the sky guide the displacements of ants, bees, beetles and other insects. In a similar way, the wind plays a major role in the orientation of insects that follow odour tracks. This is necessary because not all stimuli provide spatial information in the same way, nor the sensory system is able to extract precise spatial information from them. It is possible to locate a light spot in the night without difficulty, even with just one open eye, because light produces a discrete spatial pattern, propagates radially, it is not altered by air currents and our eyes are capable of producing a detailed image, even monocularly. This is not the case, however, for all potential stimuli, nor for all sensory systems (Lazzari 2009).

If we consider two main cues emitted by hosts, that is, odours (kairomones) and heat , each modality presents its own particularities and requires specific mechanisms in order for them to serve as orienting cues.

Odours disperse in the air and are transported by the wind and, as a consequence, only provide spatial information in the close proximity of the source. Insects, in general, and kissing bugs, in particular, use different mechanisms for olfactory orientation , according to the situation. Those chemical stimuli relevant for locating and recognizing elements in close proximity or physical contact usually act in high concentration and form stable concentration gradients, which can be followed by the insect. For instance, this is the way chemical tastants on the host skin are recognized before deciding to bite or not (Barrozo 2019). On the other hand, the detection of volatiles emitted by a distant host does not provide enough spatial information. In that case, upon detection of host-associated chemicals, bugs turn upwind, following the direction of the air current transporting the odour.

Heat disperses radially as light, but there is no ‘thermal retina’ for its detection, but specialized receptors located in the antennae (Zopf et al. 2014a, b). Then, from the point of view of a kissing bug, the spatial information delivered by a warm object is not as precise as that of a light source, but nor as imprecise as that of a source of odours. As indicated above, triatomines use thermal cues for locating a host and, once in contact with its skin, to locate the most adequate spot to be bitten (see below). Interestingly, each task involves a different use of sensory information or orientation mechanism (Wigglesworth and Gillett 1934, Flores and Lazzari 1996). The approach to a warm object can be performed using just one antenna and the bugs can determine the direction and follow thermal gradients for approaching, using a mechanism called ‘telotaxis ’ (i.e. enough information is provided by only one bilateral organ). Yet, when they extend their proboscis, bilateral inputs from both antennae need to be integrated in order to reach the target precisely, that is, ‘tropotaxis ’ (i.e. the contribution of both bilateral organs is necessary, Flores and Lazzari 1996; Lazzari 2009,).

The consideration of these possible alternatives (i.e. orientation mechanisms) in the use of sensory information is crucial in the design of experiments. For instance, the use of olfactometers using air currents for transporting odours is a very usual way for testing the response of insects to odours, kissing bugs included. Yet, this type of device is conceived to evoke odour-triggered anemotaxis, but this is not the only way a chemical compound may attract an individual, and a negative result does not necessarily mean that the insect does not respond or does not detect the odour. Triatomines do not respond to their arresting pheromone left on walked substrates (Lorenzo Figueiras and Lazzari 1998b).

2.3 Biting and Feeding

In addition to its role as an orienting signal during host search , heat is also used to locate blood vessels hidden under the host skin (Ferreira et al. 2007). By analysing the bugs feeding behaviour on live hosts, it has been shown that the insects do not bite randomly; instead, they extend their proboscises mostly towards vessels. When the host skin was replaced by a vessel-shaped heat source placed on a heated metal plate, both with independently controlled temperature, a similar precisely directed choice was observed for the warmest linear area. This suggests that heat discontinuities over the skin surface area are used to guide the bite. Biting the warmest patch of skin surface requires a bilateral integration of the thermal inputs provided by both antennae. If this integration is experimentally altered by a unilateral or bilateral antennectomy, bugs either miss the target (unilateral) or do not exhibit proboscis extension responses at all (bilateral antennectomy). This suggests that, if present, rostral thermoreceptors do not provide information for triggering or guiding biting (Ferreira et al. 2007). Interestingly, heat is a key factor for host finding and biting, but once a blood is contacted, heat becomes no longer relevant (Lazzari and Núñez 1989a, b).

The decision to bite the host skin and ingest its blood is a complex process for triatomines. Classical work revealed that food recognition in triatomines is based on the analysis of specific food properties, such as osmolarity (Guerenstein and Núñez 1994) and the presence of phagostimulant compounds (Friend and Smith 1977). For a long time, this remained the only information available. Yet, recent work has shed supplementary light on the process, revealing an elaborated feeding decision-making system, depending on specific sensory pathways and cognitive processes (Barrozo 2019).

Not all triatomines are obligate haematophagous insects. Entomophagy has been frequently reported in different species of kissing bugs. This habit, which can also be expressed towards conspecifics, involves either haemolymphagy or cleptoheamatophagy (i.e. stealing part of the vertebrate blood ingested by a conspecific). For instance, these phenomena have been reported to occur in Triatoma infestans and in Rhodnius prolixus (Schaub et al. 1989; Alves et al. 2011; Lazzari et al. 2018), eventually resulting in direct Trypanosoma cruzi transmission amongst Chagas disease vectors (Schaub 1988). Interspecific entomophagy is also frequent (Garrouste 2009; Pontes et al. 2011; Duran et al. 2016). Even though haemolymphagy and cleptohaematophagy can be considered two forms of cannibalism; there are different processes. Cleptohaematophagy is usually intraspecific and the gathered food is similar to that obtained from a vertebrate host, that is, blood. Haemolymphagy can occur against conspecifics or heterospecifics and the food is not vertebrate blood. It can be concluded then that the triatomine saliva is not only adapted to gathering blood from vertebrates, but also insect haemolymph (Alves et al. 2011).

Intriguingly enough, recent reports describe Rhodnius prolixus consuming a sugar solution (Diaz-Albiter et al. 2016) and water from a drop (Páez-Rondón et al. 2018) in the laboratory. These observations deserve to be further investigated, in order to evaluate their biological implications for triatomines.

3 Sexual Behaviour

Mating is a major biological necessity and insects have developed complex mechanisms for locating, attracting and choosing potential partners. Triatomines are not an exception and, even though no elaborated courtships can be observed as in other insects, mating relies on specific interactions between male and female individuals. One of them is polyandry , which has been observed in females of different bug species (Baldwin et al. 1971; Manrique and Lazzari 1994, 1995; Vitta and Lorenzo 2009; De Simone et al. 2018).

One aspect deserving major interest is the chemical attraction between sexes. The study of sexual pheromones has been particularly challenging in triatomines, with respect to their origin, biological significance and composition. Despite a considerable amount of experimental work and published observations on the chemical ecology of triatomines (Cruz-Lopez et al. 2001), it took time to understand how sexual pheromones work in this group. The first evidence of their occurrence in triatomines was obtained in Rhodnius prolixus (Baldwin et al. 1971) and much later in Triatoma infestans (Manrique and Lazzari 1995). In these species, mating couples release volatiles that attract and gather males, who eventually mate with a single female.

The origin of sexual volatiles has been a matter of controversy due to early reports describing the detection of isobutyric acid, produced by Brindleys glands, in the ‘headspace’ (i.e. air surrounding) of mating pairs (e.g. Fontan et al. 2002). It is worth highlighting that this compound is usually released when adult triatomine bugs are disturbed (Barrett et al. 1979). However, experiments occluding different exocrine glands of males and females alternatively revealed that female metasternal glands are the source of sexual signals, but not Brindleys glands (Crespo and Manrique 2007; Pontes et al. 2008; Vitta et al. 2009). Chemical analysis subsequently confirmed that isobutyric acid is not present in metasternal glands (Manrique et al. 2006) and does not make part of the sexual signal. Further evidence supports that female metasternal glands odours are emitted preferentially during the scotophase (Pontes et al. 2008), and that the pheromone is capable of inducing males to leave their shelters (Pontes 2010), take flight (Zacharias et al. 2010), aggregate (Pontes and Lorenzo 2012) and display odour-modulated anemotaxis in airstreams associated with female odours (Vitta et al. 2009; Pontes et al. 2014; May-Concha et al. 2013). The secretion of metasternal glands can be a complex mixture eventually including ketones, alcohols, dioxolanes and aldehydes (Manrique et al. 2006; Pontes et al. 2008; Vitta et al. 2009; May-Concha et al. 2013). Chemical and behavioural analyses allowed the recent development of a synthetic female-pheromone blend, capable of attracting males of Rhodnius prolixus in the laboratory (Bohman et al. 2018).

Additionally, female cuticular hydrocarbons seem to play a role in mate recognition (Cocchiararo-Bastias et al. 2011).

Non-receptive females reject male copulatory attempts by displaying various evasive behaviours including stridulation (Manrique and Lazzari 1994). Although differences have been reported for the mating behaviour of various triatomine species, stridulation by unreceptive females during male mating attempts seems to be a frequent feature (Lazzari et al. 2006).

4 Aggregation and Alarm

Triatomine bugs are gregarious insects sharing shelters. Depending on the species, this behaviour can be mediated by two chemical signals: a volatile aggregation pheromone emitted by bug faeces and a cuticular contact factor deposited on substrates. These signals act parallelly to the known tendency to maintain physical contact with the substrate and conspecifics (thigmotaxis) and by visual cues (i.e. darkness).

The faeces of triatomines are a source of aggregation pheromones that attract and gather bugs in their proximity (Lorenzo Figueiras et al. 1994; Schofield and Patterson 1977). It was originally suggested that the functionality of this signal would be to indicate a suitable food source (Schofield and Patterson 1977). Further studies revealed that faeces accumulate in the access path to refuges and play a major role as chemical landmarks for finding shelters (Lorenzo and Lazzari 1996). The pheromones emitted by bug excrement do not seem to have species-specific effects as they are capable of assembling bugs of other species (Lorenzo Figueiras and Lazzari 1998a; Pires et al. 2002a, b, c). They can be extracted using polar solvents, and chemical analysis has revealed multiple compounds whose biological role is not yet clear (Alzogaray et al. 2005; Cruz-Lopez et al. 2001; Mota et al. 2014).

As said above, bugs of some species deposit on substrates a non-volatile footprints signal promoting aggregation . This signal can be extracted with non-polar solvents and seems to be composed by a mixture of hydrocarbons of cuticular origin (Lorenzo Figueiras et al. 2009) and insects require physical contact for its detection (Lorenzo Figueiras and Lazzari 1998b). This pheromone, therefore, can be considered to be an arresting factor rather than an attractive one. It is worth noting that R. prolixus and T. brasiliensis do not seem to use footprints to promote arrestment (Lorenzo Figueiras and Lazzari 2002; Vitta et al. 2007), suggesting that mechanisms promoting bug aggregation differ across species.

Bugs exhibit a tendency to avoid illuminated environments, that is, negative phototaxis, which is mediated by the compound eyes and, in adult bugs, also by the ocelli (Lazzari et al. 1998; Reisenman et al. 1998). This photophobic behaviour also mediates shelter choice (Reisenman et al. 2000).

It is a frequent observation that mechanically disturbed adult triatomines release a pungent odour, which origin is the secretion of their Brindleys glands. As indicated above, the secretion is composed of isobutyric acid, together with a complex mixture of other volatiles (Cruz-Lopez et al. 2001; Guerenstein and Guerin 2004; Manrique et al. 2006; Palottini and Manrique 2016) produced by these glands. Triatomines respond by walking away from conspecifics releasing the Brindleys glands secretion (Manrique et al. 2006); yet, some reports also indicate that bugs approach to sources releasing low quantities of isobutyric acid (Ward 1981; Guerenstein and Guerin 2001). This dual response has not facilitated the distinction of the biological significance of this compound in triatomine biology. Its release by physically disturbed bugs and its repulsive role on other individuals have allowed considering the secretion of Brindleys glands as an alarm pheromone . It should be noted that Brindleys and metasternal glands are only present in adults. Given that the latter are involved in sexual communication , its exclusively imaginal character can be easily understood. Nevertheless, the fact that nymphs could respond to, but not produce alarm pheromones, remains to be interpreted in adaptive terms. As in other insect species, the response of bugs to their own alarm pheromone is modulated by the individual experience (Minoli et al. 2013).

5 Learning and Memory

In the past, insects were considered to be ‘reflex machines ’ whose behaviour were mostly ruled by stereotyped, innate responses to external stimuli (Giurfa 2004). At present, their ability to adapt their behavioural responses as a function of their previous experience is widely recognized. Despite possessing tiny brains and much less neurons than vertebrates, they have largely proven to be capable of complex forms of learning, including the acquisition of rules and concepts (Giurfa 2003; Giurfa et al. 2001). As a consequence, insects became major models in the study of learning and memory. Most studies in this area have focused only on a handful of species, and particularly, on vinegar flies and honeybees . Provided that none of them presents a haematophagous habit, the selective pressures that have modelled their cognitive abilities are much different to those acting on blood-sucking arthropods. Yet, the potential learning abilities of disease vector insects may have significant epidemiological consequences, because they can induce heterogeneous biting patterns within host populations (Kelly 2001; Kelly and Thompson 2000). Due to the strong selection pressures to which blood-sucking insects are exposed, it is reasonable to ask to what extent their cognitive abilities are developed and how could they modulate vector behaviour. For instance, learning to recognize the most vulnerable hosts through individual experience could influence host selection and, as a result, affect parasite transmission patterns (McCall and Kelly 2002; Vinauger et al. 2016).

Many field and semi-field studies have evinced behavioural responses that might indicate learning capabilities in disease vectors . Nevertheless, only a few controlled studies have succeeded in demonstrating conclusive experimental evidence (Alonso and Schuck-Paim 2006; Alonso et al. 2003).

Amongst Chagas disease vectors , early experiments failed to demonstrate learning capabilities (Abramson et al. 2005; Aldana et al. 2005, 2008). In most cases, the absence of positive results can be associated with issues that often plague this kind of studies. Learning experiments need a rigorous control over experimental variables (insect motivational state, time of the day etc.), adequate neutral stimuli (i.e. unquestionably perceived by the insect, but ineffective in evoking the evaluated response) and finally, a deep knowledge of the experimental model. It should also be noted that experimental psychology imposes a strict theoretical framework and numerous control experiments in order to properly assess the learning capabilities of an animal. In contrast to the study of honeybees and Drosophila, this has not always been respected in studies dealing with disease vectors (Alonso and Schuck-Paim 2006). In many cases ‘learning-compatible’ results are considered as evidence of learning to occur, without adequately excluding other, more parsimonious, explanations.

Triatomines are adequate models for the experimental study of the cognitive capabilities of blood-sucking insects because: (1) they can be reared in the laboratory under controlled conditions and fed using artificial feeders; (2) they are haematophagous during their whole life, thus facilitating experimentation with juveniles and adults; (3) their size facilitates testing them in constrained conditions (i.e. fixation to a holding support or to a locomotion compensator); (4) they express a widely exploited response in insect learning studies, the proboscis extension reflex or PER . Last but not least, our knowledge about the behavioural biology and the sensory ecology of triatomines is one of the deepest amongst disease vectors. This knowledge allows investigating cognitive abilities in meaningful biological contexts, such as blood feeding, aggregation and escape.

One of the most generalized forms of learning is establishing novel associations between pairs of stimuli by classical or Pavlovian conditioning. In its most basic form of classical conditioning, a ‘neutral’ stimulus , known to be perceived by the animal, but unable to trigger a given response (e.g. orientation) by itself, is paired with another stimulus, the ‘unconditioned stimulus ’, which innately evokes the desired response. After presenting them together several times, the originally neutral stimulus may acquire the capacity of evoking the same response, turning in what is called a ‘conditioned stimulus ’.

Classical conditioning protocols have been adapted for the appetitive and aversive training of Rhodnius prolixus . Vinauger et al. (2011a, b) successfully conditioned bugs to the same neutral odour, either making it act as an appetitive (i.e. inducing attraction) or aversive (i.e. inducing avoidance) conditioned stimulus. In that study bugs learnt to associate lactic acid , which is perceived but does not evoke any oriented behavioural response by itself in bugs, with either a blood meal or with mechanical disturbance. After being trained to reinforce a specific association, bugs were confronted with an air current loaded with lactic acid to test whether their innate behaviour changed. Not surprisingly, they manifested either significant attraction or significant repulsion to lactic acid-associated air currents , depending on their previous appetitive or aversive individual experience. This demonstrates not only the ability of bugs to learn and to remember information, but also their ability to use that knowledge in different contexts, for example, use in orientation tests in an olfactometer, what they have learnt when confronted to an artificial feeder (Vinauger et al. 2011a, b).

The ability of triatomines to modify their responses to chemical stimuli as a function of their individual experience has been proven to affect their host choices (Vinauger et al. 2012).

The conditioning of the PER is another classical insect learning paradigm in insects, which has been successfully applied with triatomines (Vinauger et al. 2013). This well-characterized response used with bees, flies, butterflies and bumblebees constitutes a simple bioassay for investigating different facets of insect learning and memory. In the case of triatomines, the PER could be aversively conditioned in Rhodnius prolixus and revealed that these bugs can remember novel associations for at least 72 h (Vinauger et al. 2013). Furthermore, those experiments showed that they are only able to learn during the night hours, that is, in the temporal context when they display their daily activity (Vinauger and Lazzari 2015).

The successful application of different learning protocols in experiments with Triatoma infestans and Rhodnius prolixus also provided relevant information about how can the previous experience modify their responses to alarm and aggregation pheromones (Minoli et al. 2013; Mengoni et al. 2016), which are genetically determined, but modifiable by experience. Learning protocols also allowed demonstrating that kissing bugs can generalize and discriminate between different bitter compounds (Asparch et al. 2016), proving the utility of this approach for investigating basic sensory principles (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of factors modulating host search in triatomines and other blood-sucking insects . The search for a host depends on external stimuli, helping the insect to find a potential blood source, and internal factors modulating its motivational state. The balance between inducing and inhibiting drivers will determine whether or not the insect will respond to the presence of a host, as well as the timing of these responses and the selection of the most appropriate host to feed on

6 Triatomine Chronobiology

A salient characteristic of the biology of triatomines is the marked temporal organization of their behaviour. It is known that selective pressures have acted to adjust the biting activity of blood-sucking insects to the time of the day when hosts are less active (reviewed by Barrozo et al. 2004b). However, the degree of synchronization of the various behavioural and physiological processes that have been proven to occur in triatomines is quite unusual. This temporal arrangement begins at a very early stage in the life of these bugs. Even ‘once-in-a-lifetime’ events in their biology are controlled by circadian clocks and not by the direct effect of the environment. First-instar larvae hatch from eggs in the early morning when the environmental relative humidity reaches a daily maximum. Nevertheless, this is not triggered by environmental changes but by an endogenous circadian clock (Lazzari 1991a; Schilman et al. 2009). The same temporal window is used for ecdysis across the various instars. Again, a circadian clock has been proven to control this process (Ampleford and Steel 1982). The temporal synchronization of egg hatching and ecdysis has an evident adaptive value that is probably related to the deleterious effect of low relative humidity on these processes. As indicated above, some triatomine species exhibit a marked xeropreference and this preference is not modified at egg laying activity nor for moulting (Guarneri et al. 2002; Roca and Lazzari 1994). Thus, we can conclude that triatomines exhibit a temporal hygropreference rather than a spatial one to adapt their needs at critical moments of their life. Instead of moving to humid places to perform humidity-sensitive activities, they perform them at a specific phase of the day that usually presents high humidity.

Even though triatomines are frequently described as nocturnal insects, their general, spontaneous activity can be categorized as bimodal, provided that it splits into two different temporal windows: one that encompasses the first hours after dusk and a second during the first hour after dawn, each controlled by a different internal oscillator (Lazzari 1992). These two peaks of activity comprise all the various activities displayed by these bugs, including the modulation of related sensory sensitivities. As a result, part of these processes take place during the first activity period, for example, host search, feeding, departure from refuges, egg laying and dispersion by flight, while others occur during the second activity peak as, for example, ecdysis, egg hatching, refuge search and bug aggregation (Barrozo et al. 2004a, b; Constantinou 1984) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Host and refuge search are two linked, but mutually excluding behaviours in triatomines. When the animal has consumed its nutritional reserves, it should go out from its refuge to look for a potential host. Once gorged, the bug looks for shelter. The figure synthesises the different steps and stimuli associated with these two activities, in the form of a loop that makes bugs alternating between food and shelter search

The behavioural response to external stimuli is also rhythmically modulated as evidenced by the fact that the sensitivity of the insects is higher at the time in which a given cue or signal becomes biologically relevant. As an example, bug eyes become more sensitive to light during the night thus enhancing the negative phototactic response of bugs (Reisenman et al. 2002; Reisenman et al. 1998). The responsiveness to odours also varies in a very specific manner, as it is maximal to host odours during the nocturnal activity peak, but during the early morning peak in the case of aggregation pheromones. These modulatory changes match the time of the day in which each olfactory stimulus has biological relevance, maximizing response efficiency (Barrozo et al. 2004a, b; Bodin et al. 2008).

A rhythmic change in the thermopreference of bugs has been described in several triatomines species. It causes bugs to expose themselves to relatively higher temperatures at the beginning of the night, before displaying the first activity peak. In the early photophase at the end of the second peak, the bugs return to cooler places. This rhythm is also controlled by an internal circadian clock (Lazzari 1991b; Minoli and Lazzari 2003). It is suggested that this thermopreference rhythm is intended to increase the body temperature of bugs and, in turn, their metabolic rate , just before exhibiting their regular daily activity. Conversely, both body temperature and metabolism would be reduced during resting periods.

In summary, many triatomine activities and processes are modulated in the form of daily rhythms. Most of them express at the individual level (activity, oviposition etc.), while others take the form of population rhythms (egg hatching and ecdysis). Interestingly, all daily rhythms have an endogenous basis and are controlled by circadian clocks with the exception of the modulation of aggregation pheromone sensitivity (Bodin et al. 2008) and the adaptation of ocelli to light (Lazzari et al. 2011). The precise temporal allocation of every sensitivity and action would be significant enough to cause the development of dedicated, specific clocks by natural selection. The fact that each activity has a particular temporal window is not just the result of its association with one of the two activity periods but it is a result of the temporal adjustment of that activity to a particularly adequate temporal context, for example, host resting period. The study of the chronobiology of triatomines also contributes to the understanding of the insect clock structure and the manner in which it coordinates different physiological processes (Steel and Vafopoulou 2006; Vafopoulou et al. 2007; Vafopoulou et al. 2010), revealing once more the value of these bugs as model systems to analyse fundamental biological questions.

One important consequence for researchers dealing with this strong temporal organization of triatomine physiology and behaviour is the conclusion that any process of interest must be studied in its appropriate temporal context . If the proper timing is not respected in studies of this nature, the results obtained may be biased, therefore masking the significant patterns (Lazzari et al. 2004) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The daily organisation of a kissing bug’s life. Typically nocturnal, these insects distribute their different activities in two temporal windows, one at dusk and another at dawn. Bug sensitivity for stimuli such as odours and light is also modulated accordingly, in order to render them more sensitive and more prone to learn about their environment during the night hours

7 Behavioural Manipulation

Controlling disease vectors by exploiting their behaviour requires a deep knowledge of their biology. Information such as the identity of relevant attractive cues, the profile of preferred shelters and environments, as well as the estimation of their ability to escape from traps is potentially useful for the planning of sampling, detection and killing activities designed for controlling these insects. It is also critical that the stimuli used to attract the insects, as well as their presentation context (i.e. environment, timing), were biologically relevant and coherent to the insect, and not just a combination of all potentially attractive stimuli (pheromones, host odours, light); any association of stimuli must be significant for the insect. Here, ‘biological context’ means a clear message (i.e. host, refuge, partner), an adequate spatial setting (indoors, outdoors, ground, wall, roof) and timing that matches the proposed information to the insect needs (e.g. time for host search or for refuge localization).

In addition to light-traps employed for trapping nocturnal insects , whose attractivity principle is not fully understood, trapping triatomines and other blood-sucking insects is mostly based on the use of baits delivering host-associated cues. The most efficient is, as expected, the use of live hosts which usually consist of a mouse or baby chicken (Abad-Franch et al. 2000; Noireau et al. 1999, 2002). The use of live hosts as bait can sometimes be impractical due to the difficulty of providing them adequate care during extended field campaigns. Some alternative baits have been proposed and have already passed both laboratory and field testing. The simplest is using Baker’s yeast cultures (a simple mixture of yeast, sugar and water) producing carbon dioxide and other volatiles that are attractive for kissing bugs (Guerenstein et al. 1995; Lorenzo et al. 1998, 1999a, b; Pedrini et al. 2009; Pires et al. 2000). In addition, a relatively simple combination of multimodal cues including heat , water vapour, carbon dioxide and short-chain fatty acids has been reported (Ryelandt et al. 2011). Both types of baits described above deliver compounds recognized to be attractive to many haematophagous arthropods and are useful for capturing not only blood-sucking bugs, but also mosquitoes and ticks (Ryelandt et al. 2011; Smallegange et al. 2005).

Artificial refuges operate in a different manner than baited traps. Rather than attracting the bugs, they provide convenient resting places. Due to the physical structure that induces bugs to enter and remain inside, they can even be used without chemical baits. Eventually, the bugs will produce ‘their own bait’ when their excrement, containing aggregation pheromones , starts to accumulate, attracting other bugs searching for shelters. Cardboard boxes simulating refuges have been used as part of the intra-domiciliary surveillance of Chagas disease vectors in endemic areas of Latin America (Gómez-Núñez 1965; Wisnivesky-Colli et al. 1987). Other refuge-like devices , composed of simple, resistant materials, have also proven to be effective for outdoor bug detection (Vazquez-Prokopec et al. 2002). An attractive perspective for the use of shelter-like devices is the incorporation of chemical lures into artificial refuges, as tested by Mota et al. (2014) and Forlani et al. (2015). Some devices combine attractive baits with killing or trapping tools (Pedrini et al. 2009; Rojas de Arias et al. 2012).

8 Perspectives and Research Needs

Our present knowledge allows identifying relevant aspects of the triatomine biology needing additional research efforts. Moreover, further investigation could provide important insight into the fundamental aspects of haematophagy and help in the development of novel control tools. The availability of the genome sequence of Rhodnius prolixus is an extraordinary opportunity to dig deeper into the genetic and molecular bases of haematophagous behaviour, confirming once more this species as one of the most useful and fascinating models available in insect science. I will mention here only three aspects that, in the opinion of the author, need to be prioritized.

Dispersion

Dispersion is a key element for the spread of Chagas disease and for the reinfestation of treated houses (Abrahan et al. 2011). Several relevant questions remain to be answered, in particular, concerning the sensory ecology of long-distance displacements (e.g. the use of terrestrial and celestial navigation cues, orientation mechanisms).

Behavioural Impacts of Infection

It is well known that many parasites modify the physiology and behaviour of vertebrate hosts and insect vectors to their own advantage (Lehane 2005; Schaub 2006). The behaviour of parasitized bugs is a major issue for which very restricted data are available. Trypanosoma cruzi infections seem to have physiological consequences for the insects (Vallejo et al. 2009; Fellet et al. 2014; Elliot et al. 2015), and it is highly probable that their behaviour is also affected. It has been shown that some key behaviours, like feeding and activity, differ between infected and non-infected bugs (Botto-Mahan et al. 2006; Marliere et al. 2015), yet the impact of infection on other behavioural traits needs to be further investigated, along with the underlying mechanisms (Takano-Lee and Edman 2002).

Behavioural Manipulation

Synthetic pheromones could be used to interfere with bug communication in the sexual, alarm and aggregation contexts. Even though these alternatives appear as promissory options and despite the steadily advancing knowledge on this field, the use of synthetic pheromones for triatomine behavioural manipulation still requires further research efforts to be useful at an operational level.