Skip to main content

Cohort Effects

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Geropsychology
  • 332 Accesses

Synonyms

Birth cohort; Cohort effect; Generation (e.g., Baby boomers); Generational shift

Definition

The term cohort refers to a group of people born at about the same time.

In geropsychology, the term cohort refers to a group of people born at about the same time. While it has long been considered a nuisance variable for age-based developmental studies, cohort is an important variable for many researchers in the social and health sciences because it provides evidence of secular changes. Studies of cohort effects on intelligence, reasoning, memory, and other cognitive abilities have garnered recent attention in both the academic and lay communities (Schaie et al. 2005; Williams 2013). A cogent example is the Flynn effect (Flynn 1987), which is the observation that generation by generation, people are becoming smarter with respect to intelligence test scores.

This entry will focus on cohort effects on cognitive performance primarily in adulthood and older age. After reviewing some of...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alwin, D., & McCammon, R. (2001). Aging, cohorts, and verbal ability. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences [serial online], 56B(3), S151–S161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In Theoretical models of human development (Handbook of child psychology 6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 569–664). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R., & de Frias, C. (2004). The Victoria longitudinal study: From characterizing cognitive aging to illustrating changes in memory compensation. Aging, Neuropsychology, And Cognition [serial online], 11(2–3), 346–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embretson, S. (1996). Item response theory models and spurious interaction effects in factorial ANOVA designs. Applied Psychological Measurement [serial online], 20(3), 201–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. (1987). Massive IQ, gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin [serial online], 101(2), 171–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J., & Weiss, L. (2007). American IQ gains from 1932 to 2002: The WISC subtests and educational progress. International Journal of Testing [serial online], 7(2), 209–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortier, I., & Ferretti, V. (2015). Retrieved from https://www.maelstrom-research.org

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Hoppmann, C., Willis, S., & Schaie, K. (2011). Cohort differences in cognitive aging and terminal decline in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Developmental Psychology [serial online], 47(4), 1026–1041.

    Google Scholar 

  • Health and Retirement Study, public use dataset. (2015). Produced and distributed by the University of Michigan with funding from the National Institute on Aging (grant number NIA U01AG009740). Ann Arbor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Kramer, A., Wilson, R., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest [serial online], 9(1), 1–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J., & Donaldson, G. (1976). On the myth of intellectual decline in adulthood. American Psychologist [serial online], 31(10), 701–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R. (2013). Who discovered the Flynn effect? A review of early studies of the secular increase of intelligence. Intelligence [serial online], 41(6), 765–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909–2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science [serial online], 10(3), 282–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rundquist, E. (1936). Intelligence test scores and school marks of high school seniors in 1929 and 1933. School & Society [serial online], 43, 301–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. (2015). Implications of the Flynn effect for age–cognition relations. Intelligence [serial online], 48, 51–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study (p. 144). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K., & Hofer, S. (2001). Longitudinal studies in aging research. In Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 53–77). San Diego: Academic [e-book].

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K., Willis, S., & Pennak, S. (2005). An historical framework for cohort differences in intelligence. Research in Human Development [serial online], 2(1–2), 43–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skirbekk, V., Stonawski, M., Bonsang, E., & Staudinger, U. (2013). The Flynn effect and population aging. Intelligence [serial online], 41(3), 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, T., & Owen, D. (2008). Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect. Intelligence [serial online], 36(2), 121–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuddenham, R. (1948). Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. American Psychologist [serial online], 3(2), 54–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M. (2010). Time-lag study. In N. J. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of research design (pp. 293–357). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R. (2013). Overview of the Flynn effect. Intelligence [serial online], 41(6), 753–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelinski, E., & Kennison, R. (2007). Not your parents’ test scores: Cohort reduces psychometric aging effects. Psychology and Aging [serial online], 22(3), 546–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelinski, E., Kennison, R., Watts, A., & Lewis, K. (2009). Convergence between cross-sectional and longitudinal studies: Cohort matters. In Aging and cognition: Research methodologies and empirical advances (pp. 101–118). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert F. Kennison .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media Singapore

About this entry

Cite this entry

Kennison, R.F., Situ, D., Reyes, N., Ahacic, K. (2016). Cohort Effects. In: Pachana, N. (eds) Encyclopedia of Geropsychology. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287-080-3_262-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287-080-3_262-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-287-080-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Social SciencesReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics