Skip to main content

Wild Anticipation: On the Evolution of Meaning

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbook of Anticipation
  • 1333 Accesses

Abstract

The present paper offers an approach to anticipation and meaning, based on Wild Systems Theory (WST), which begins by describing organisms as self-sustaining energy transformation systems that constitute embodiments of context. This idea leads to the assertion that anticipation refers to a self-sustaining system’s ability to prespecify and constrain the dynamic possibilities of its nested transformation systems. The paper describes how anticipation, defined as the prospective constraint of context, evolved from the small-scale contexts constrained by a single cell to the full-blown, self-aware prespecification and constraint of contexts (i.e., forward-looking thinking) exhibited in human anticipation. Specifically, anticipation scaled up because (1) the systems that phylogenetically entailed it (i.e., organisms) were simultaneous energy transformation systems whose status as such rendered them a possible energy source for potentially emergent energy transformation systems (i.e., plants and herbivores) and (2) as self-sustaining embodiments of context, such systems are naturally and necessarily “about” the contexts they embody. As a result, they are inherently meaningful, and the phenomenon we refer to as consciousness, or self-awareness, is a phylogenetically scaled-up recursion of the self-sustaining prespecification and constraint of nested, dynamic possibilities we see in single-cell organisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 549.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 649.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bauer, W. (2011). An argument for the extrinsic grounding of mass. Erkenntnis, 74(1), 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, R., & Atmanspacher, H. (2006). Contextual emergence in the description of properties. Foundations of Physics, 36, 1753–1777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself? Neuroreport, 11, 11–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 158, 1243–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., & Jordan, J. S. (2014). Prospective cognition in education and enculturation: An overview. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13(2), 139–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehmelt, H. (1989). Triton,...Electron,..., Cosmon...: An infinite regression? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 86, 8618–8619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget, M., & Grafton, S. (2003). Feedback or feedforward control: End of a dichotomy. In S. H. Johnson-Frey (Ed.), Taking action: Cognitive neuroscience perspectives on intentional acts (pp. 291–338). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dussutour, A., Fourcassie, V., Helbing, D., & Deneubourg, J. L. (2004). Optimal traffic organization in ants under crowded conditions. Nature, 428, 70–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, S. (2007). The limits of naturalism and the metaphysics of German idealism. In E. Hammer (Ed.), German idealism: Contemporary perspectives (pp. 19–49). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt, H. C., & Huber, F. (2002). Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghin, M. (2005). What a self could be. Psyche, 11(5), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golfinopoulos, E., Tourville, J. A., & Guenther, F. H. (2009). The integration of large-scale neural network modeling and functional brain imaging in speech motor control. NeuroImage, 52, 862–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grezes, J., Costes, N., & Decety, J. (1998). Top-down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: A PET investigation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(6/7/8), 553–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, T., & Jordan, J. S. (2014). Anticipation and embodied knowledge: Observations of enculturating bodies. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13(2), 272–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, T., & Jordan, J. S. (2017). Sensible objects: Intercorporeality and enactive knowing through things. In C. Meyer, J. Streeck, & J. S. Jordan (Eds). Intercorporeality: Emerging socialities in interaction (pp. 267–288). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, L., Illes, A., & Vehrencamp, S. L. (2006). Overlapping signals in banded wrens: Long-term effects of prior experience on males and females. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 260–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R. (1986). Varieties of realism: A rationale for the natural sciences. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeyr, J. H. S. (2007). The biochemical factory that autonomously fabricates itself: A systems biological view of the living cell. Systems biology: Philosophical foundations, 217–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding others: Imitation, language and empathy. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From mirror neurons to memes (pp. 77–99). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 304–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jammer, M. (2000). Concepts of mass in contemporary physics and philosophy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2003a). The embodiment of intentionality. In W. Tschacher & J. Dauwalder (Eds.), Dynamical systems approaches to embodied cognition (pp. 201–228). Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2003b). Emergence of self and other in perception and action. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 633–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2006). Born to be wild: Faust, Pinocchio and the Marlboro man meet the embodied other. Mitteilungen, 3, 5–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2008a). Toward a theory of embodied communication: Self-sustaining wild systems as embodied meaning. In I. Wachsmuth, M. Lenzen, & G. Knoblich (Eds.), Embodied communication in human and machines (pp. 53–75). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2008b). Wild-agency: Nested intentionalities in neuroscience and archeology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B (Biological Sciences), 363, 1981–1991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2009). Forward-looking aspects of perception-action coupling as a basis for embodied communication. Discourse Processes, 46, 127–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2010a). Wild systems theory: Overcoming the computational-ecological divide via self-sustaining systems. In Proceedings of the 32nd annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Portland: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2010b). Shusterman, Merleau-Ponty, and Dewey: The role of pragmatism in the conversation of embodiment. Action, Criticism & Theory for Music Education, 9(1), 67–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2012). What’s new in new realism: A review of Edwin Pierce’s a new look at new realism [Review of the Book A New Look at New Realism]. PsycCRITIQUES: Contemporary Psychology – APA Review of Books, 57, Article 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2013a). Seeing through the noise: Where was this Guy headed? Ecological Psychology, 25, 219–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S. (2013b). The wild ways of conscious will: What we do, how we do it, and why it has meaning. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Day, B. (2015). Wild systems theory as a 21st century coherence framework for cognitive science. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND: 21(T). Frankfurt: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Ghin, M. (2006). (Proto-) consciousness as a contextually-emergent property of self-sustaining systems. Mind & Matter, 4(1), 45–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Heidenreich, B. (2010). The intentional nature of self-sustaining systems. Mind & Matter, 8, 45–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Ghin, M. (2007). The role of control in a science of consciousness: Causality, regulation and selfsustainment. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(1–2), 177–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Knoblich, G. (2004). Spatial perception and control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(1), 54–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Mays, C. (2017). Wild meaning: The intercorporeal nature of objects, bodies, and words. In C. Meyer, J. Streeck, & J. S. Jordan (Eds). Intercorporeality: Emerging socialities in interaction (pp. 361–378). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, S., & Ranade, E. (2014). Multiscale entrainment: A primer in prospective cognition for educational researchers. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13(2), 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J.S., & Vandervert, L. (1999). Liberal education as a reflection of our assumptions regarding truth and consciousness: Time for an integrative philosophy. Modeling Consciousness Across the Disciplines, 307–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Vinson, D. (2012). After nature: On bodies, consciousness, and causality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19, 229–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., & Wesselmann, E. D. (2015). The contextually grounded nature of prosocial behavior: A multiscale, embodied approach to morality. In D. A. Schroeder & W. G. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior. New York: Oxford University Press. Published online November 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195399813.013.031.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. S., Bai, J., Cialdella, V., & Schloesser, D. (2015). Foregrounding the context: Cognitive science as the study of embodied context. In E. Dzhafarov & J. S. Jordan (Eds.), Contextuality from physics to psychology (pp. 209–228). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kauffman, S. (1995). At home in the universe. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawato, M., Furukawa, K., & Suzuki, R. (1987). A hierarchical neural-network model for control and learning of voluntary movement. Biological Cybernetics, 57(3), 169–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (2002). The role of imitation in body ownership and mental growth. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 311–330). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., & Jordan, J. S. (2009). Embodied anticipation: A neurodevelopmental interpretation. Discourse Processes, 46, 103–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., & Jordan, J. S. (2003). Action coordination in groups and individuals: Learning anticipatory control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 1006–1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koziol, L., & Lutz, J. (2013). From movement to thought: The development of executive function. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 2(2), 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koziol, L., Budding, D., & Chidekel, D. (2011). From movement to thought: Executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. The Cerebellum, 11, 505–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lotka, A. J. (1945). The law of evolution as a maximal principle. Human Biology, 17, 167–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H.R., & Varela, F.J. (1980). Problems in the neurophysiology of cognition. In Autopoiesis and cognition (pp. 41–47). Springer Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., & Boag, P. T. (2003). Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. Science, 296, 873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2003). Being no one. The self-model theory of subjectivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miall, R. C. (2003). Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. Neuroreport, 14, 2135–2137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odum, H. T. (1988). Self-organization, transformity, and information. Science, 242, 132–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulin, M. G. (1993). The role of the cerebellum in motor control and perception. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 41(1), 39–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, E., Pargetter, R., & Jackson, F. (1982). Three theses about dispositions. American Philosophical Quarterly, 19, 251–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2002). From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 247–266). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, R. (1985). Anticipatory systems: Philosophical, mathematical. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffer, J. (2003). Is there a fundamental level? Noûs, 37(3), 498–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., & Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 119–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr, R., & Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 359–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1976). About behaviorism. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J., & Jordan, J. S. (2009). Communication as a dynamical self-sustaining system: The importance of time-scales and nested contexts. Communication Theory, 19, 445–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert, L. (1995). Chaos theory and the evolution of consciousness and mind: A thermodynamic-holographic resolution to the mind-body problem. New Ideas in Psychology, 13(2), 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wesselmann, E., & Jordan, J. S. (2016). Wild heroes: The complexity of being “moral”. In T. Langley (Ed.), The psychology of civil war (pp. 111–124). New York: Sterling.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 338–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 358, 593–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Scott Jordan .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Scott Jordan, J. (2019). Wild Anticipation: On the Evolution of Meaning. In: Poli, R. (eds) Handbook of Anticipation. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91554-8_59

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics