Background

Analysis of functional and effective connectivity across distributed brain regions can provide new insight into how the brain functions. ‘Functional’ connectivity considers only the correlation between signals while ‘effective’ connectivity also maps the causal relationships between signals [1]. The brain contains a highly complex collection of neurons that interact and communicate in order to perform motor and cognitive actions. Many studies have examined human brain connectivity using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and Positron-Emission Tomography (PET) by treating image voxels as anatomical network nodes [26]. A drawback to these methods is that they require subjects to remain still during imaging, resulting in the study of constrained and somewhat artificial behaviors.

One way to study brain connectivity during more natural, whole body, behaviors is to combine high-density electroencephalography (EEG), Independent Component Analysis (ICA), and source localization techniques. We have recently demonstrated that high-density electroencephalography (EEG) combined with Independent Component Analysis (ICA) enables the study of electrocortical activity related to locomotor control and cognition during walking [79]. Clustering electrocortical sources across subjects according to spatial and spectral properties enables calculation of changes in effective connectivity between cortical regions using techniques such as Granger causality [10].

Previous studies have demonstrated that the cortex plays a significant role in postural control during standing [11, 12] but there is much less information about cortical control of human walking. Walking relies heavily on spinal locomotor networks that are capable of generating rhythmic muscle activity [1316]. While considerable evidence has documented the importance of spinal central pattern generators in non-human vertebrates, spinally generated locomotor activity in humans without functional descending motor pathways has proven to be difficult to document [13, 1719]. Transcranial magnetic stimulation [2022] and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) [2327] have been used to investigate cortical connectivity during locomotion. These approaches each have limitations in measurement area or temporal resolution that restrict their use to assess cortical network connectivity during gait. For example, fNIRS is limited to measuring activity in the outer cortex and the temporal resolution is limited to a few seconds at best.

The purpose of this paper was to assess the relative effective cortical connectivity in humans during walking and standing. We examined cortical connectivity in healthy young subjects under the following four conditions: walking while performing a simple cognitive task, walking without the concurrent task, standing while performing a simple cognitive task, and standing without the concurrent task. We hypothesized that the effective connectivity among independent sensorimotor electrocortical processes would be lower during walking than during standing. We based this hypothesis on the belief that standing in humans is predominantly controlled by supraspinal mechanisms and walking in human relies substantially on spinal neural networks.The inclusion of a concurrently performed cognitive task allowed us to examine whether these changes were consistent in the presence of competing attentional demands [28].

Methods

Experimental design

Eight healthy volunteers (7 males and 1 female) between the ages of 20–31 years participated in the study. None had any history of major lower limb injury or known neurological or locomotor deficits. All subjects were provided with, and signed, consent forms prior to the experiment. All procedures were approved by the University of Michigan Internal Review Board and complied with the standards defined in the Declaration of Helsinki. All processing and analysis was performed in Matlab (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) using scripts based on EEGLAB (sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab), an open source environment for processing electrophysiological data [29] or the Granger Causality Toolbox [30].

Subjects stood (0.0 m/s) and walked (0.8 m/s and 1.25 m/s) on a treadmill while we recorded 248-channel electroencephalography at 512 Hz (ActiveTwo, BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). Before data collection, the locations of the electrodes were measured with respect to anatomical head reference points and electrode gel was used to bring electrode impedance below 25 kΩ. Subjects participated in a visual odd-ball discrimination and response task for some of the data collection. Standard (80%) and target (20%) stimuli (vertical or 45° rotated black crosses on a white background, respectively) were displayed on a monitor placed at eye level about 1 m in front of the subjects. For each movement condition subjects completed one test block where they actively responded to the target stimuli by pressing a handheld trigger (we refer to this condition as engaged) and one test block where they passively observed the screen (we refer to this condition as passive). Each session began with the standing condition (5 minutes), followed by the walking conditions (10 minutes each) in random order.

Identical preprocessing steps were applied across all subjects and trials. Furthermore, data were appended for identical subjects across sessions to compute identical statistics and allow for identical IC/dipole comparison between sessions. This approach also allows for robust comparisons to be made between the session, as you will see in the following sections. After collection, EEG data were high-pass filtered above 1 Hz. 60 Hz line noise was also removed. As in [7, 8], EEG signals exhibiting substantial noise throughout the collection were removed from the data in the following manner: 1) channels with std. dev. > 1000 μV were removed, 2) any channel whose kurtosis was more than 5 std. dev. from the mean was removed, and 3) channels that were uncorrelated (r < 0.4) with nearby channels for more than 1% of the time-samples were removed.

Prior to performing ICA decomposition, time periods of EEG with substantial artifact, as defined by z-transformed power across all channels, across the appended subject specific sessions, in a given 2 second time window being larger than 0.8, were rejected using EEGLAB. An average of 130.4 EEG channels were retained for analysis (Range - 89–164; STD - 24.6). We refer the reader to [79], for a more in depth overview of this approach. These remaining channel signals were then re-referenced to an average reference across the scalp and mastoid external channels. The remaining channels and epochs were input into to an adaptive mixture ICA algorithm [AMICA] [31, 32] that utilizes the infomax [33] approach. ICA linearly decomposes EEG signals into a set of maximally independent components (ICs) [34].

Data analysis

We then estimated an anatomical source location for each IC using DIPFIT functions within EEGLAB [35]. DIPFIT computes an equivalent current dipole model that best explains the scalp topography of each IC using a boundary element head model. ICs were excluded if the projection of the equivalent current dipole to the scalp accounted for less than 85% of the scalp map variance, or if the topography, time-course, and spectra of the IC were reflective of eye movement or electromyographic artifact [36, 37]. The remaining ICs then reflected electrocortical sources. These sources were clustered across subjects using EEGLAB routines that implemented k-means clustering on vectors jointly coding differences in equivalent dipole locations and power spectra. Prior to clustering, the resulting joint vector was reduced to 10 principal dimensions using principal component analysis, as in (Gwin, Gramann et al. 2011), setting the maximal number of clusters. Clusters of electrocortical sources existed in prefrontal (PFC)(5 sources), anterior cingulate (AC) (9 sources), posterior parietal (PC) (13 sources), left sensorimotor (LSM) (7 sources), and right sensorimotor cortex (RSM) (6 sources) (Figure 1). The effective number of sources used may have been smaller depending on the significance/strength of connections coming from them.

Figure 1
figure 1

Granger causality network connections for all subjects while walking at 1.25 m/s while not engaged in the visual oddball task. Nodes shown are from statistically significant connections aggregated across all subjects. Nodes were in slightly different locations for each subject, some subjects had multiple nodes/region some had none. The thicker the line, the stronger the Granger Causality value. Nodes are clustered and color coated per their anatomical regions. Significant connections were determined by the F-statistic.

We applied conditional Granger causality analysis to each subject’s electrocortical source signals to assess effective connectivity between the cortical regions. Granger causality is a directional connectivity measure that determines the linear causal influence of one signal on another [30, 38, 39] through a statistical test of the null hypothesis that a particular signal ‘Granger causes’ another signal. The postulate is that each signal is a linear combination of all other signals at a previous time (i.e., the order parameter). A full mathematical description of Granger causality has been published previously [39, 40]. In short the linear regression is,

X i t = j = i k A ij X j t - τ + E t - τ

where X i is a signal at time t, X j = X i…k are all signals in the system/network, τ is the order parameter or time lag, and E is the residual. A signal is considered to ‘Granger cause’ another if, by adding the signal to the system, E is sufficiently reduced.

We calculated conditional temporal Granger causality values for all intra-subject electrocortical source signals with the aid of the freely distributed Granger Causality Toolbox [30]. To compute Granger causality, electrocortical source signals were divided into 10 second epochs before being demeaned and detrended. The best order parameter for each pair of electrocortical sources was then computed using the Bayesian information criterion and, on average, corresponded to about 20 ms, with a low of 15 ms and high of 25 ms. Varying the order parameter within this range, for all pairs, did not greatly affect the connectivity values. IC/dipole time series were checked for stationary using the Kwiatkowski-Phillips-Schmidt-Shin test and the Augmented Dickey Fuller test, and were not used in the connectivity analysis if they did not pass both. Granger causality values representing connectivity between electrocortical sources within the same brain region were ignored. Figure 1 demonstrates the anatomical locations of these regions, for all subjects, while walking at 1.25 m/s. For clarity only the most significant connections (determined by the F-statistic) are shown.

Average Granger causality values were computed for each intra-subject pair of electrocortical sources, within each condition, across the 10 second epochs. We evaluated the difference in connectivity strengths for each pair of electrocortical sources between walking and standing; cognitively engaged and cognitively passive conditions were treated separately. In addition, during standing we evaluated the difference in connectivity strengths for each pair of electrocortical sources between the cognitively engaged and cognitively passive conditions. Next, pairs of sources were grouped as sensorimotor if at least one of the sources in the pair was from the left or right sensorimotor cluster and non-sensorimotor otherwise. Connections coupling identical regions across subjects were then grouped and averaged. One-sample t-tests assessed whether the differences in connectivity strengths within each group (i.e., sensorimotor and non-sensorimotor) were significant; the p-value threshold was 0.05.

We also analyzed the Granger Causality differences using a second approach to ensure statistical validity. Instead of averaging similar region-region connections for each subject, then averaging across subjects, all sensorimotor and non-sensorimotor connections were binned into respective groups, and averages were taken for each. Then 10,000 surrogate data sets were created by taking the same connections (calculated over 10 second epochs) and randomly shuffling the regions they connected. The average connectivity for the surrogate sensorimotor regions and non-sensorimotor regions were then calculated as the statistical baseline, and the standard deviation of this surrogate distribution was used as our confidence level. For the sensorimotor regions there were an average of 10.5/16 connections per subject, and for the non-sensorimotor regions there were an average of 5.5/9 connections per subject.

Results

The Granger causality analyses identified three main changes in connectivity among brain regions across conditions (Additional file 1: Table S1). First, connections involving the sensorimotor cortex (including both communication with association areas and interhemispheric communication within the sensorimotor cortex) were significantly weaker during walking than during standing. This was true regardless of whether the subject was actively engaged in the visual oddball discrimination and response task (p < 0.03) or the subject was passively observing the screen (p < 0.001) (Figures 2a & 2b, blue bars). Second, effective connectivities involving non-sensorimotor areas (i.e., prefrontal, posterior, and anterior clusters) were significantly stronger during walking than standing only when subjects were engaged in the simultaneous cognitive task (p < 0.03) (Figure 2b, red bars). Third, during standing, effective connectivities involving non-sensorimotor areas were significantly weaker when subjects were actively engaged in the visual oddball discrimination and response task than when subjects passively observed the visual stimuli (p < 0.02) (Figure 2c, red bars).

Figure 2
figure 2

Grand average percentage changes in network connectivity between conditions. Connectivity during walking minus connectivity during standing is shown for sensorimotor (blue) and non-sensorimotor (red) network nodes when subjects were not engaged in the cognitive task (panel a) and when subjects were engaged in the cognitive task (panel b). Walking significant decreases sensorimotor network connectivity compared to standing and, when subjects are actively engaged in the cognitive task (panel b), walking significantly increase non-sensorimotor network connectivity. Panel c shows connectivity during active standing (actively engaged in the cognitive task) minus connectivity during passive (not engaged in the cognitive task). Passive standing elicited greater non-sensorimotor network connectivity than active standing, demonstrating the expected non-sensorimotor default mode network. * = p < 0.04.

The second statistical analysis provided similar results to the primary. Some important results are stated here and all data are presented in Table 1. For the passive case subjects showed an average connectivity decrease of 13.38% in sensorimotor regions for walking at 0.8 m/s compared to standing. The surrogate sample produced an average decrease of -7.62 ± 4.91%. Similarly, a connectivity decrease of 20.64% was measured in sensorimotor regions for walking at 1.2 m/s compared to standing. The surrogate sample produced an average decrease of -9.57 ± 5.40%. For the active case subjects showed an average connectivity decrease of 9.31% in sensorimotor regions for walking at 0.8 m/s compared to standing. The surrogate sample produced an average increase of 3.87 ± 6.16%. Similarly, a connectivity decrease of 7.73% was measured in sensorimotor regions for walking at 1.2 m/s compared to standing. The surrogate sample produced an average increase of 2.06 ± 5.32%.

Table 1 Connectivity differences vs. random surrogate differences

Matrix representations of directional network connectivity are shown in Figures 2, 3, and 4. In these figures, connections are from brain areas plotted vertically to brain areas plotted horizontally. The sensorimotor network nodes are outlined in red and the non-sensorimotor network nodes are outlined in green. Statistically significant differences in connection strength (more than 2 standard errors from 0) are indicated by gold plus signs (increases) and blue minus sings (decreases).

Figure 3
figure 3

Connectivity diagrams when subjects were not engaged in the cognitive task for (first column) walking, (second column) standing, and (third column) the connectivity strength differences between walking and standing (i.e., walking minus standing). Walking speeds were (first row) 0.8 m/s and (second row) 1.25 m/s. The sensorimotor network nodes (left sensorimotor (LSM) and right sensorimotor (RSM)) are outlined in red and the non-sensorimotor network nodes (prefrontal cluster (PFC), posterior parietal cortex (PC), and anterior cingulate (AC)) are outlined in green. Statistically significant increases or decreases in connectivity strength that are more than two standard errors from zero are identified by gold plus signs and blue minus signs, respectively. All significant changes in connectivity strength between walking and standing within the sensorimotor network, at both speeds, were negative (i.e., functional connectivity involving sensorimotor areas was weaker during walking than during standing). The diagonal blocks are brown because no connectivity strengths were calculated for connections within the same brain area. GC Connectivity Strength can range from 0–1.

Figure 4
figure 4

Connectivity diagrams when subjects were actively engaged in the cognitive task for (first column) walking, (second column) standing, and (third column) the connectivity strength differences between walking and standing (i.e., walking minus standing). Walking speeds were (first row) 0.8 m/s and (second row) 1.25 m/s. The sensorimotor network nodes (left sensorimotor (LSM) and right sensorimotor (RSM)) are outlined in red and the non-sensorimotor network nodes (prefrontal (PF), posterior parietal cortex (PC), and anterior cingulate (AC)) are outlined in green. Statistically significant increases or decreases in connectivity strength that are more than two standard errors from zero are identified by gold plus signs and blue minus signs, respectively. Nearly all significant changes in connectivity strength between walking and standing within the sensorimotor network, at both speeds, were negative (i.e., functional connectivity involving sensorimotor areas was weaker during walking than during standing). All significant changes in connectivity strength within the non-sensorimotor network, at both speeds, were positive (i.e., when actively engaged in a cognitive task, walking enhances connectivity among non-sensorimotor network nodes). The diagonal blocks are brown because no connectivity strengths were calculated for connections within the same brain area.

Regardless of whether or not subjects were actively engaged in the cognitive task, nearly all pairwise significant differences in connectivity strength, between walking at both speeds and standing, within the sensorimotor network, were negative (Figures 2 & 3). Connectivity involving sensorimotor areas was weaker during walking than during standing independent of the concurrent cognitive task. Note that Figure 3, which contains connectivity diagrams for standing and walking when subjects were not actively engaged in the cognitive task, shows the data used to calculate the grand averages shown in Figure 2a. Figure 4, which contains connectivity diagrams for when subjects were performing the visual oddball discrimination and response task, shows the data used to calculate the grand averages shown in Figure 2b.

When subjects were not actively engaged in the cognitive task, there were heterogeneous changes in the pairwise connectivity strength changes for the non-sensorimotor network. However, connectivity strengths between the PC and AC of electrocortical sources tended to be greater during walking than during standing (Figure 3). When subjects were actively engaged in the oddball discrimination task, all statistically significant (2 standard errors) differences in connectivity strength within the non-sensorimotor network, at both walking speeds, were positive (Figure 4). This indicates that walking uniformly enhanced connectivity among non-sensorimotor network nodes for the cognitive task condition.

When comparing the no cognitive task standing to the cognitive task standing, all statistically significant differences in connectivity strength within the non-sensorimotor network were negative (Figure 5). Specifically, statistically significant weakening occurred for the prefrontal to/from PC cluster connections (bi-directionally) and for the AC to prefrontal cluster connection (uni-directionally). Note that Figure 5, which contains connectivity diagrams during standing with the cognitive task and without the cognitive task, shows the data used for the grand averages shown in Figure 2c.

Figure 5
figure 5

Connectivity diagrams during standing (first column) when subjects were actively engaged in the cognitive task, (second column) when not actively engaged in the cognitive task, and (third column) the connectivity strength differences between active engagement and passive observation (i.e., cognitively active minus cognitively passive). The sensorimotor network nodes (left sensorimotor (LSM) and right sensorimotor (RSM)) are outlined in red and the non-sensorimotor network nodes (prefrontal cortex(PFC), posterior parietal cortex (PC), and anterior cingulate (AC)) are outlined in green. Statistically significant increases or decreases in connectivity strength that are more than two standard errors from zero are identified by gold plus signs and blue minus signs, respectively. All significant changes in connectivity strength within the non-sensorimotor network were negative (i.e., the cognitive network was suppressed when subjects actively engaged in the cognitive task). The diagonal blocks are brown because no connectivity strengths were calculated for connections within the same brain area.

Discussion

We found a distinct, homogeneous, weakening in electrocortical sensorimotor network connectivity for walking compared to standing. While not all connectivity changed reached significance, all that did showed a sensorimotor weakening linked to walking (except for the PC-RSM conenction between 0.8 m/s and 0.0 m/s while cognitively engaged). Conversely, while engaged, the subjects showed a uniform increase in non-sensorimotor activity when walking. The most likely explanation for this finding is that standing requires considerable active cortical control for maintaining balance and posture [11, 12], while walking relies more on spinal neural networks for producing the dominant muscle activation patterns. Given that locomotion requires less coordinated input from the brain than standing, it is not surprising that there is a measurable decrease in connectivity in sensorimotor networks during walking. Although there have been several studies using functional near-infrared spectroscopy to document changes in cortical activation during human locomotion [2327, 41] and during active balancing [42], there are no studies that have compared cortical activation during standing and walking. An important difference between the results presented here and previous studies analyzing cortical activation during walking with functional near-infrared spectroscopy is that our analyses focused on effective connectivity rather than just overall activity assessed through cerebral blood flow. The two approaches are not identical and are likely to result in some differences due to their measurements.

An important point to highlight is that there are many supraspinal neural substrates involved in the control of human locomotion other than just cortical areas. The brain stem, cerebellum, hippocampus, and basal ganglia also play a substantial role in the control of human walking and running [4346]. Our study is limited in only being able to document cortical areas with spectral power synchronized with the gait cycle [7]. Future research that includes subject-specific head models for source localization and more advanced blind source separation algorithms might have more success in identifying other supraspinal sources involved in human locomotor control [47].

Another significant finding of this study was the increase in non-sensorimotor network connectivity strength during dual-task walking (i.e., walking while performing a cognitive task) compared to dual-task standing (i.e., standing while performing a cognitive task). This suggests that when engaged in a cognitive task, the act of walking increases the connectivity of the non-sensorimotor network while reducing the connectivity in the sensorimotor network. The data presented here do not allow us to probe the neurophysiological underpinnings of this observation. However, prior research leads to two possible interpretations. First, locomotion may enhance the performance and integration of brain regions associated with cognitive processing. Brisswalter et al. recently reviewed the evidence for acute exercise effects on cognitive performance and concluded that exercise improves non-motor performance [48]. Second, increased non-sensorimotor network connectivity may reflect the additional processing and communication needed for dual tasking. Walking at a controlled treadmill speed, where position on the treadmill must be continually monitored to prevent drifting off the belt, in addition to the cognitive demands of the oddball discrimination task may have required additional information processing in the non-sensorimotor network compared to standing while dual tasking [28].

In this study, we did not find consistent network differences between the two cognitive loading levels during walking. One possibility is that the connectivity effects of walking dominate over the effect of the simple cognitive task we employed. Another limitation of the study was the inability to assess the effect of walking speed given that only two walking speeds were evaluated. Future studies should examine a wider range of walking speeds and additional types of cognitive loading.

Unfortunately, our current approach does not allow for robust network analysis and graph theoretical approaches because of the limited number of nodes (electrocortical sources) that are extracted from EEG using ICA. If more electrocortical sources could be extracted then the non-sensorimotor network could likely be broken down into multiple other brain networks (e.g., default-mode and attentional networks). Additionally, integration across small and large time scales, as well as short and long distances, may provide valuable insights into the distributed processing of brain networks.

It is important to note the effect ICA has on the Granger causality measures. Although ICA extracts maximally independent signals, these signals are maximally simultaneously independent. On the other hand, Granger causality measures the contribution of one signal to another at time lags (1–20 ms in our case). Granger Causality is, therefore, well-suited for identifying causal relations between independent electrocortical source signals. In fact, Granger causality is susceptible to volume conduction in EEG measurements [49] but ICA can effectively remove instantaneous correlations due to volume conduction [50]. In contrast, the effects of the ICA transforms, coupled with other standard processing techniques like the removal of nearby uncorrelated channels may account for the overall low GC strengths (Figure 2) of ~ 0.01. It is likely that these techniques, along with the consideration of only a few specific anatomical regions, limits our computation of the complete causal network map.

It is also important to note that the amount of activity in a particular cortical region may not be correlated with the effective connectivity of that region. Therefore, increased effective connectivity does not necessarily indicate increased cortical activation. While previous cognitive studies show both increased network connectivity and increased overall activity [5153], we have only demonstrated increased connectivity of the sensorimotor networks for standing compared to walking.

GC is also susceptible to detecting indirect connections between sources. Future work should investigate measures such as direct Directed Transfer Function (dDTF) and partial Directed Coherence (PDC). However the limited number of sources in this analysis would likely create extremely sparse networks. GC therefore is optimal for detecting closely coupled sources of activity (either directly or an open triplet). We believe that both cases are important for the experiment presented here.

Conclusions

In summary, we found that effective sensorimotor connectivity was reduced during walking compared to standing, and that when humans are engaged in a cognitive task walking increased the effective connectivity of non-sensorimotor brain regions. These findings provide insight into how cortical regions interact during human gait and demonstrate the potential for future research studies to examine cortical connectivity in other mobile tasks. In particular, we have considerable optimism for applying these techniques to clinical populations with gait impairments such as ataxia and freezing gait.