FormalPara Key Points

A meta-analysis including 63 independent datasets was conducted

Use of legal supplements, perceived social norms, and positive attitudes towards doping were strong positive correlates of doping intentions and behaviors

Morality and self-efficacy to refrain from doping were negatively associated with doping intentions and behaviors

1 Introduction

According to the World Anti-Doping Agency (WADA) [1], doping behavior refers to the use of illegal performance-enhancing drugs and methods to improve performance. Anti-doping rules and regulations in sport are founded upon the premise that doping use violates the rules of competition and the spirit of sport, and can lead to health risks in users [1]. Nonetheless, doping behaviors are not uncommon across all levels of sport and exercise participation [2, 3]. In a study conducted in a gym setting, more than 10 % of participants self-reported engaging in doping behaviors [4]. Amongst competitive bodybuilders, the figures are as high as 77.8 % [5]. In competitive sport, the prevalence statistics are wide-ranging and appear to be dependent on the measurement technique employed. Questionnaire-based evidence indicates that approximately 10–15 % of elite and sub-elite athletes reported using doping substances in the past [6]. The use of indirect measures (e.g. implicit association test or the randomized response technique) indicates that the prevalence of doping use in competitive sport can be as high as 30 % [79]. The Athlete Biological Passport paradigm has also provided estimates of the prevalence of blood doping. Analysis of samples from 2,737 international athletes provided a mean estimate of 14 % for the combined sample, but subpopulation analysis highlighted wide-ranging estimates from 1 to 48 % [10]. All these figures are in stark contrast to the fact that, year on year, less than 2 % of drug tests analyzed by WADA laboratories prove positive.

Therefore, despite the efforts of WADA, the International Olympic Committee (IOC) and global sports federations, drug use in sport and exercise settings is on the rise [11], with a recent high-profile case being that of the cyclist Lance Armstrong. The reality is that detection techniques are not keeping pace with developments in the pharmaceutical industry [12] and the need for anti-doping education is greater than ever [11]. Thus, preventive and educational action must be afforded greater attention and resources to reduce instances of doping. Such preventative action necessitates an understanding of the psychosocial predictors of doping intentions and behavior. Research examining such predictors has significantly increased over the last decade. As such, a number of positive and negative correlates at the personal and socio-contextual level (e.g. achievement goal orientations, moral values, social norms) have been identified. In line with health-related behavior research, most studies in the doping field have adopted the theory of planned behavior (TPB) [13, 14] as a broader theoretical background to examine the psychosocial mechanisms that may lead to doping in sport. In this theory, behavior is viewed as a distal or proximal outcome of (i) intention, (ii) attitude, (iii) subjective norm, and (iv) perceived behavioral control. Attitudes represent an individual’s positive or negative evaluations of performing the behavior in question. Subjective norms are conceptualized as the pressure that individuals perceive from significant others to engage (or not) in the target behavior. Perceived behavioral control represents one’s evaluation about their capabilities to perform the behavior. As such, it may reflect obstacles encountered in past behavioral performances. Attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioral control are proposed to influence behavior through their influence on intention to perform the behavior. Hence, intentions represent an individual’s proximal predictor of doping behavior.

Researchers utilizing TPB have documented the ability of doping attitudes, perceived behavioral control and subjective norms to predict doping intention and self-reported doping behavior (e.g. Goulet et al. [1517], Lazuras et al. [1517], and Lucidi et al. [1517]). These studies have involved a variety of populations, including elite athletes [16], gym users [18], and adolescents [17, 19], suggesting generalizability of findings across different samples and settings. Alongside the core TPB variables, researchers have also used constructs outside the TPB framework to predict doping intentions and behaviors. The extension of the TPB approach was undertaken because of (a) the need to investigate morality issues, bearing in mind that doping is considered an immoral behavior; (b) recent developments in TPB research in other areas of enquiry (e.g. smoking), showing that the addition of new constructs (i.e. descriptive norms, situational temptation) improves the predictive ability of the TPB model; and (c) recent arguments suggesting the use of integrative approaches [14, 20] to incorporate distal predictors of behavior (e.g. sportspersonship, moral disengagement, or achievement goals in the case of doping) and demographics. As an example of such new approaches, Lucidi et al. [17] measured participants’ moral disengagement (i.e. disassociating oneself from the moral implications of one’s unethical actions) and showed that this variable predicted doping intentions and behaviors. Barkoukis et al. [21] found that athletes with high sportspersonship (e.g. prosocial attitudes and behaviors), autonomous motivation (i.e. motivation resulting from enjoyment or personal value) and mastery-oriented achievement goals (i.e. emphasis on personal improvement and effort) reported lower doping intentions compared with those with low sportspersonship, controlled motivation (i.e. motivation resulting from pressure, social approval, or feelings of guilt) and performance-oriented achievement goals (i.e. emphasis on displaying normative superiority). Mixed evidence accompanies research exploring the demographic predictors of doping use. For instance, males, compared with females, have been found to be at a greater risk for doping across samples of adolescents [22] and competitive athletes [23, 24]. However, Wiefferink et al. [18] did not find any effects of sex or age on self-reported use of doping substances in a sample of gym and fitness users.

The aforementioned findings highlight that doping use is complex and is probably predicted by a variety of situational and personal variables. The vast majority of studies in this area are correlational in design and doping behavior is self-reported. Anti-doping interventions from a psychosocial perspective are scarce [25]. As an exception, Elliot et al. [26] designed an intervention consisting of eight 45-min meetings held during sport team training in US high schools. This was a peer-led intervention which did not focus exclusively on doping; students engaged in cooperative learning exercises regarding healthy nutrition and strength training regimes, as well as the effects of legal and illegal supplements and other substances on performance. The intervention was sex-specific targeting different aspect of males’ (i.e. steroid use; project adolescents training and learning to avoid steroids—ATLAS) and females’ (i.e. disordered eating behavior; project athletes targeting healthy exercise and nutrition alternatives—ATHENA) behaviors. Using a randomized control trial design, Elliot et al. showed that the program significantly reduced the use of recreational drugs and doping substances in participants 1–3 years after the students graduated from high school. However, the effect sizes found were very small.

To date, only one systematic narrative literature review has been performed to synthesize the research findings in this area [25]. The authors reviewed 103 articles focusing on attitudes, values and beliefs towards doping, predictors and precipitating factors in anti-doping education and prevention programs. Most included studies examined attitudes towards doping. Only 21 studies examined the use of performance-enhancing drugs, and most of those studies examined risk factors for anabolic steroid use amongst weight lifters or body builders. As such, the findings from this review may not be generalizable to other populations and settings, and are better suited for understanding attitudes toward doping but not necessarily doping intention and use. By conducting a meta-analysis of existing studies, researchers can provide a vigorous test of theoretical frameworks utilized in the field of doping, by incorporating a large number of studies using different study designs and including participants with varying demographic backgrounds. A meta-analysis can accumulate, quantify, and summarize existing empirical evidence controlling for sampling and measurement errors that cannot be taken into account by narrative reviews. By quantifying the magnitude of the size of effect between variables, and testing the homogeneity of this effect across different groups or contexts, researchers can identify key psychosocial predictors of doping intention and behavior, so that resources are specifically targeted on these risk factors. Furthermore, based on the results of meta-analysis, researchers may refine existing theoretical frameworks or develop new ones that could better explain the links between demographic and psychosocial variables with doping intention and behavior.

1.1 Objectives of the Current Study

In order to provide a statistical synthesis of research findings in the literature, we conducted a meta-analysis of existing studies. The first aim of this study was to identify the strongest psychosocial correlates (both positive and negative) of doping intentions and use (behaviors). The studies reporting the results of the ATLAS/ATHENA interventions were included in our meta-analysis. Although these studies did not focus exclusively on doping, doping behavior was one of their outcome measures and hence they met our inclusion criteria. In addition to psychological (e.g. attitudes) and social-contextual factors (e.g. social norms), the predictive effects of demographic variables (e.g. sex and age) on doping intentions and behaviors were also examined. Results from intervention studies were also meta-analyzed in order to examine the effectiveness of anti-doping interventions by comparing the findings between the experimental and control arms. By conducting a meta-analysis, one can examine whether real effects (i.e. effects that are unlikely to be due to chance) exist between two variables. Moreover, the size of such effects could be calculated using a standardized metric, which is useful for comparison purposes.

The second aim of this study was to identify moderator variables (i.e. whether the effect size associated with the relationship between two variables varies at different levels of the moderator variable). Testing for moderators is important because researchers can identify whether certain effects, such as the influence of social norms on doping intentions, need to be dealt with differently in various groups (e.g. males vs. females) or in certain contexts (e.g. competitive athletes vs. gym users). Furthermore, moderator analysis could help researchers to explore whether doping interventions are equally effective across populations and settings. The third aim of this study was to test a path analysis model, using the meta-analyzed effect sizes, using variables proposed by TPB [13]. We chose TPB for the path analysis because most of the empirical studies in the extant literature were based on this theory.

2 Methods

2.1 Information Sources and Search Strategy

A literature search was conducted to identify studies that could be included in the meta-analysis. PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, PubMed, Google Scholar, and MEDLINE were searched using combinations of keywords specifying the subject (e.g. doping, doping intention, steroid) and the context of interest (e.g. sport), with no date limits. The full electronic search strategy is described in the electronic supplementary material (ESM) Appendix S1. In addition, the WADA website was searched for reports of previous studies funded by the agency, and the reference lists of included articles were scanned for relevant manuscripts that were not retrieved from the database searches. To locate and retrieve information from unpublished datasets, messages were posted on electronic mailing lists (e.g. SPORTPSY) reaching out to social science researchers. Authors with a background in doping research were also invited, via personal email requests, to contact us if they had any unpublished information that was relevant to our study.

2.2 Eligibility Criteria and Study Selection

Studies that measured doping behaviors and/or doping intentions, and at least one other demographic (e.g. age, sex), psychological (e.g. attitudes, morality), or social-contextual (e.g. participation in team sport, motivational climate) variable were included. Using the above criteria, 104 independent studies were identified. When statistical information required for the meta-analysis was not available in the articles retrieved, we contacted the authors in order to request it. Forty-one studies were excluded because the corresponding authors either did not reply or were unable to provide the required information. A flow diagram showing the flow of dataset selection is shown in Fig. 1. Consequently, the final pool included 63 independent studies [4, 8, 1719, 2123, 2781]. The majority of included studies were journal articles (number of studies k = 46). Other sources included online datasets (k = 11), theses/dissertations (k = 3), unpublished manuscripts/datasets (k = 2), and a report retrieved from the WADA website (k = 1). Most of the included studies were cross-sectional (k = 55). Far fewer studies used longitudinal/prospective (k = 4) or experimental designs (k = 4).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram. WADA World Anti-Doping Agency

2.3 Data Collection Process/Summary Measures

Coding of study information was conducted by one author, with a sample of studies coded independently and cross-checked by another author. We coded for the type of publication (e.g. published journal article, student thesis, manuscript under review), study design (e.g. cross-sectional, longitudinal/prospective, experimental), background of participants (e.g. competitive athletes, gym users, students), type of sport participants engaged in (e.g. team, individual), and age group of participants (e.g. adolescent, adults). These variables were subsequently tested as potential moderators of effect sizes. The summary measures/effect sizes recorded were odds ratios [ORs] (for dichotomous variables predicting doping behaviors), zero-order Pearson’s correlations (when one or both variables were interval or continuous), and Cohen’s d (for intervention vs. control group comparisons). Cronbach’s alphas were also recorded to correct for measurement error in the meta-analyzed effect sizes [82]. A summary of the information coded in each included study is presented in ESM Appendix S2.

2.4 Assessment of Quality/Risk of Bias

To ensure study quality would not lead to biases in results, the quality of included studies was assessed [83]. The assessment tool developed by Higgins et al. [83] was adopted in this study. However, as this tool was designed for studies using an experimental design only, we created other criteria (see ESM Appendix S3) for cross-sectional (e.g. whether valid measures were used) and longitudinal/prospective studies (e.g. whether dropout from the study was random). For each criterion, included studies were rated as having ‘low risk’ or ‘high risk’ of bias. A study was deemed to have low risk of bias if it was rated as having ‘low risk’ for all assessed criteria. The remaining studies (i.e. those with one or more criteria being considered as indicating ‘high risk’) were deemed as having potential risk of biases. We tried to eliminate risk of bias across studies by actively seeking for unpublished datasets and by calculating the fail-safe numbers (FSNs) N statistic (see Sect. 2.6).

To determine whether the assessment criteria were appropriate, two of the authors independently rated the study quality of three studies (one cross-sectional, one longitudinal/prospective, and one experimental). The assessment ratings were compared; of 29 ratings given for the three studies, only one was different, and this disagreement was resolved after a discussion. The rationales for giving the ratings for each subcategory were also discussed between the two raters to ensure that ratings were given based on similar evidence or reasons. The assessments of all other studies included in the meta-analyses were then conducted by one of the two authors.

2.5 Synthesis of Results

The random effect methods proposed by Shadish and Haddock [84] were used to meta-analyze ORs. The procedures suggested by Hunter and Schmidt [82], which also adopt a random effect model, were used for Pearson’s correlations and Cohen’s d. The population effect size [OR, correlation (ρ), or standardized difference (δ), respectively] was calculated for each meta-analyzed relation, adjusting for sampling error (by taking into account the sample size). When computing effect sizes, attenuation due to measurement error was corrected using Cronbach’s alphas. As Cronbach’s alphas were not available for all recorded effect sizes, the artifact distribution meta-analytical procedures [82] were employed. Furthermore, 95 % confidence intervals (CIs) were generated for all meta-analyzed effect sizes. If the 95 % CI of an effect size did not encompass the point estimate representing a null effect (i.e. 1 for OR, 0 for ρ and δ), the effect was considered to be real. When a real effect was found, the magnitude of the meta-analyzed effect sizes was labeled using the criteria suggested by previous researchers [85, 86]. Essentially, an OR with a value between 1.68 and 3.47 was considered small, 3.47 and 6.71 medium, and above 6.71 large (the reciprocals of these cut-off values were used when OR <1). The cut-off values used to label small, medium, and large correlations were 0.1, 0.3, and, 0.5, respectively; for standardized mean differences, we used values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8, respectively.

2.6 Additional Analyses

To address the issue of possible publication bias in the literature [87], we calculated the FSNs when small to medium, medium, or large effects were found. An FSN represents the number of studies with null findings which, if included in the meta-analysis, would reduce the effect to a small size (i.e. 1.68 for OR, 0.1 for correlation, and 0.2 for standardized differences). The formula provided by Hunter and Schmidt [82] was used to derive the FSNs for the meta-analyzed effect sizes. If the FSN corresponding to an effect size is large, it is considered unlikely that the effect found is due to publication bias.

To test for heterogeneity in the obtained effect sizes, the I 2 statistic [88] was used. Specifically, the I 2 is an estimate of the percentage of differences in combined effects sizes which is due to true heterogeneity (i.e. not due to chance). A smaller I 2 suggests that effect sizes are more consistent across studies. Effect sizes with I 2 values smaller than 25 % were considered to be homogeneous [88]. When I 2 values exceeded 25 %, heterogeneity was suggested and moderator analyses were conducted. In essence, separate subgroup effect sizes were calculated for each level of the potential moderator (e.g. separate effect sizes for the moral disengagement-doping intention relationship were calculated for different types of study design). A variable was considered to be a moderator if the 95 % CIs of any two subgroup effect sizes did not overlap [89]. The potential moderators we tested in this study were sex of participants (males vs. females), publication status [published (k = 46) vs. unpublished (k = 17)], study design [cross-sectional (k = 55) vs. longitudinal/prospective (k = 4) vs. experimental (k = 4)], background of participants [competitive athletes (k = 16) vs. gym users (k = 12) vs. students (k = 35)], type of sport [team sports (k = 4) vs. individual sports (k = 11) vs. a mixture of both (k = 21)], age group of participants [adults (k = 22) vs. adolescents (k = 29) vs. a mixture of both (k = 10)], and study quality [studies with low risk of bias (k = 55) vs. studies with potential risk of bias (k = 8)].

Using the meta-analyzed effect sizes, we conducted a path analysis of a model based on the TPB [13]. Path analysis and meta-analysis can complement each other [90, 91]. Path analysis can capture interdependencies between variables that meta-analysis cannot capture because the latter can look at the relation of only two variables at a time. On the other hand, meta-analysis removes the effects of artifacts (i.e. sampling error) from data before path analysis. Hence, our path analysis takes the two-variable associations of the TPB variables established by the meta-analysis and tests them in a multiple variable context.

3 Results

The results of all meta-analyzed effect sizes are presented in Table 1. A glossary of the specialized terms in this table is provided in ESM Appendix S4. Effect sizes from a single study are shown in the tables but will not be interpreted further. Where moderator effects were found, the subgroup effect sizes are presented in Table 2.

Table 1 Results of meta-analyses and homogeneity tests predicting doping behavior and doping intentions
Table 2 Differences in effect size across levels of moderators

3.1 Effect Sizes of Experimental Studies Comparing Intervention and Control Groups

With respect to doping behaviors, we compared the differences in numbers of new reported cases of doping between the intervention and control groups over a sport season/school year. We found that the interventions did not show a real effect in terms of reducing doping behaviors (OR 0.76, 95 % CI 0.27–2.17; OR <1 indicates there were fewer reports of new doping behaviors in intervention groups, but the CI included 1). In terms of doping intentions, standardized difference scores between the intervention and control groups were meta-analyzed. Over the same period of time, the interventions showed a very small reduction in doping intentions (δ = −0.12, 95 % CI −0.13 to −0.11). Nonetheless, few studies (k = 3 and k = 2 for behaviors and intention, respectively) were included in these analyses (hence the reason why forest plots for these studies are not presented), and therefore the results should be interpreted with caution.

3.2 Effect Sizes of Demographic and Social-Contextual Variables Predicting Doping Behaviors/Doping Intentions

Demographic and social-contextual variables were examined in conjunction with doping behaviors and/or intentions in the included pool of non-experimental studies. In terms of demographic variables, we found that males reported more doping behaviors than females (OR 2.72, 95 % CI 2.16–3.42). We also found that doping behaviors were more prevalent in people who had friends that doped compared with those who did not (OR 6.40, 95 % CI 3.46–11.84). The use of legal supplements was related to more doping behaviors (OR 8.24, 95 % CI 5.07–13.39) and higher levels of doping intentions (ρ = 0.36, 95 % CI 0.20–0.52). Age was also found to be related to doping intentions (ρ = 0.05, 95 % CI 0.02–0.09); however, the size of this effect is very small.

In terms of moderator analyses, we found that the background of participants moderated the effect between sex and doping behaviors. Specifically, we found that this effect was stronger for gym users (OR 7.77, 95 % CI 5.31–11.37) than competitive athletes (OR 2.17, 95 % CI 1.16–4.08) and students (OR 2.48, 95 % CI 2.04–3.00). For the relation between age and doping behaviors, a very small negative effect was found in female (ρ = −0.03, 95 % CI −0.05 to −0.02) but not male (ρ = 0.005, 95 % CI −0.01 to 0.02) participants. Another moderator of the association between age and doping behaviors was the background of participants. Specifically, we found a very small effect between these variables in gym users (ρ = 0.09, 95 % CI 0.01–0.17) but not in students (ρ = −0.01, 95 % CI −0.02 to 0.003) as the 95 % CIs did not overlap. Furthermore, studies with low risks of bias showed a very small negative effect of age on doping behaviors (ρ = −0.01, 95 % CI −0.02 to −0.001), but those with potential risk of bias reported a small positive effect (ρ = 0.14, 95 % CI 0.07–0.21). We also found that study design was a moderator of the age–doping intentions relation. Although a very small effect was found in both cross-sectional studies (ρ = 0.07, 95 % CI 0.04–0.11) and longitudinal/prospective studies (ρ = 0.003, 95 % CI 0.002–0.004), their corresponding 95 % CIs did not overlap, indicating that these effect sizes were not equivalent.

3.3 Effect Sizes of Psychological Variables Predicting Doping Behaviors/Doping Intentions

Some of the effect sizes of psychological variables predicting doping behaviors and intentions were based on a very small number of studies. Consequently, moderator analyses could not be conducted because when these effect sizes were broken down to moderator subgroups, there was an insufficient number of studies for all subgroups to allow meaningful comparisons. To allow more meaningful comparisons across different levels of moderators, two more stages of analyses were conducted, after the initial stage that calculated an effect size for each psychological variable. In stage 2, we collapsed the effect sizes of variables that are conceptually similar (e.g. self-efficacy to refrain from doping and perceived behavioral control; see Table 1 for details regarding how variables were combined) with appropriate reversing (e.g. moral disengagement was reversed when combining effect sizes of morality variables). If conceptually similar constructs were measured in the same study, the weighted averages of the coefficients of interest were used to form a single effect size from this study for the meta-analysis. For brevity reasons, we report below the findings from stages 2 and 3 only, but the results from all stages are shown in Table 1.

With respect to doping behaviors, we found positive effects from intentions (ρ = 0.38, 95 % CI 0.21–0.55), attitudes (ρ = 0.17, 95 % CI 0.04–0.29), norms (ρ = 0.36, 95 % CI 0.27–0.45), and amotivation (ρ = 0.17, 95 % CI 0.07–0.26). The sizes of these effects were small to medium. Negative effects on doping behaviors were also found from self-efficacy to refrain from doping (ρ = −0.12, 95 % CI −0.21 to −0.02), morality (ρ = −0.21, 95 % CI −0.32 to −0.10), autonomous motivation (ρ = −0.06, 95 % CI −0.09 to −0.03), and task achievement goal orientation (ρ = −0.09, 95 % CI −0.17 to −0.01). The effects of the latter two variables though were small. The FSN for the effect between self-efficacy to refrain from doping and doping behaviors was 1 (k = 5), suggesting that only one study with null findings was needed to reduce this effect to what is considered a small effect (i.e. reduce it from ρ = −0.12 to ρ = −0.10).

In terms of predicting doping intentions, large positive effects were found for attitudes (ρ = 0.52, 95 % CI 0.44–0.60) and norms (ρ = 0.53, 95 % CI 0.43–0.63). Small effects were found for dissatisfaction with appearance/body image (ρ = 0.20, 95 % CI 0.12–0.29), amotivation (ρ = 0.24, 95 % CI 0.20–0.27), and ego achievement goal orientation (ρ = 0.14, 95 % CI 0.09–0.20). However, the effect size between doping intentions and ego achievement goal orientation has an FSN of 2 (k = 4). Small to moderate negative effects were also found from self-efficacy to refrain from doping (ρ = −0.27, 95 % CI −0.41 to −0.14) and morality (ρ = −0.31, 95 % CI −0.47 to −0.16). An effect was also found from task achievement goal orientation (ρ = −0.08, 95 % CI −0.14 to −0.02). Nonetheless, the size of this latter effect is small.

For the third stage of the analyses, variables were categorized as either a positive or negative correlate of doping intention or behaviors. A variable was categorized as a positive correlate if a real positive effect with doping intention/behaviors was found in the previous stages. In contrast, variables with real negative effects were considered as negative correlates. Based on this categorization, we collapsed the effect sizes associated with all positive and negative correlates and conducted meta-analyses and moderator analyses using the combined effect sizes. The variables included in these analyses are shown in Table 1. Forest plots representing the third stage of meta-analyses are presented in ESM Appendix S5.

We found that the effect sizes associated with the positive (ρ = 0.20, 95 % CI 0.08–0.32) and negative (ρ = −0.13, 95 % CI −0.19 to −0.07) correlates were small to medium. The FSN for the effect between doping behaviors and negative correlates was also small (FSN = 4, k = 11), suggesting that a relatively small number of studies (i.e. four) with null findings would bring the meta-analyzed effect to a small value. With respect to doping intentions, we found a medium effect from positive (ρ = 0.44, 95 % CI 0.36–0.51) and negative (ρ = −0.28, 95 % CI −0.39 to −0.17) correlates.

In this third stage of the analysis we also found some moderation effects. For the positive correlates–doping behaviors relation, a medium effect size was found when only studies with low risk of bias were included (ρ = 0.31, 95 % CI 0.21–0.41). However, a small negative effect was found in studies that were rated to have potential risks of bias (ρ = −0.10, 95 % CI −0.13 to −0.06). We also found that the relation between positive correlates and doping intentions was moderated by publication status and study design. Specifically, we found that the effects in published datasets (ρ = 0.49, 95 % CI 0.40–0.59) were stronger than those found in unpublished datasets (ρ = 0.31, 95 % CI 0.24–0.39). Nonetheless, the size of both effects was medium. In terms of study design, we found a medium effect for cross-sectional studies (ρ = 0.40, 95 % CI 0.31–0.48) but a large effect for longitudinal/perspective studies (ρ = 0.56, 95 % CI 0.52–0.61). Study design also moderated the negative correlates–doping intentions relation. A small effect was found in cross-sectional studies (ρ = −0.21, 95 % CI −0.33 to −0.10) but the magnitude of the effect was stronger in longitudinal/prospective studies (ρ = −0.44, 95 % CI −0.45 to −0.43). Finally, we found that the effect size between negative correlates and doping intentions was small in competitive athletes (ρ = −0.16, 95 % CI −0.29 to −0.02) but large in gym users (ρ = −0.62, 95 % CI −0.85 to −0.40).

3.4 Path Analyses of a Theory of Planned Behavior Model

The TPB [13, 20] was the most frequently utilized theoretical framework within the pool of included studies. Thus, in order to examine the inter-relation between all TPB constructs and doping behaviors and intentions, we conducted path analyses using meta-analyzed effect sizes from the second stage of our analyses. Path analyses were conducted using the Mplus 6.1 software [92]. Following recommendations by Viswesvaran and Ones [91], the harmonic mean of the sample sizes underpinning the corresponding effect sizes of the correlation matrix was used as the total sample size (n = 5,046) for the tested models. Based on the model proposed by Ajzen [13], we tested an initial model with attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy to refrain from doping predicting intentions to dope; the latter, in turn, predicted doping behaviors. We used self-efficacy to refrain from doping instead of perceived behavioral control as they both reflect efficacy beliefs. Furthermore, past evidence has shown that self-efficacy to refrain from doping is a stronger predictor of doping intentions [16] and, in contrast to perceived behavioral control, a significant predictor of doping intentions in both users and non-users [93]. This model did not fit well [94]: χ 2 (3) = 288.98, p < 0.001, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.92, root mean square error of estimation (RMSEA) = 0.14, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = 0.04.

We tested three other plausible alternative models by freeing direct paths from attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy to refrain from doping (only one of these paths was freed in each alternative model) to doping behaviors. A plausible model was accepted if the improvement in CFI was larger than 0.01 compared with the initial model [95]. The alternative models with direct paths from attitudes or self-efficacy to refrain from doping to behaviors did not meet this criterion and were rejected. However, the model with a direct path from norms to doping behaviors (see Fig. 2) was accepted (change in CFI = 0.060). This final model had a significant χ 2, but other fit indices suggested an acceptable model fit: χ 2 (2) = 78.35, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.09, SRMR = 0.02. In this model, the direction of paths from attitudes (β = 0.31), norms (β = 0.34), and self-efficacy to refrain from doping (β = −0.14) to doping intentions were in the expected direction (all paths p < 0.001). Paths from norms (β = 0.22, p < 0.001) and intention (β = 0.26, p < 0.001) to doping behaviors were also positive. Indirect effects on doping behaviors from attitudes (β = 0.12) and self-efficacy to refrain from doping (β = −0.06) were also significant at p < 0.001.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Path diagram of a theory of planned behavior model using meta-analyzed correlations (k = 22, n = 5,046). All paths are significant at p < 0.001

4 Discussion

This article represents the first meta-analysis within the doping literature. Its primary aim was to synthesize data from independent samples in an effort to identify key demographic, psychological, and socio-contextual factors that are related to doping intentions and doping behaviors. In sum, our findings corroborate Petróczi and Aidman’s [96] argument that decisions to dope reflect a complex interplay of multiple factors and may not be accurately explained by a few variables alone. This is a notable conclusion when one considers that although a variety of social cognitive models purporting to delineate the key determinants of behavior exist, the TPB has been the most widely tested theory in the context of doping in sport. Moreover, for some of the identified predictor variables there was only a limited pool of studies available. Consequently, the findings with regard to these variables must be interpreted with caution, including findings from experimental studies. Nevertheless, collapsing the variables in the final stage analysis of positive and negative correlates of doping intention and behaviors gives some indication of the magnitude of the statistical effects, which were small to medium. This meta-analysis reconfirms the conclusion drawn by Backhouse and colleagues [25] that the evidence base on the psychosocial predictors of doping intentions and behaviors is limited by span and methodology. This absence of evidence continues to be an important consideration for policy makers and developers of anti-doping prevention programs.

4.1 Effect of Interventions on Doping Intentions and Behavior

We firstly evaluated the effectiveness of existing randomized controlled trials (RCTs), and only four experimental studies met the eligibility criteria for inclusion in our meta-analysis. The four studies presented data from the evaluations of two long-standing North American prevention programs, namely the ATLAS [42, 97, 98] and ATHENA [26, 98] programs. The ATLAS/ATHENA-based RCTs included in our analyses showed a very small but significant reduction in doping intentions but no changes in doping behavior. This might be attributed to the content of the ATLAS and ATHENA interventions. Both tackle doping alongside other behaviors, such as healthy eating and training regimes. Perhaps the lack of a clear focus on doping explains the small effect on intentions and the non-significant effect on actual behavior. In particular, ATHENA is largely focused on information and activities about healthy and disordered eating, drug use (i.e. alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, and anabolic steroids), monitoring nutritional intake, and improving psychological factors, such as depression and mood [26, 67, 99]. Hence, both interventions aim to influence adolescent athletes’/students’ overall health-related behaviors, and this may happen at the expense of a greater focus on doping use. Furthermore, it is possible that the small effects found in the ATLAS/ATHENA interventions reflect floor effects; participants’ doping intentions at baseline (pre-intervention) were low. Therefore, there was not much room for a further reduction in intentions and behavior. For example, in the study by Goldberg et al. [97], pre-intervention intentions to use steroids were 5.7 and 6.3 in the experimental and control groups, respectively (1–7 scale, with 7 being no intent to use).

4.2 Effect Sizes of Demographic and Social-Contextual Variables

Effect sizes from non-experimental studies were also included in the present meta-analysis. We looked at the effect sizes associated with demographic and socio-contextual variables. Males and gym users were more likely to report doping use compared with females and competitive athletes or students. Furthermore, the use of legal nutritional supplements and being affiliated with peers who use doping substances, predicted greater self-reported doping use. Use of legal supplements had a large positive effect on doping behavior. This suggests that users of legal supplements are at a much higher risk of using illegal drugs. This finding is notable when one considers the widespread use of legal supplements across all levels of sport [100103]. In a recent study, Backhouse et al. [23] suggested that a gradual increase in the consumption of nutritional supplements served as a gateway to doping use. Specifically, in a sample of competitive athletes, Backhouse and colleagues found that doping use was 3.5-fold more prevalent in nutritional supplement users compared with non-users. This finding was accompanied by significant differences in doping attitudes and beliefs between nutritional supplement users and non-users. A potential explanation is that the habitual use of otherwise legal nutritional supplements familiarizes athletes with the notion of chemically-assisted performance enhancement [96] and this can further facilitate transitions to doping use. In a similar vein, Tsorbatzoudis et al. [104] indicated that nutritional supplement users are two to three times more likely to report doping use and they also displayed biased normative beliefs related to doping use (i.e. they perceived doping as more prevalent in fellow athletes and as being socially approved).

4.3 Effects of Psychological Variables

The present meta-analysis also investigated the effects of psychological variables on doping intentions and behavior. Specifically, task goal orientation was inversely related to doping use, whereas ego goal orientation and controlled motivation were positively related. These findings suggest that ego-oriented and externally regulated athletes display a more maladaptive set of behavioral responses. These athletes are more inclined to dope and this should be taken into account when developing prevention education interventions.

Many of the included studies utilized TPB but expanded on this framework by utilizing their own measures and incorporating additional distal predictors of doping behavior. The meta-analysis showed that pro-doping attitudes and biased normative beliefs (i.e. overestimating the actual prevalence and social acceptability of doping use) directly and positively predicted doping intentions. Accordingly, higher scores in self-efficacy to resist temptations and anticipated regret inversely predicted doping intentions and use. These findings imply that the TPB—and its extensions—offer a useful framework for understanding the decision-making process towards doping use.

Having said this, it is important to acknowledge that the TPB is a highly rational model, and Ajzen [105] argues that it does not adequately account for the cognitive and affective processes that bias judgments and behavior. Stewart and Smith [106] have argued that athletes’ decisions may not always rest on rational thinking. This may also apply to intentions to dope which might be based on unconscious factors that bias judgment. Furthermore, doping use might be unintentional via the consumption of contaminated nutritional supplements [107]. Therefore, the application of alternative models and methods is encouraged in this field in order to further our understanding of the complex doping phenomenon. Moreover, new approaches have been proposed suggesting that doping use is influenced by the interplay of sociocultural, socioeconomic, and personality factors. For instance, Donovan et al. [108], Petróczi and Aidman [96], and Strelan and Boeckmann [109] discussed the role of personality factors, decisions regarding the pros and cons of doping use, the legislative system, and the affordability and availability of prohibited substances. Stewart and Smith [106] provided a macro-analysis of doping use in sport, suggesting that, alongside personality factors, globalization and commercialization of sport, and sport cultures are key factors in explaining doping use. However, research testing these ideas is rather scarce and future studies that take into account these variables are encouraged [106].

In the current study, higher levels of sportspersonship and moral disengagement negatively predicted doping intentions and doping use. These findings highlight the important role of morality in preventing doping use. Finally, dimensions of body image, more specifically concerns about muscularity and thinness, and overall dissatisfaction with body appearance were found to significantly relate to doping intentions and doping use. These findings suggest that maladaptive perceptions of body image may facilitate doping use. This could be through the use of illegal substances as these can be used as a strategy to augment or reduce an individual’s physique. Therefore, body image should be targeted in prevention programs, particularly those that reach adolescent athletes.

In this meta-analysis we also examined whether several factors moderated the reported effect sizes. Although some of the effects varied in size across different levels of moderators, in general, the direction of the effects remained unchanged. However, we found that the effect sizes from some studies deemed as having potential biases may alter the directions of the effect in an unexpected way (e.g. a negative effect between positive correlates of doping and doping behaviors). Furthermore, published studies reported bigger effect sizes than unpublished studies. Therefore, these findings emphasize the importance of eliminating possible sources of biases when conducting empirical research in this field to avoid dubious results.

4.4 Path Analysis

Using the meta-analyzed effect sizes, we also tested a path model based on the TPB. We found that the paths from attitudes, perceived norms, and self-efficacy to refrain from doping were significant predictors of doping intentions, and in the expected direction. Furthermore, a positive path from doping intentions to doping behaviors was found. By comparing alternative nested models, we also found that perceived norms had a direct effect on doping behaviors. These results are slightly unexpected because in the TPB literature perceived norms are often found to be the weakest, or even non-significant, predictor of intentions [110, 111]. However, unlike many non-doping-related TPB studies that assessed social norms through subjective norms (i.e. perceived social approval of doping), many of the studies we analyzed assessed other aspects of social norms, such as descriptive norms (i.e. what significant others themselves do). This encompassing assessment appears important when one considers the assertion that the role of norms varies dependent on the type of behavior [112].

Overall, the findings of this meta-analysis underscore the importance of normative influences on doping use. Normative influences to engage in doping can range from mere peer pressure and perceived social acceptability of doping by teammates, to internal and external pressures associated with results and enhanced performance. Hence, the athlete’s entourage (e.g. coach and teammates) and perceptions about the prevalence of doping use among other athletes seem to be important determinants of the decision to engage in doping. It is noteworthy that perceptions of social norms do not have to be accurate to be influential [113] and future research should address the cognitive processes (e.g. heuristics) that lead to biased normative beliefs. In order to increase the predictive power of the normative component of the TPB, future research could pay closer attention to other normative variables (e.g. personal, injunctive and moral norms) [111]. For example, it is unclear whether observing the doping behavior of others is of greater motivational importance compared with perceived social pressure from others. Finally, the observation of Rivis and Sheeran [110] that young people are particularly susceptible to descriptive norms suggests that interventions with adolescent groups should be bespoke to the needs of that group in order to foster a strong sense of individuality and uniqueness.

4.5 Limitations

The most important limitation of this work is that many meta-analyzed effect sizes were based on a small number of studies. This is because the studies conducted in this area have been very broad, perhaps too broad, in terms of identifying demographic, personal, and situational predictors of doping intentions and behavior. Nevertheless, this review represents an initial effort to quantify the psychosocial variables that are most strongly related to doping behaviors or intentions to engage in such behaviors. Also, the current conclusions assume that self-reported behaviors are accurate reflections of people’s actions (all included studies used self-reports to measure doping behavior/use). Moreover, most of the meta-analyzed effect sizes between predictors of doping with doping intentions and behaviors were derived from correlational studies; more prospective and intervention studies are needed in this area.

As a consequence, the identified effect sizes show statistical and not causal effects. For example, a large effect size between perceived norms and intentions may also suggest people having stronger intentions to dope would perceive more people to be doping [63]. Similarly, people may report having more positive attitudes towards doping because they intend to use illegal substances. Another limitation was that we were unable to test some potentially important moderator effects. For example, in studies that reported the numbers or percentages of participants using legal substances, these were reported for the total sample and not by subgroups. Therefore, comparing effect sizes for different levels of moderators was not possible, unless authors were willing to provide us with access to their raw data.

4.6 Practical Implications

Anti-doping education should target normative influences and attempt to de-normalize doping use in sports and exercise settings. Prevention programs are particularly encouraged in the early adolescent years when attitudes are forming [114] and when adolescents tend to be more vulnerable to normative influences. Thus, related interventions should be more intensive for this age group and greater attention should be afforded to peer influence and related socio-emotional processes that facilitate risky behavior under social pressure [115, 116]. For example, forging a unique and individual identity could be important in this context as adolescents appear particularly susceptible to descriptive norms [110]. Finally, there are experimental studies that have demonstrated powerful effects of norms-based interventions in changing adults’ behavior [117, 118]. Doping researchers could utilize these findings and develop norms-based interventions to reduce the risk for doping use.

Future research and preventive guidelines regarding the social environment should also focus on the role of coaches and athlete support personnel in promoting adaptive types of motivation (autonomous motivation, task achievement goals). Importantly, future prevention interventions should improve athletes’ self-efficacy in resisting temptations (i.e. outcomes of success such as money, fame etc.) that might lead to doping use. In addition, aspects of ethical decision making should be incorporated in interventions designed to tackle doping use. Finally, doping-related interventions should inform athletes on the risks and appropriate use of nutritional supplements. In recent years, contaminated nutritional supplements have become a real cause for concern [107], and at a global level there has been a growing number of claims of inadvertent doping through the use of nutritional supplements. So that athletes can make an informed choice, there is an urgent need to regulate the industry and undertake efficacy trials to assess the performance benefits and health risks associated with the use of these supplements. Furthermore, the use of nutritional supplements may act as a gateway to illegal drug use [23] and this should be acknowledged through increased educational efforts in this area.

5 Conclusions

In sum, we found that doping behaviors may be explained by a combination of demographic, social-contextual, and psychological variables. However, the available evidence is still not sufficient in volume and rigorous enough in terms of methodology or diverse enough in terms of its conceptual underpinnings, and this limits our understanding of doping intentions and doping use in sport. Nevertheless, our results support a curriculum shift by placing greater emphasis on modifying norm-based doping perceptions and creating coach and peer task-involving motivational climates. Effective prevention interventions to tackle doping use should target athletes’ motivation, cognitions and moral stance, empowering athletes and their entourage to resist unethical temptations and pursue performance improvements through the use of legal substances and methods.