Over the past two decades, compassion has emerged as a focus of scientific inquiry (Kirby et al. 2017; Oman 2011). While most compassion research is situated in medicine (e.g., Duarte et al. 2016; Sinclair et al. 2017), social work (e.g., Brill and Nahmani 2017; Collins and Garlington 2017), and clinical psychology (e.g., Steindl et al. 2018) domains, what is known so far about compassion in daily life is also promising. Dispositional compassion increases resilience to trauma (Fredrickson et al. 2003) and stress (Pace et al. 2008), and encourages prosocial behaviour (Leiberg et al. 2011). Furthermore, compassion ‘interventions’ can increase positive emotions (Fredrickson et al. 2008; Klimecki et al. 2012; Mongrain et al. 2011), enhance self-esteem (Mongrain et al. 2011), and promote social connectedness (Hutcherson et al. 2008).

In philosophical and spiritual scholarship, there has been interest in compassion for thousands of years. Compassion features prominently in Buddhism, where it is considered the highest virtue (Dalai Lama 1995; Kirby et al. 2017). The lack of compassion research in psychology has resulted partly from definitional difficulties, as it is often used interchangeably with strengths like kindness, empathy, sympathy, and altruism by both laypeople and academics. However, researchers are beginning to converge on a multidimensional definition that is characterized by the ability to recognize emotional distress coupled with a motivation to help (Neff 2003; Pommier 2011). Different models have been suggested, but almost all include the capability to recognize emotions, possessing concern for others who are suffering, and having a desire to alleviate suffering (Dalai Lama 1995; Gilbert 2014; Gu et al. 2017; Kirby et al. 2017; Neff 2003; Pommier 2011; Strauss et al. 2016).

Empathy is defined as the ability to understand the emotions of others and experience a congruent affect, and it is the construct most frequently conflated with compassion. What distinguishes compassion from empathy (and from sympathy and pity) is its motivational caring component (Goetz et al. 2010). Previous research has found that compassion not only correlates with empathy (Klimecki et al. 2012; Sprecher and Fehr 2005) but also provides explanatory power beyond it (e.g. it is compassion that promotes prosocial behaviour, owing to its motivational quality; Lim et al. 2015; Lim and DeSteno 2016). A grounded theory study of palliative care patients suggested that most people can distinguish compassion from empathy, perceiving compassion as more altruistic and loving (Sinclair et al. 2017).

An unresolved question is what predisposes someone to experiencing compassion, or in other words, what comprises the profile of a compassionate person. A second question is how compassion might differ across cultures. Steindl et al. (2019) observed that social roles can influence the expression of compassion, finding differences between Australians and Singaporeans on both compassionate actions and the fear of receiving compassion. However, to our knowledge, little other research has been conducted on the topic.

There are several personality traits that should, theoretically, predict compassion. Here, we opted to employ the HEXACO model of personality rather than the widely used five-factor model (FFM; McCrae and Costa 1987). While several HEXACO domains resemble the FFM, HEXACO has numerous advantages (for a detailed review, see Ashton and Lee 2007). Most pertinent is that its six factors—honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness—have emerged organically in lexical studies across at least 12 diverse cultures (Ashton and Lee 2001; Ashton et al. 2004; Ashton and Lee 2007). This is an important benefit, as some psychologists have questioned whether personality inventories are applicable across different cultural contexts (e.g. Poortinga and Van Hemert 2001). Additionally, HEXACO is demonstrably superior in explaining prosocial characteristics like altruism (Ashton and Lee 2007), making it more relevant to compassion research. Much of the literature on human strengths in social and personality psychology has used the FFM; fortunately, however, HEXACO and the FFM have demonstrated strong convergence between analogous domains (Ashton et al. 2008; Gaughan et al. 2012) suggesting that the FFM can inform predictions about HEXACO.

First, honesty-humility is characterized by sincerity, lack of manipulative tendencies, and modesty, versus egocentrism and selfishness (Ashton et al. 2004; Lee and Ashton 2004). Honesty-humility represents a good-natured, considerate outlook that should be a prerequisite for compassion. Common humanity—a non-judgmental recognition of the human condition—is a component of Pommier’s (2011) and Neff’s (2003) compassion construct, and this interconnectedness seems unlikely to manifest with low honesty-humility.

Second, emotionality is characterized by sensitivity, perceived interconnectedness, and sentimentality (Lee and Ashton 2004). It is one of the domains that, across cultures, best captures altruism (along with honesty-humility and agreeableness; Ashton and Lee 2007). However, it should be noted that mindfulness (the tendency to engage with others without becoming overwhelmed) is an aspect of Pommier’s (2011) Compassion Scale, and that emotionality contains some content (specifically fearfulness and anxiety) that could relate negatively with mindfulness.

Third, like emotionality, extraversion has an affective component and relates to prosocial variables. The literature on empathy can partly inform predictions about compassion, and extraversion has demonstrated positive relationships with scores on Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright’s (2004) Empathy Quotient (EQ) in Japan (Wakabayashi and Kawashima 2015) and the USA (Nettle 2007).

Fourth, agreeableness has shown to relate to empathy in American, Spanish, Chinese, German, and Japanese samples (Del Barrio et al. 2004; Melchers et al. 2016; Nettle 2007; Wakabayashi and Kawashima 2015). Agreeableness also predicts prosocial behaviour (Graziano and Eisenberg 1997) and forgivingness (Shepherd and Belicki 2008). Additionally, because many extraversion and agreeableness HEXACO items refer to adaptive interpersonal functioning (Lee and Ashton 2004), it is likely that compassion would correlate with both. In short, there are possible links between honesty-humility, emotionality, agreeableness, and extraversion with compassion, owing both to the theoretical content of these domains and to previous research involving other prosocial variables.

There are individual differences that cannot be subsumed under HEXACO, an important one being trait emotional intelligence (EI; Petrides and Furnham 2001). Trait EI is a collection of perceptions about one’s abilities to manage emotions, socialize, and demonstrate self-control (Petrides et al. 2007). Unlike ability EI, which is assessed with performance measures, trait EI is assessed with self-report measures (Petrides et al. 2016; Siegling et al. 2015). Because it is distinct from HEXACO, it could contribute uniquely to compassion. Generally, those with high trait EI see themselves as flexible, optimistic, and empathetic (Petrides and Furnham 2001). In addition, trait EI has been linked to accurate emotion perception and other-reported prosocial behaviour (Mavroveli et al. 2009), suggesting a link with compassion.

The purpose of this study was to investigate the relations between compassion for others with personality traits and how these might vary in different cultural contexts—specifically, Canada and Spain. These contexts are distinct in key aspects; for example, Canada is more individualistic (versus collectivistic) than Spain, indicating that relationships are less tight-knit, and individuals are expected to take care of themselves (Hofstede et al. 2010). Spanish culture has a higher power distance (the importance of social hierarchies) and much higher uncertainty avoidance (how well a culture tolerates unknowns; Hofstede et al. 2010). However, we opted not to make specific hypotheses about compassion cross-culturally, as broad cultural values are not necessarily deterministic of individual traits. Importantly, Canada and Spain also possess similarities that make them ideal for cross-cultural comparison. Most notably, they are similarly heterogeneous, with about 21% and 13% of Canada and Spain’s populations, respectively, being foreign-born nationals (Statistics Canada 2017; United Nations 2017).

Method

Participants

We obtained data from Canadian and Spanish adults. In Canada, 590 undergraduate students at a large Ontario university were recruited. These participants were compensated with academic course credit. Of these students, 139 (25.0%) were male, 415 (74.8%) were female, and one participant identified as transgender. Participants ranged in age from 17 to 37 years, with a mean age of 18.34 (SD = 1.48). Thirty-five participants were excluded due to missing data. Spanish participants were 372 adults recruited from the personal networks of students attending the Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (National Distance Education University; UNED). Students were compensated with course credits for recruiting these participants. A total of 226 (60.8%) participants were women, and 144 (38.7%) were men. The age range was 17 to 68 years, with a mean age of 38.92 (SD = 11.97). Two participants were removed for missing data. Both studies received ethical approval from the respective universities. Participation was fully voluntary, and participants could withdraw from the study at any time.

Procedure

All materials were administered online. Participants read the letter of information and provided their consent to participate. Canadian participants accessed the study via a university recruitment database and completed all questionnaires on the Qualtrics survey platform, while Spanish participants were emailed a link to the survey and completed it on the Google Questionnaire platform. Measures were presented in a randomized series to control for order effects. Upon completion, participants read a debriefing letter.

Measures

The Compassion Scale (Pommier 2011)

We employed a relatively new measure of compassion, Pommier’s (2011) Compassion Scale, which is based on the six-factor structure of Neff’s (2003) Self-Compassion measure. Pommier found evidence for this structure in a sample of American students, which has more recently been replicated in a community sample of adults (Sousa et al. 2017). It has been suggested that a two-general factor solution (compassion and disconnectedness, or pro- and con-trait compassion) is ideal for the Self-Compassion and Compassion scales (Brenner et al. 2017; Coroiu et al. 2018; Sousa et al. 2017), rather than a unidimensional score.

Pommier’s (2011) Compassion Scale is a 24-item measure composed of kindness, mindfulness, and the recognition of common humanity factors in addition to their opposites indifference, disengagement, and separation, which are represented by two higher-order factors of compassion and disconnectedness (Sousa et al. 2017). The scale has demonstrated good psychometric validity as well as test-retest and internal consistency reliabilities (Pommier 2011; Sousa et al. 2017). Items (e.g. “I tend to listen patiently when people tell me their problems”) are responded to on a 5-point Likert scale. For this study, excellent internal consistency was found for two factors of compassion, α = 0.84, and disconnectedness, α = 0.89.

Because the Compassion Scale is relatively new, we did not locate an adapted or translated version in the published literature, which is required to expand research in a wider context. Following the guidelines proposed by the International Test Commission (2017) and extensively described elsewhere (e.g. Hambleton and Lee 2013), the scale was translated into Spanish by a fully bilingual collaborator. To ensure construct equivalence with the English version, it was then back-translated, reviewed, and items were adjusted for meaning where necessary. Compassion and disconnectedness demonstrated internal reliabilities of α = 0.81 and α = 0.88, respectively.

HEXACO-60 (Lee and Ashton 2004)

The HEXACO-60 is a 60-item short version of the HEXACO inventory. Each domain (honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience) contains ten items and can be divided into four facets. Items are assessed with a five-point Likert scale. In the Canadian sample, the subscales of the HEXACO revealed good internal consistency: honesty-humility (α = 0.77), emotionality (α = 0.78), extraversion (α = 0.81), agreeableness (α = 0.78), conscientiousness (α = 0.79), and openness (α = 0.75). For the Spanish sample, the Spanish version of the HEXACO-60 was administered. In the Spanish sample, the subscales of the HEXACO revealed good internal consistency: honesty-humility (α = 0.79), emotionality (α = 0.75), extraversion (α = 0.77), agreeableness (α = 0.69), conscientiousness (α = 0.74), and openness (α = 0.74). All versions and translations of the HEXACO can be found at http://hexaco.org/hexaco-inventory.

Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire, Short Form (TEIQue-SF; Petrides 2009a)

The TEIQue-SF is a 30-item short-form version of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue-SF; Petrides 2009a). The TEIQue has demonstrated good reliability (Petrides 2009b), factor structure (Perera 2015), construct validity (Laborde et al. 2016), and predictive validity for adaptive outcomes (Mikolajczak et al. 2009; Petrides et al. 2007; Saklofske et al. 2007), and it is effective at capturing the distinct nature of trait (versus ability) EI (Petrides and Furnham 2001).

The TEIQue-SF assesses self-perceptions of emotional competencies and adaptive dispositions. Example items include “I’m usually able to influence the way other people feel” and “I often pause and think about my feelings”. Responses are assessed on a seven-point Likert scale and items are summed to create a total trait EI score. In the Canadian sample, the TEIQue-SF demonstrated good internal reliability, α = 0.88. The Spanish version of the TEIQue-sf (Petrides et al. 2017) also demonstrated good internal consistency (α = 0.88).

Data Analyses

All variables were screened for skewness and kurtosis and no problematic deviations were observed. We then conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for the Compassion Scale on the Canadian and Spanish data separately to establish support for its factor structure in both samples. Recent evidence has suggested the Compassion Scale should be divided into two factors—compassion and disconnectedness—with its subscales representing six sub-factors (Brenner et al. 2017; Sousa et al. 2017). The CFA suggested that in the Canadian sample, a two-factor model (χ2 (245) = 687.826, p < .001; RMSEA = .057 [.052, .062]; CFI = .908; SRMR = .054) demonstrated better fit than a one-factor model (χ2 (246) = 929.429, p < .001; RMSEA = .071 [.066, .076]; CFI = .858; SRMR = .072), χ2diff (1) = 241.603. This was also found for the Spanish sample, χ2diff (1) = 75.613, such that a two-factor solution (χ2 (245) = 616.717, p < .001; RMSEA = .064 [.058, .070]; CFI = .874; SRMR = .057) demonstrated better fit than one factor (χ2 (246) = 692.330, p < .001; RMSEA = .070 [.064, .076]; CFI = .849; SRMR = .066).

To investigate the cross-cultural invariance of the Compassion Scale, we also conducted multigroup CFAs for configural, metric, and scalar invariance with the two-factor model of the Compassion Scale. Three multigroup CFAs were conducted with different parameter restrictions using the lavaan package for R (see Table 1 for fit indices). A maximum likelihood estimation was used for all CFAs. Results suggested that participants’ patterns of responding to the Compassion Scale differ between Canada and Spain, thus limiting the ability to directly compare group means between Canadian and Spanish participants.

Table 1 Multi-group confirmatory factor analyses for the two-factor model of compassion: fit indices

We conducted a two-by-two ANOVA to investigate whether compassion and disconnectedness differed by gender and culture. The samples were then examined separately using bivariate correlation analyses to reveal the zero-order correlations between age and personality variables with compassion and disconnectedness. Stepwise multiple regressions were then conducted with both compassion and disconnectedness as outcomes in the Canadian and Spanish samples separately. Listwise deletion was used to account for missing data. In each case, gender was controlled by entering it at step 1, followed by the HEXACO factors and trait EI in step 2 in order to ascertain which personality variables were the most meaningful predictors of compassion.

Results

Demographic Differences

Table 2 presents mean scores and standard deviations for the study variables in the two samples. A 2 × 2 (culture by gender) ANOVA of compassion scores revealed significant main effects of gender, F (1, 920) = 51.34, p < .001, η2 = .05, and culture, F (1, 920) = 23.21, p < .001, η2 = .02, but no interaction effect, F (1, 920) = .04, p = .85, η2 = .00. Men reported lower compassion than women, and Canadian participants reported lower compassion than their Spanish counterparts. This was the case for disconnectedness as well, with significant main effects of gender, F (1, 920) = 47.70, p < .001, η2 = .05, and culture, F (1, 920) = 33.80, p < .001, η2 = .03, but no interaction effect, F (1, 920) = .20, p = .66, η2 = .00. Men reported higher disconnectedness than women, and Canadian participants reported higher disconnectedness than Spanish participants. However, the findings on cross-cultural differences in compassion scores must be interpreted cautiously due to the lack of measurement invariance found for the Compassion Scale. Age was unrelated to compassion and disconnectedness in the Canadian sample (r = .01, p = .885 and r = − .01, p = .891, respectively) as well as in the Spanish sample (r = − .02, p = .717 and r = .01, p = .877).

Table 2 Compassion, disconnectedness, trait EI, and HEXACO means and standard deviations in Canada and Spain

Bivariate Correlations

Correlational analyses (see Table 3) indicated that HEXACO factors and trait EI were related to compassion (positively) and disconnectedness (negatively). Most of these relations were medium in size (Cohen 1988). Agreeableness and openness had the weakest relations with compassion, while emotionality and trait EI had the strongest relations. Agreeableness and openness also had the weakest relations with disconnectedness, while honesty-humility, emotionality, and trait EI were most strongly related.

Table 3 Correlations between compassion, disconnectedness, and personality variables in the Spanish sample (above diagonal) and Canadian sample (below diagonal)

Canadian Sample

Honesty-humility, emotionality, agreeableness, and trait EI were significant, positive predictors of compassion. As well, conscientiousness and openness demonstrated an unanticipated relation with compassion. Extraversion had no relation. Emotionality was the strongest predictor of compassion among Canadians, accounting for 9% of variance. In the second model, honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, trait EI—and again, unexpectedly, openness—were significantly related to disconnectedness. However, extraversion and openness’s contributions were negligible, accounting for less than 1% of variance each. Emotionality was again the strongest predictor, contributing 15% of unique variance. Tables 4 and 5 depict the results of the regression analyses.

Table 4 Stepwise multiple regression for compassion in the Canadian sample
Table 5 Stepwise multiple regression for disconnectedness in the Canadian sample

Spanish Sample

The results of the analyses in the Spanish sample diverged somewhat from the Canadian sample. Emotionality and trait EI again predicted compassion, accounting for 10% and 6% of the variance in compassion scores respectively. Openness was also unexpectedly related to compassion. However, no relations were found with honesty-humility, extraversion, or agreeableness. Disconnectedness was significantly related to honesty-humility, emotionality, and trait EI, but not to agreeableness or extraversion. Its strongest relations were with emotionality and trait EI, with each explaining 7% of the variance. Tables 6 and 7 depict the results of the regression analyses.

Table 6 Stepwise multiple regression for compassion in the Spanish sample
Table 7 Stepwise multiple regression for disconnectedness in the Spanish sample

Discussion

The present research sought to identify the personality variables linked to compassion and explore how they might differ between Canadian and Spanish cultures. Compassion is characterized by the ability to recognize, feel concern for, and be motivated to alleviate suffering in others, and it is a personal strength predicting a range of positive outcomes. Given the prosocial nature of compassion, we expected trait EI and the HEXACO domains of honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, and agreeableness would be positively correlated with compassion scores.

We found gender effects consistent with previous literature (Pommier 2011; Sinclair and Saklofske 2018; Sousa et al. 2017) such that Canadian and Spanish women reported higher compassion and lower disconnectedness than their male counterparts. In fact, the size of this difference was almost identical; Canadian and Spanish women, respectively, scored on average 0.25 and 0.26 points higher on compassion, and 0.31 and 0.34 points lower on disconnectedness.

To summarize the personality effects, we found evidence for the influence of emotionality, trait emotional intelligence, honesty-humility, and to a lesser extent, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness. Extraversion demonstrated no relation with compassion or disconnectedness in the Spanish sample and a negligible effect on compassion (but not disconnectedness) in the Canadian sample. Emotionality and trait EI were strong predictors of compassion (positively) and disconnectedness (negatively) in both cultures. However, in the Canadian sample, honesty-humility and agreeableness were significant contributors to both compassion and disconnectedness as well, whereas in the Spanish sample, honesty-humility correlated negatively only with disconnectedness, and agreeableness had no effect on either. The differential findings for compassion and disconnectedness underscore the need to measure these dimensions separately.

While there were some cultural differences, the most powerful predictors overwhelmingly were emotionality and trait EI; importantly, they accounted for a significant amount of variance in both the compassion and disconnectedness factors. Individuals high in emotionality can be described as vulnerable and sensitive (Ashton et al. 2014). As the Compassion Scale contains items like “My heart goes out to people who are unhappy” (Pommier 2011), sensitivity to others’ distress seems to produce higher compassion. Additionally, emotionality contains sentimentality, representing the tendency to feel emotionally attached to others, implying a capacity for other-directed concern. It does not appear that emotionality’s reactivity content—specifically the fearfulness and anxiety facets—precludes emotionality from facilitating compassion.

Trait EI was the second most powerful unique predictor, particularly in the Spanish sample, where it accounted for 6% of the unique variance in compassion and 7% in disconnectedness (compared with 2% of each among Canadians). Trait EI represents a collection of self-perceptions regarding intra- and interpersonal skills, and several aspects of trait EI likely contribute to compassion, including emotional perception and trait empathy (necessary for recognizing and understanding others’ distress), emotion regulation and stress management (likely precursors to mindfulness, described as the ability to feel concern without detachment or personal distress), and relationships (the ability to bond with others and have fulfilling connections; Petrides and Furnham 2001). Future research could explore which unique aspects of trait EI most strongly influence compassion by employing the long-form TEIQue or other EI measures.

Extraversion has been linked with empathy in past research, but this was not the case with compassion in the current study. One explanation for this might be discrepancies between HEXACO’s extraversion versus the five-factor NEO-PI-R (Costa and McCrae 1992). NEO-PI-R’s extraversion contains facets of interpersonal warmth, while HEXACO measures extraversion as a combination of boldness, sociability, liveliness, and self-esteem (Ashton et al. 2004; Ashton et al. 2014). Warmth could be the primary driver of the five-factor model extraversion’s relation with prosocial variables (Nettle 2007; Wakabayashi and Kawashima 2015). Additionally, it is unclear why the relations with agreeableness were relatively weak, given the variable’s prosocial connotations and its links with empathy. One possibility is that agreeableness is also characterized by conflict avoidance or even obedience (e.g. Bègue et al. 2015), traits which are theoretically unrelated to compassion.

The small relation with openness was unexpected and, interestingly, manifested only with compassion (not disconnectedness). Openness is characterized by aesthetic appreciation, inquisitiveness, creativity, and unconventionality (Lee and Ashton 2004). At a glance, these qualities do not seem relevant to compassion. However, given that artistic endeavours are often a process of emotional expression, having a degree of sensitivity might go together with high openness. It could also be argued that the unconventionality facet, which contains items such as “I like people who have unconventional views,” implies non-judgmental acceptance and interconnectedness.

Personality accounted for a slightly higher percentage of variance in the Canadian sample. The predictors explained 37% and 47% of variance in compassion and disconnectedness in Canada, but 35% and 40% of variance, respectively, in Spain. One explanation for this discrepancy might be found in cultural value dimensions. Geert Hofstede’s (2011) individualism-collectivism axis is likely most pertinent for compassion research; for example, Bengtsson et al. (2016) found that the relation between compassion for others and for the self is stronger in more collectivistic societies, suggesting that cultural collectivism plays a role in determining how compassion manifests. This link makes theoretical sense given the collectivistic emphasis on interpersonal harmony; notably, Canada is much less collectivistic than Spain (Hofstede 2001; Hofstede et al. 2010). In an individualistic culture, the development of compassion might rely more on individual differences, while in a collectivist culture, it might be driven by shared cultural values. In fact, some researchers have gone as far as arguing that personality itself is “less apparent in collectivistic cultures... because the situation is such a powerful determinant of social behaviour” (Triandis 1995, p.74). Future research should endeavour to expand on this possibility regarding compassion by recruiting samples from highly collectivistic cultures (e.g. South Korea, China, Mexico, or Chile; Hofstede et al. 2010).

Limitations and Future Directions

While women reported higher compassion scores in both cultures, it should be noted that with personality accounted for, gender’s influence on compassion was small. Additionally, while we found that Spanish participants reported higher compassion than Canadians, the lack of measurement invariance between groups makes this difficult to interpret. Spanish and Canadian adults may respond to the Compassion Scale differently as a result of unique cultural norms, or simply differences in interpretation resulting from the translation process. This research reveals a need to modify or develop a novel scale for compassion that is comparable across distinct cultures. Alternatively, compassion as a construct could simply mean different things in different cultures—perhaps indicating a need for culture-specific measures of compassion.

The Spanish sample contained a much wider age range than our Canadian sample, due to differences in recruitment strategy. While it is conceivable that compassion increases with age, given that some evidence has suggested empathy and prosocial tendencies are higher in older populations (Beadle et al. 2015; O’Brien et al. 2013; Richter and Kunzmann 2011), others have found that older adults have a diminished capacity for empathy (Bailey et al. 2008; Chen et al. 2014). Here, a correlational analysis found that age had no relation with compassion. We did not collect data on participants’ occupation or socioeconomic status, but these could be other characteristics that influence the presentation of compassion. Possible research directions are thus how compassion develops across the lifespan, how it might manifest in different professions, or how it varies across socioeconomic groups.

The study’s use of self-report measures means that the study suffers from common method bias—that is the presence of variance that is due to measurement error in the instrument, rather than actual variations among the participants (Podsakoff et al. 2003). Additionally, some researchers have discussed how successfully questionnaires can be compared across cultures. For example, Melchers et al. (2016) questioned whether there are real cross-cultural differences in personality, or whether these are driven mostly by response biases; furthermore, it is unclear whether differences are products of cultural influence (as some traits are valued differently across cultures; Melchers et al. 2016) or have a genetic component. Another limitation is this study’s focus on compassion for others. Compassion can also be directed at the self and received from others, and these motivations may have distinct origins as well as cross-cultural manifestations. Future studies could investigate these motivations and their associations with personality.