Introduction

Mark Smith was an indispensable member of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) research community. A renegade to some and inspiration to others, Mark was not afraid to take an unpopular position if his data suggested that was the right position to take (Perry et al. 2000; Castellani et al. 2009). Data generated by Mark and his colleagues questioned the AD field’s prevailing dogma, and Mark attacked that dogma with both gusto and, somehow, charm. Mark certainly had a remarkable ability to disagree without being disagreeable. Even during heated arguments he seemed to be enjoying himself, which made him a joy to be around. These qualities endeared him to both allies and antagonists alike.

One of Mark’s insights was that in a disease such as AD in which neurons are dying, many phenomena would manifest but only one could constitute (to borrow a quote from Mark) “the head of the snake”. He further assumed downstream phenomena would, more often than not, represent adaptive responses (Lee et al. 2006). Preventing those adaptive downstream responses without fixing the upstream pathology that drives them would probably not help, and could conceivably hurt, AD patients. Those working to understand why AD subjects treated with semagacestat, a gamma secretase inhibitor, showed accelerated cognitive decline will certainly need to consider this possibility.

This review will discuss several different AD brain phenomena and relationships that are known to exist between these phenomena. In doing so it will discuss pro and con arguments over which particular pathologic phenomenon constitutes the “head of the snake”.

Does Beta Amyloid Cause AD?

This is certainly the most popular current view. The idea that beta amyloid (Aβ) protein causes AD forms the basis of the “amyloid cascade hypothesis” (Hardy and Allsop 1991; Hardy and Higgins 1992; Hardy and Selkoe 2002). The amyloid cascade hypothesis proposes accumulation of Aβ, as either a consequence of increased production or decreased removal, incites all other downstream AD-associated phenomena and ultimately the disease itself (Fig. 1). Mark was a fierce opponent of this hypothesis (Perry et al. 2000; Lee et al. 2006).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The amyloid cascade hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, an unprovoked imbalance in either Aβ production or removal rates causes Aβ accumulation within particular cortical regions. Mutations in the APP gene or in presenilin genes presumably cause this imbalance in autosomal dominant, familial AD. Either polymorphisms or mutations in genes whose products directly influence Aβ production, removal, or handling cause the imbalance in non-autosomal dominant, late-onset AD. This crucial Aβ imbalance may ultimately reflect higher total Aβ or else an increased Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio. In any case, the Aβ42 monomer excess results in Aβ42 oligomer production. Aβ oligomers eventually form extracellular fibrils, the fibrils collect to form plaques, and the plaques themselves may be toxic. Extracellular oligomers also attack synapses and destroy neurons. Exactly how Aβ oligomers cause most of their other downstream consequences is not settled. Downstream phenomena, such as oxidative stress, tangle formation, mitochondrial failure, and cell cycle re-entry may arise due to Aβ intracellular effects, Aβ extracellular effects, or as a general indirect effect of cell damage

The evolution of this hypothesis is discussed in other reviews but is worth briefly summarizing (Swerdlow 2007a). Alois Alzheimer’s initial presenile dementia patients, who were later said to have “Alzheimer’s disease” (Kraepelin 1910), were noted to have extracellular protein aggregations within their brains (Alzheimer 1907; Alzheimer et al. 1995). These extracellular protein aggregations, called plaques, were also known to collect within the brains of elderly humans as well as aging animals (Blocq and Marinesco 1892; Fischer 1907; Redlich 1898; Critchley 1929). These plaques were initially felt to represent a consequence, and not a cause, of either aging or (in the case of presenile dementia) whatever causes AD (Davis and Chisholm 1999). Over subsequent decades the main plaque constituent was shown to be an amyloid protein (Divry 1927). This means that following Congo red staining, exposure to polarized light generates a green birefringent fluorescence pattern.

Amyloid proteins generally contain β-sheet folded peptides (Sipe and Cohen 2000). Eventually the main β-sheet folded amyloid peptide within plaques was determined and this protein was named the beta amyloid, or Aβ, protein (Glenner and Wong 1984b). This Aβ protein was also shown to accumulate in the plaques of aging Down’s syndrome patients with trisomy 21 (Glenner and Wong 1984a; Masters et al. 1985). It turned out to be the case that Aβ derived from processing of a larger protein, the amyloid precursor protein (APP), which interestingly resided on chromosome 21 (Kang et al. 1987). In 1990, an APP gene mutation was found to cause the Dutch variant of a devastating autosomal dominant cerebral hemorrhage syndrome, hereditary cerebral hemorrhage with amyloidosis (HCHWA), in which carriers develop vascular wall amyloid aggregations that structurally resemble those which accumulate in AD brain cortex (Levy et al. 1990). The following year, an APP gene mutation was shown to segregate with disease in a family with autosomal dominant amyloid angiopathy, cortical plaques, and presenile dementia (Goate et al. 1991).

The amyloid cascade hypothesis soon followed (Hardy and Allsop 1991; Hardy and Higgins 1992). This hypothesis speculated the intact, mutated APP molecule itself did not directly initiate the essential AD cascade, but rather APP mutation increased Aβ to high levels and at high levels Aβ initiated the cascade. Studies performed over the next 20 years have concluded Aβ can be toxic when administered to cultured cells, that the 42 amino acid Aβ species (Aβ42) is more toxic than other physiologically relevant species, and oligomeric configurations of Aβ42 are more likely to initiate the cascade than fibrillar, protofibrillar, or monomeric forms (Yankner et al. 1989; Hardy and Selkoe 2002; Walsh and Selkoe 2007).

The amyloid cascade hypothesis was developed from the studies of rare, autosomal dominant AD kindreds with APP mutations. It was extrapolated to also apply to the non-autosomal dominant pre-senile and also the senile AD variants that comprise over 99% of AD cases (Swerdlow 2007a). Sporadic pre-senile AD and senile AD patients, however, do not have demonstrable APP mutations and APP polymorphisms appear to have no-to-little impact on the disease (Guyant-Marechal et al. 2007). It has therefore been proposed variants in other genes that directly influence either APP processing or Aβ disposal cause Aβ to accumulate. Several genes found to associate with AD risk, including some genes implicated through large scale genome-wide association studies (GWAS), may indeed ultimately impact amyloidosis but how directly or indirectly these variants do in fact influence amyloidosis is unclear (Rogaeva et al. 2007; Jun et al. 2010; Castellano et al. 2011; Harold et al. 2009). Further, unless specific variants in the apolipoprotein E protein turn out to directly affect Aβ homeostasis (Castellano et al. 2011), genetically determined variations in Aβ-handling appear to minimally affect AD risk. The obvious conceptual inference, which much of the AD research field has been reluctant to accept, is that although it is possible early onset, autosomal dominant and late-onset, non-autosomal dominant AD could ultimately share common final pathways they may not share a common etiology.

It is important to emphasize this conceptual approach does not claim amyloid protein cannot initiate and drive central neurodegeneration and cause dementia. There are few reasons to question whether amyloid protein accumulation initiates a toxic cascade in autosomal dominant AD arising from APP mutation. Amyloid accumulation also appears to initiate degeneration in other degenerative dementia syndromes, including familial British dementia (FBD) and familial Danish dementia (FDD) with amyloid angiopathy (Revesz et al. 2009; Mead et al. 2000; Holton et al. 2001, 2002). FBD and FDD arise from mutations in the BRI2 gene, which cause overproduction of a non-Aβ amyloid product. A key question that remains to be settled is whether it is more appropriate to group AD arising from APP mutation with other identified primary amyloidoses, such as FBD and FDD, than it is to group it with late-onset, sporadic AD, which could constitute a secondary amyloidosis.

Observational and experimental data have related and tied Aβ to other AD-associated phenomena. AD subject biomarker studies suggest Aβ changes temporally precede tau changes (Jack et al. 2010). In transgenic mice with concomitant production of Aβ and a mutant tau protein, Aβ perturbs tau homeostasis (Oddo et al. 2004). Transgenic mice that overproduce Aβ due to mutant APP overexpression develop synapse damage (Dong et al. 2007). Some of these mouse models also show an increase in the number of neurons that re-enter the cell cycle (Yang et al. 2006). Aβ can facilitate the production of reactive oxygen species (ROS) through reactions mediated by a particular methionine residue (Varadarajan et al. 1999). Inhibition of the electron transport chain (ETC) and enzyme cytochrome oxidase (COX) is observed in isolated mitochondria or whole cells exposed to Aβ protein, and APP-mutant transgenic mice have reduced COX activity and other mitochondrial perturbations (Canevari et al. 1999; Casley et al. 2002; Crouch et al. 2005; Dragicevic et al. 2010; Du et al. 2010; Hauptmann et al. 2009; Pereira et al. 1998).

The AD and physiologic relevance of these experimental data, though, is unclear. It is abundantly clear that amyloidosis can occur in the human brain for years, if not decades, without concomitant clinical symptoms or neurodegeneration (Sperling et al. 2011). Even if Aβ does constitute an upstream AD pathology, it is not sufficient by itself to cause full-blown AD. Supporters of the amyloid cascade hypothesis have responded to this by essentially changing the definition of AD so that changes in Aβ homeostasis are now the sole required diagnostic feature; the presence of a dementia or even a dementia prodrome syndrome is no longer required (Sperling et al. 2011).

Beyond this semantic nuance, though, the biggest problem with the amyloid cascade hypothesis is that for most AD patients why Aβ production or disposal changes in the first place is unknown. Because of this, it seems reasonable to consider that both APP processing and Aβ handling are highly regulated processes that are controlled by upstream physiologic events. If it turns out to be the case that a change in any of these upstream events alters Aβ accumulation in AD, then Aβ accumulation cannot constitute the primary cause.

Does Tau Cause AD?

Neurofibrillary tangles were the other hallmark feature described by Alois Alzheimer in his original presenile dementia clinical-pathological case reports (Alzheimer 1907, 1911). These intracellular formations were later shown to consist of phosphorylated tau protein (Grundke-Iqbal et al. 1986). In subsequent studies primarily consisting of late-onset AD subjects, tangle counts were shown to correlate better with cognitive status than plaque counts (Berg et al. 1993, 1998; Giannakopoulos et al. 2003; Nagy et al. 1996), and both hyperphosphorylated tau and tangles were found in the brains of middle-aged, non-demented individuals (Braak and Braak 1995; Arriagada et al. 1992; Hof et al. 1996). For reasons such as these, some investigators considered whether tangles, or at least tau protein, might initiate AD.

Tau protein aggregations are in fact observed in several neurodegenerative diseases. To recognize this fact the “tauopathy” family of diseases was defined (Lee et al. 2001). In the tauopathies, aberrant tau physiology is presumed to either cause disease or, by contributing to neurodegeneration, mediate disease symptoms. The strongest evidence that tau can play an etiologic role in neurodegenerative diseases comes from genetic studies of families with autosomal dominant frontotemporal dementia syndromes in which tau gene mutations segregate with the disease (Seelaar et al. 2011). Neurofibrillary tangles are typically seen in these families.

Tau also appears to be etiologically relevant, although not causal, in two other related neurodegenerative diseases, progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and corticobasal degeneration (CBD) (Pittman et al. 2006; Sergeant et al. 1999). This is because polymorphisms in the tau gene influence disease risk; most PSP and CBD patients are homozygous for the H1 tau gene haplotype (Baker et al. 1999). Tangles are seen in the brains of PSP and CBD patients. In addition, the H1 tau gene haplotype influences Parkinson’s disease (PD) risk (Elbaz et al. 2011). This is particularly interesting because PD brain histology studies do not typically emphasize tangles as being characteristic features.

Tau gene mutations have not been implicated in autosomal dominant AD families, and if tau gene polymorphic variation does contribute to AD that contribution is at most very small (Myers et al. 2005; Mukherjee et al. 2007). In AD biomarker studies, changes in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) tau levels occur after changes in CSF Aβ (Jack et al. 2010). For reasons such as these, while many still suspect aberrant tau protein plays an important role in AD neurodegeneration cascades few seem to believe tau protein initiates those cascades.

Compared to Aβ, tau’s relationship to other AD brain phenomena has not been as extensively studied. Despite this, tau’s normal physiologic role is perhaps better understood than that of Aβ (Goedert et al. 2006; Goedert 1998; Lovestone and Reynolds 1997). Tau interacts with microtubules, constitutes part of the cytoskeleton, and in this capacity tau likely influences axonal transport (Adalbert et al. 2007). Malfunctioning axonal transport systems would predictably impair the delivery of mitochondria to synapses, as well as the return of degraded mitochondria to the cell body. This could lead to synaptic degradation and oxidative stress.

As is the case with Aβ, tau homeostasis is an exquisitely regulated process. For example, during embryonic development, while cells are rapidly dividing and migrating tau is highly phosphorylated (Lovestone and Reynolds 1997; Goedert 1998). Later on, tau in differentiated cells is found in its relatively unphosphorylated form. In various situations, such as in AD neurons or in tumor cells, tau phosphorylation occurs again. As noted by Mark Smith, AD neurons actually appear to re-enter the cell cycle, which could relate to why AD neurons also have phosphorylated tau (McShea et al. 1997, 2007; Zhu et al. 2004a). In essence, phosphorylated tau may therefore simply be a characteristic of dividing cells (Swerdlow and Khan 2004).

In the mouse brain, fasting induces tau phosphorylation (Yanagisawa et al. 1999) and tau phosphorylates during hibernation (Arendt et al. 2003; Hartig et al. 2007). This phosphorylation is apparently mediated through kinase activation. It is interesting that just as bioenergetic stress alters APP processing and promotes Aβ production, bioenergetic stress also promotes tau phosphorylation (Blass et al. 1990; Szabados et al. 2004). It therefore seems to be the case that cell bioenergetics constitutes an important upstream regulator of both Aβ production and tau phosphorylation.

Does Synaptic Loss Cause AD?

In AD brains, synaptic loss correlates better with cognitive decline than either Aβ plaques or tau tangles (Coleman and Yao 2003; DeKosky et al. 1996; Terry et al. 1991). Clinical-pathological surveys of subjects with AD therefore suggest synapse degradation could constitute a defining event. Despite this, few would place synaptic degradation at the apex of any potential AD pathophysiologic cascade. Synapse formation and maintenance are highly regulated processes, so synapse degradation would more likely represent a consequence of failed synaptic maintenance or induced synaptic damage than it would represent a primary event. In other words, failing neuron health in AD is probably not a function of synaptic loss; synaptic loss in AD is probably a function of failing neuron health.

If synapses are being externally attacked in the AD brain, then Aβ could represent a potential culprit (Walsh et al. 2002). If synapses are being destroyed from within, oxidative stress or perturbed mitochondria could be responsible. Failed axonal transport due to tau alterations, or bioenergetic stress due to mitochondrial failure, could also interfere with synapse maintenance.

Does Oxidative Stress Cause AD?

Mark Smith and colleagues demonstrated how pervasive oxidative stress is within the AD brain (Smith et al. 1994, 1995; Perry et al. 2002; Nunomura et al. 1999). This line of influential work represents one of Mark’s major contributions to the AD research field.

In this respect, several seminal observations made by Mark Smith and colleagues are particularly relevant to the AD etiology debate. One observation is that oxidative stress in the AD brain is seen independent of plaques. In fact, oxidative stress levels are actually lower in plaque-containing areas than they are in plaque-free areas (Nunomura et al. 2001). One potential explanation for this is that Aβ production could actually represent a compensatory response to increased oxidative stress (Smith et al. 2002; Lee et al. 2006). This is one of the reasons why Mark predicted removing Aβ from the AD brain or preventing its formation might prove detrimental to AD patients. This further suggests oxidative stress temporally precedes Aβ production (Nunomura et al. 2001). Consistent with this view is the observation that oxidative stress activates the beta secretase (BACE) enzyme that helps process APP to Aβ (Tamagno et al. 2002).

The presence of oxidative stress in the AD brain, though, does not address why it developed or where it was produced. Oxidative stress is classically considered to represent a consequence of either ROS overproduction or underscavenging. Free radicals are eliminated by families of antioxidant enzymes that include the superoxide dismutases, glutathione peroxidase and reductase, catalase, and the thioredoxins. Free radical scavenging molecules, which are commonly referred to as “antioxidants”, also counter oxidative stress because they can accept and stabilize unstable electrons. Scant evidence, though, suggests critical free radical scavenging enzyme defects or antioxidant deficiencies occur in AD. For this reason, oxidative stress in AD is mostly suspected to result from radical overproduction.

ROS are produced through a variety of enzyme-mediated oxidase reactions; xanthine oxidase, monoamine oxidase, and NADPH oxidase catalyzed reactions are each associated with oxygen radical production (Bruce-Keller et al. 2010). Because nitric oxide is a radical molecule, nitric oxide synthase (NOS) produces reactive nitrogen species (RNS). Some ROS are also produced or modified through non-enzymatic reactions. In the Fenton reaction, hydrogen peroxide oxidizes ferrous iron (Fe2+) to produce ferric iron (Fe3+) and hydroxyl radicals. In the Haber–Weiss reaction, hydrogen peroxide reacts with superoxide to produce hydroxyl radical. Hydroxyl radical also results from the peroxynitrite reaction in which nitric oxide and superoxide react with each other. Arachidonic acid produced through phospholipase C or phospholipase A2-mediated phospholipid hydrolysis further promote hydroxyl radical production (Singh et al. 1981). Isoprostanes, which are used as a clinical biomarker of oxidative stress, are themselves a byproduct of arachidonic acid auto-oxidation.

The mitochondrial ETC is commonly cited as the main producer of intracellular oxidative stress (Shigenaga et al. 1994), and evidence suggests mitochondria also constitute the source of increased oxidative stress in AD brain (Nunomura et al. 1999). Mitochondrial ROS production increases when the mitochondrial membrane potential increases (Brand et al. 2004). In this case, mitochondrial ROS likely performs an important signaling function that informs the nucleus of the cell’s bioenergetic status (Finkel 2001, 2003; Petersen et al. 2007). ETC enzyme defects also cause mitochondrial ROS production, and in the setting of ETC enzyme defects mitochondrial ROS overproduction occurs in the presence of depolarized mitochondria (Barrett et al. 2004; Swerdlow 2011).

Mitochondrial dysfunction-oxidative stress relationships, though, can be reciprocal. Free radicals can inhibit ETC enzymes (Brown 1999; Riobo et al. 2001). Free radicals also oxidize mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA). During mtDNA replication, oxidized bases are misread and somatic mutations result (Minnick et al. 1994; Pavlov et al. 1994; Ozawa 1997).

If free radicals are a cause of AD mitochondrial dysfunction, and not a consequence of this, then where might these radicals arise from? One hypothesis is that Aβ itself acts as a pro-oxidant (Varadarajan et al. 1999). Data from Mark Smith and colleagues, though, is not consistent with this view because in the AD brain Aβ deposition anatomically correlates with reduced oxidative stress (Nunomura et al. 2001).

Does Cell Cycle Re-entry Cause AD?

Mark Smith was one of the first to report aberrant cell cycle activity in the AD brain (McShea et al. 1997, 2007; Zhu et al. 2004a; Nagy et al. 1998). Specifically, in the AD brain neurons appear to re-enter the cell cycle (Vincent et al. 1996, 1998; Arendt et al. 2000; Herrup and Arendt 2002; Mosch et al. 2007; Yang et al. 2001, 2003). These cycling neurons proceed through the G1, S, and even the G2 phases of replication but cannot successfully replicate. G2-M arrest and degeneration result in neurons that make it that far, although abnormal entry into any phase probably reflects compromised neuron health.

Work conducted by Mark and colleagues further suggests that from a temporal perspective, AD brain cell cycle abnormalities are a relatively early pathologic manifestation. Because data from Mark’s lab has also shown oxidative stress constitutes an early pathologic manifestation, a “two-hit hypothesis” was proposed in which mitotic perturbation was postulated to induce oxidative stress and oxidative stress was postulated to induce mitotic perturbation (Zhu et al. 2001, 2004b, 2007). According to this hypothesis, critical dysfunction and degeneration result when oxidative stress and cell cycle re-entry co-exist within a neuron. The two-hit hypothesis assumes amyloidosis and tau changes may or may not facilitate neuron loss, but are unlikely to initiate the critical pathologic cascades. Other investigators have also proposed aberrant cell cycle phenomena directly contributes to AD pathogenesis (Herrup 2010).

Do Mitochondria Cause AD?

Mitochondria are abnormal in AD patients and increasingly mitochondria are considered to play an important role in AD (Swerdlow 2009, 2011; Swerdlow and Kish 2002). In 2001, Mark Smith and colleagues published a highly influential immunochemical and electron microscopy study of AD brain mitochondria (Hirai et al. 2001). In this study, the number of normal-appearing mitochondria within AD brain hippocampal neurons was reduced relative to controls, but the number of degraded or degrading mitochondria was increased. The amount of PCR-amplifiable mtDNA was reduced but mtDNA deletion levels were increased. In control subject brains levels of a COX protein subunit, COX1, increased with advancing age and interestingly COX1 protein was even higher in AD brains. All these characteristics were most prominent in hippocampal neurons that showed the highest degree of concomitant oxidative stress.

With the exception of its rare autosomal dominant variants, AD and aging are closely associated (Swerdlow 2007a, c). In some elderly demographics, the percentage of those with AD probably exceeds the percentage of those that don’t have AD (Corrada et al. 2010). In addition to being abnormal in the AD brain, particular mitochondrial alterations are observed in the brains of aged humans and animals (Boveris and Navarro 2008; Navarro and Boveris 2007). In some instances, age and AD-associated mitochondrial defects overlap but differ in magnitude. For example, brain COX activity is probably lower in elderly individuals than it is in young individuals, and COX activity is even lower in AD subjects than it is in non-demented elderly subjects (Lin et al. 2002; Swerdlow 2011; Swerdlow and Kish 2002). Observational and experimental data suggest mitochondria may contribute to the aging process itself (Trifunovic et al. 2004; Kujoth et al. 2005). Mitochondria therefore constitute a nexus through which aging and AD may relate.

AD-associated mitochondrial defects were recently reviewed in detail elsewhere (Swerdlow 2011). Several of these details, though, seem particularly relevant to the current discussion of whether mitochondria initiate an AD-typical pathologic cascade. Of note, some AD mitochondrial defects are not brain-limited. In addition to its reduction in the AD brain, COX activity is lower in platelet and fibroblast mitochondria from AD subjects than it is in age-matched control subject platelet and fibroblast mitochondria (Parker et al. 1990; Curti et al. 1997). From a mitochondrial perspective, then, AD is not a brain-limited condition and therefore AD-associated mitochondrial characteristics are not simply a consequence of neurodegeneration or, it appears, any other brain limited pathology.

Data from cytoplasmic hybrid (cybrid) studies suggest AD-specific mitochondrial dysfunction can initiate key AD-associated pathologies (Swerdlow 2007b, d, 2011). Cybrid cell lines are created when mitochondria from a particular source are transferred to cultured cell lines (Fig. 2). In the case of AD cybrid studies, mitochondria from AD subject platelets were transferred to mtDNA-depleted human neuroblastoma or teratocarcinoma cell lines with neuronal characteristics. Because these cell lines contained no endogenous mtDNA (such cells are called ρ0 cells), all mtDNA-encoded proteins in the resulting cybrid cell lines derive from AD subject mtDNA.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Cybrid studies conclude mtDNA is at least partly responsible for low COX activity in AD subjects. COX has 13 subunits, ten of which are nuclear-gene encoded and three of which are encoded on mtDNA genes (a). If a cell’s mtDNA is removed, it cannot produce its three crucial mtDNA-encoded subunits; these “ρ0” cells have virtually no detectable COX activity (b). Transferring mtDNA-containing mitochondria from an exogenous source, such as platelets restores the ρ0 cell’s mtDNA, allows it to again produce mtDNA-encoded COX subunits, and creates a complete COX holoenzyme (c, d). Because the ten nuclear subunits are equivalent between cybrid lines containing mtDNA from different sources, differences in COX function between cybrid lines reflect differences in their mtDNA. Cybrid cell lines containing mtDNA from AD subjects (d) have a lower COX activity than cybrid cell lines containing mtDNA from control subjects (c)

Several respiratory chain holoenzymes contain a mix of both nuclear and mtDNA-encoded protein subunits. These include complex I, complex III, complex IV (COX), and complex V. In newly created cybrid cell lines, a point is eventually reached in which the nuclear-encoded subunits all derive from the cell line nucleus, and the mtDNA-encoded subunits all derive from the mtDNA that was transferred with the original platelet mitochondria (Fig. 2). All cybrid cell lines produced from a common ρ0 stock have the same nuclear genes and therefore their nuclear DNA-derived respiratory chain subunits should be identical. Differences in respiratory chain function between cybrid cell lines, therefore, most likely reflect differences in their mtDNA. These mtDNA differences in turn reflect the fact that each cybrid cell line’s mtDNA originated from a different platelet donor.

COX activity is lower in cybrid cell lines prepared through the transfer of AD subject platelet mitochondria than it is in cybrid cell lines prepared through the transfer of control subject platelet mitochondria (Swerdlow 2007b, d, 2011). The most straightforward interpretation for this is that mtDNA from AD subjects differs from that of control subjects, these differences result in lower AD cybrid COX activity, and mtDNA at least partly accounts for low COX activity in AD subjects.

ROS production is also higher in AD cybrid cell lines than it is in control cybrid cell lines (Swerdlow et al. 1997; Swerdlow 2007b, d, 2011). This occurs despite the fact that the mitochondrial membrane potential is reduced in AD cybrids. The most likely source of increased AD cybrid ROS production is a defective AD cybrid ETC.

Compared to control cybrid cell lines, AD cybrid cell lines also generate increased amounts of Aβ (Khan et al. 2000; Swerdlow 2007b, d, 2011). Administering antioxidants prevents this. Overall, cybrid data suggest AD subject mtDNA generates defective ETC subunits that alter respiratory chain function, altered respiratory chain function produces oxidative stress, and oxidative stress shifts APP processing towards its Aβ derivative.

AD cybrid data in general are consistent with other data that indicate respiratory chain inhibition or bioenergetic stress shift APP processing away from α secretase-mediated processing, which should enhance its BACE-mediated processing and produce Aβ (Gabuzda et al. 1994; Gasparini et al. 1997; Webster et al. 1998; Khan et al. 2000; Tamagno et al. 2002). Cybrid data are also consistent with other data that show ETC inhibition increases ROS production (Barrett et al. 2004).

ETC inhibition and bioenergetic stress have also been shown to increase tau phosphorylation and aggregation (Blass et al. 1990; Szabados et al. 2004). In general, the more aerobic a cell is the more differentiated it tends to be, and more anaerobic cells tend to be more mitotically active (Swerdlow and Khan 2004).

For all these reasons, an upstream role for mitochondria in AD pathology cascades seems feasible, at least for sporadic AD. The “mitochondrial cascade hypothesis” places mitochondrial dysfunction at the apex of the late-onset, non-autosomal dominant AD pathology cascade (Swerdlow 2007c, 2011; Swerdlow et al. 2010; Swerdlow and Khan 2004, 2009) (Fig. 3). The mitochondrial cascade hypothesis proposes an individual’s genetic composition determines their baseline mitochondrial function and durability. This in turn determines the rate at which age-dependent mitochondrial functional decline proceeds within that individual. When and if mitochondrial functional capacity falls below a critical threshold other AD-typical pathologies ensue. These events are initiated by mitochondrial dysfunction and include Aβ production, tau phosphorylation, synaptic degeneration, oxidative stress, and neuron cell cycle re-entry.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The mitochondrial cascade hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, an individual’s genetic make-up determines baseline mitochondrial function and durability. With advancing age mitochondrial function declines. The rate of mitochondrial decline is determined by a combination of the individual’s genetic make-up and environmental factors. When mitochondrial function surpasses a critical threshold beyond which compensation is not possible, AD-associated phenomena occur. Apoptosis pathways are activated and neurodegeneration results; faulty electron transport chains produce superoxide, which activates BACE and causes Aβ production; bioenergetic failure at the synapse causes synaptic degradation; shifting anaerobic/aerobic bioenergetic flux relationships activate cell cycle re-entry; and oxidative and bioenergetic stresses activate the kinases that phosphorylate tau

Conclusions

Mark Smith’s research into the causes of AD challenged the field’s prevailing wisdom. His exceptional courage, energy, and intellectual prowess made him one of the AD research community’s most important and highly regarded gadflies. In losing Mark the field lost an important voice, and some lost a valued and beloved colleague.

The amyloid cascade hypothesis continues to dominate AD research and how many investigators conceptually approach this disease. The amyloid cascade hypothesis presumes all AD variants are primary amyloidoses. According to this hypothesis Aβ in all cases initiates AD neuropathology, neurodysfunction, and neurodegeneration. Indeed, newly revised criteria define AD as the presence of altered brain Aβ (Sperling et al. 2011). Research conducted by Mark and others, though, raises serious doubts as to whether Aβ could possibly constitute the “head of the snake”. After all, APP processing and Aβ handling are exquisitely regulated processes, and some of these regulatory processes are perturbed in the AD brain. While much research is still required to settle this debate, as far as AD’s etiology goes the head of the snake is increasingly shaped like a mitochondrion. At the time of his death, Mark certainly seemed to be headed in this direction (Moreira et al. 2010a, b).