Introduction

The burden of informal caregiving, or providing daily help to close friends or family members managing medical or mental illness or disability, can have an impact on caregivers’ overall health and sleep. In fact, insomnia is common among informal caregivers, with rates between 40 and 76% [1,2,3,4,5,6], which exceeds prevalence rates in the general population [7]. Caregivers show several impaired sleep characteristics, including a suboptimal sleep duration, lengthened sleep latency, frequent awakenings, daytime sleepiness, and poor self-rated sleep quality [1, 3, 5, 8]. Importantly, the moderate to severe symptoms of insomnia experienced by a large proportion of caregivers do not remit naturally [6].

Lack of adequate sleep may result in negative physical and emotional outcomes, worsening the caregiving quality and feelings of fatigue [9,10,11]. Studies exploring the impact of sleep problems on physical and emotional outcomes in patients with chronic illness (e.g., dementia or cancer) are extensive. Yet few studies have attempted to describe sleep in young adult caregivers [12]. Moreover, young adults might be at a heightened risk for dysregulated sleep, including a short sleep duration, as sleep timing (both behaviorally and circadian phasing) shifts to later hours across young adulthood [13, 14]. Therefore, it is plausible that sleep quality in young adults may be disproportionately impacted by the burdens of informal caregiving.

Young adults (18 to 29 years old) comprise between 12 and 18% of the total number of adult caregivers and is a population that is grossly understudied [12]. They experience higher levels of caregiving stress and burden than older caregivers, which in turn have a negative impact on their pursuit of educational and career goals [15]. Caregiving in young adulthood likely exerts a negative physiological toll; however, which specific aspect of sleep quality is responsive to caregiving and the impact on dynamics in stress hormone activity are largely unknown.

When an individual perceives events, such as those related to caregiving, as stressful, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis works to coordinate a cascade of patterned autonomic and neuroendocrine responses in an effort to regain physiologic homeostasis [16]. Among these responses is the secretion of cortisol by the adrenal cortex, which aids in the up- and down-regulation of adaptive stress responses [17]. Under prolonged periods of stress, the HPA axis can reflect over- or under-activation [18,19,20]. Such dysregulation, often characterized by flattened diurnal slopes, blunted cortisol awakening response (CAR), or elevated daily output, can be detrimental to health over time [21].

The level of chronic stress experienced by long-term caregiving has been shown to impact cortisol patterns, suggesting physiological changes may also be a result of the stress of caregiving [22, 23]. Specific patterns of cortisol dysregulation have been noted among caregivers, including blunted CAR [24, 25] and flattened diurnal cortisol slope [22]. In addition, cortisol dysregulation may be proportional to the level of caregiving burden [26]. Thus, dysregulation in cortisol may be a particular physiological consequence of caregiver-specific stress. Additionally, HPA axis activity has well-established relationships with various dimensions of sleep quality including nighttime awakenings [27] and poor overall sleep quality [16, 28] and might underscore disruptions in sleep quality [29, 30].

Dimensions of sleep quality including sleep duration and fragmentation are associated with coping behaviors and better sleep quality might constitute a critical coping resource. For instance, in a racially diverse sample of adolescence, poor sleep quality was associated with patterns of disengagement coping [31], which might be a poor match to caregiving demands and general life stressors. Thus, the combination of caregiving demands and poor sleep quality might impair individual ability to effectively cope with caregiving demands and together constitute a compounded risk for HPA axis dysregulation.

The current study is designed to contribute to our understanding of the unique stress and coping experiences of this population from their non-caregiving peers. The purpose is to assess the overall sleep quality of young adult informal caregivers relative to non-caregivers. A focus is on examination of 1-month subjective sleep quality and objectively measured wrist actigraph sleep monitoring. This study was driven by two primary sets of hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that informal caregivers would have poorer objective and subjective sleep quality and greater dysregulation in diurnal cortisol indices, than demographically similar non-caregiving young adults. Second, we expected to observe a caregiver status by sleep quality interaction such that caregivers with poorer sleep will exhibit pronounced cortisol dysregulation.

Methods

Participants

Data were collected from young adults attending a large public university in the USA recruited from a student subject pool and via fliers posted throughout the university on physical and electronic bulletin boards. All participants were thoroughly screened to determine their current caregiver status. This involved an initial question to establish that informal caregivers “provide significant daily caregiving to a relative, close friend, or household member who needs help because of a physical or mental illness, disability, frailty associated with aging, substance misuse or other condition.” Those responding positively underwent a brief screening interview to discuss their specific caregiving role, level of caregiving effort, and specific caregiving tasks. Only those participants who reported being the primary caregiver (part-time or full-time) and provided daily or near-daily caregiving were included.

A total of 76 participants were enrolled and completed study procedures. However, three participants were excluded from data analysis because responses to questionnaire items suggested their caregiving was related to regular parenting or was better characterized as diffuse (inconsistent or in a secondary caregiver role) caregiving responsibilities. Therefore, the final sample included thirty-five caregivers and thirty-eight demographically similar non-caregivers.

Procedures

In a laboratory setting, participants completed questionnaire measures, and height and weight were measured. Participants were then fitted with a Motionlogger wrist actigraph (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardsley, NY) to wear on their non-dominant wrist outside the laboratory for 72-weekday hours [32] as a non-invasive and ecologically valid method for obtaining objectively assessed sleep quality parameters. Devices were programmed to record activity for three consecutive nights of sleep using 1-min epoch lengths. Concurrent with days of at-home sleep monitoring, participants collected saliva upon awakening (morning), 30 min after waking, 8 h post-awakening (afternoon), and at bedtime for 3 days using Salivette collection tubes (Sarstedt, Inc.) for assessment of diurnal cortisol. They were instructed not to eat, drink, or brush teeth for at least 20 min before sampling. Participants refrigerated samples until returning them to the laboratory with the actigraph device. Salivettes were stored in a − 20 °C freezer until analysis.

All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the primary author’s institution, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [33] was used as a measure of global subjective sleep quality over the past 30 days. The PSQI provides assessment of various sleep quality parameters. Using standard PSQI scoring procedure [33], we focus on four dimensions: sleep duration, sleep disturbance, sleep latency, and sleep-related daily dysfunction. For each parameter, scores ranged from 0 (better) to 3 (worse).

Caregiver Burden

Caregiver burden was measured with the Level of Care Index (LCI) [34]. The LCI assesses overall perceived caregiving burden experienced by the caregiver and distinguishes burden level as mild, moderate, or high. A burden index is computed for each caregiver based on hours of weekly caregiving and intensity of care (e.g., assistance with activities of daily living). Burden scores can range from 2 to 8. Cut scores are used to classify index scores as mild (total score of 2 to 4), moderate (total score of 5), or high (total score of 6 to 8) levels of caregiving burden and are used for descriptive characterization. However, scores were treated as continuous variable scores in statistical testing.

Demographics and Behavioral Factors

Participants self reported demographic variables. Relevant behavioral factors were also measured and included the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [35]. During the at-home monitoring period, participants completed the Consensus Sleep Diary [36], indicating sleep behaviors (e.g., time to bed).

Wrist Actigraphy

Sleep quality parameters were calculated using the Action-W program and included sleep duration (total nighttime sleep minutes), sleep efficiency (ratio of nighttime sleep duration to total sleep period), sleep latency (minutes to first sleep epoch), and sleep fragmentation (ratio of number of nighttime awakenings to nighttime sleep duration × 100). The index of sleep fragmentation considers movements of varying intensity.

Diurnal Cortisol

Concentrations of salivary free cortisol were measured in duplicate using a commercially available immunoassay (Salimetrics, Inc.). Assay sensitivity was measured to be < 0.007 μg/dL. The lower detection limit is 0.33 nmol/L, and inter-assay and intra-assay coefficients of variance are < 15%. Three indices were calculated to characterize the distinctive circadian pattern of cortisol secretion: cortisol awakening response (CAR), diurnal slope, and area under the curve (AUCg). Participants recorded adherence to instructions for saliva sample collection across the collection period.

Data Analysis

Actigraph data were downloaded and analyzed with the Action-W (version 2.0) [37] software program using zero crossing mode of activity, which utilizes peer-reviewed sleep cycle algorithms. Data were manually trimmed to the time period of interest by members of the study team in strict adherence to protocols developed in our laboratory and in consultation with participant sleep logs. Time in bed was determined by participant diary, light sensor data, and visual inspection of histogram data.

To account for skewness, raw cortisol values were log transformed prior to analyses. CAR was assessed by changes from awakening (averaged across days) to the second sample (30 min post-awakening; averaged across days). Diurnal slope was calculated as the decrease from the first morning sample to evening sample. To assess volume, AUCg was computed and averaged across days using the trapezoidal method based on hours after awakening [38]. The 30-min post-awakening measure was excluded from AUCg calculation [39].

Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations were computed for key study variables. Relevant biometric with known relevance to salivary cortisol (e.g., body mass index) and demographic variables were examined to identify possible covariates. Variables that differed significantly by caregiver status were included in subsequent statistical models. Covariate by predictor interactions were also examined.

Independent sample t tests were conducted to detect between group differences in sleep quality. Multiple linear regression analysis was used to determine the relationship of caregiver status and indices of objective and subjective sleep quality, when controlling for relevant covariates. Analyses were conducted using SPSS v26. Moderator analyses were conducted in accordance with procedures outlined by Aiken and West [40] to test the hypothesized caregiver by sleep quality interactions using the PROCESS macro [41] in SPSS.

Results

Descriptive and Preliminary Analyses

Participant characteristics are reported in Table 1. On average, participants were nearly 21 years of age and the majority were female. Black participants were more likely to be caregivers than were White participants, and although only approaching statistical significance, caregivers had a higher body mass index (BMI) than non-caregivers. Thus, ethnic minority status and BMI were included in model testing as statistical covariates.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

As reported in Table 1, caregivers had significantly more depressive symptoms than non-caregivers, although both caregivers and non-caregivers’ average scores were within what is typically considered as “normal range” on the HADS depressive symptom scale. On average, caregivers reported anxiety symptoms in the “borderline abnormal” range, and this was significantly higher than the average HADS anxiety symptom score for non-caregivers (which was in the “normal” range).

Aspects of caregiving activities are reported in Table 2. The majority of caregiving was for a parent, grandparent, or sibling, involved 10–29 h of care per week, and was to care for a physically ill or disabled loved one. Caregiver burden scores ranged from 2 to 8 on the LCI (M = 5.35, SD = 1.69). Notably, the majority of caregivers reported a “high” level of caregiver burden. The LCI reflects a combination of high numbers of weekly caregiving hours and high interference in daily living.

Table 2 Caregiving characteristics

Subjective Sleep Quality

Mean values of subjective sleep parameters were examined for caregivers and non-caregivers (see Table 3). Relative to non-caregivers, caregivers reported significantly more sleep disturbance and greater sleep latency in the prior 30-day period. There were no significant group differences on other subjective sleep parameters.

Table 3 Subjective and objective sleep quality

Results did not differ when controlling for ethnic minority status or BMI. However, a significant interaction of caregiver status and ethnic minority status was observed for sleep duration, such that caregiver status was associated with a longer sleep duration for White-identifying caregivers (β = − 0.81, p < 0.001), but not for ethnic minority caregivers (β = 0.04, p = 0.744).

Actigraphic Sleep Quality

Mean values of actigraphic sleep parameters for caregivers and non-caregivers are reported in Table 3. Relative to non-caregivers, caregivers demonstrated significantly more sleep fragmentation during the observation period. There were no significant group differences on other sleep indices.

Results did not differ when controlling for ethnic minority status or BMI. However, a significant interaction of caregiver and ethnic minority status was observed for sleep fragmentation, such that caregiver status was associated with less fragmented sleep for White-identifying caregivers (β = − 0.54, p < 0.00) but not for ethnic minorities (β = − 0.14, p = 0.332).

Salivary Diurnal Cortisol

Regression analyses examining caregiver status as a predictor of diurnal cortisol indices, controlling for BMI and ethnic minority status, did not yield any significant direct relationships. However, in hypothesis testing (F (3,59) = 3.06, p = 0.035, R2 = 0.14), caregiver status interacted with actigraph-measured sleep duration (β = − 0.01, SE = 0.001, p < 0.01) in the prediction of daily cortisol slope. Examination of simple slopes revealed that for caregivers (β = 0.13, SE = 0.001, p = 0.019), but not for non-caregivers (β = 0.01, SE = 0.001, p = 0.722), a shorter sleep duration was associated with flatter cortisol slope. This relationship is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Caregiver diurnal cortisol patterns by sleep duration. For illustrative purposes, a median split was used to define shorter and longer sleep duration

Post hoc Analysis of Caregiver Burden

In a set of post hoc analyses, caregiver burden was regressed on indices of salivary diurnal cortisol to inform that the possibility that the burden of caregiving is linearly related to stress processes. Multiple regression testing included BMI and ethnic minority status as potential covariates and tested for sleep by caregiver burden interaction effects.

Caregiver burden (β = − 0.69, p = 0.044) and objectively measured sleep duration (total sleep minutes) (β = 0.25, p = 0.030) were significantly associated with diurnal cortisol slope. Moreover, a significant caregiver burden by objectively measured sleep duration (total sleep minutes) interaction (p = 0.032) reveals that the relationship of caregiving burden to flatter cortisol slopes are most pronounced for caregivers experiencing relatively fewer overall minutes of sleep.

Caregiver burden was not associated with other cortisol indices and no other caregiver sleep parameter interactions were significant.

Discussion

The demands of caregiving can negatively impact caregivers’ functioning across many life domains, particularly among young adult caregivers who are simultaneously engaged in the developmental tasks associated with the transition from adolescence to early adulthood [42]. The current study provides preliminary evidence for the negative impact of informal caregiving on sleep quality in this group. Results suggest that caregiving is associated with several dimensions of diminished sleep quality including self-reported sleep disturbance and sleep latency, as well as objectively measured sleep fragmentation.

Consistent with a primary hypothesis, caregiver status interacted with sleep quality in the case of objective sleep duration to predict diurnal patterns of salivary cortisol. Typically, during sleep, blood levels of cortisol fall [43], but chronic disturbances in sleep have been associated with over-activation of the HPA pathway [43]. In this study, caregivers getting less overall sleep exhibited a flatter cortisol slope. That appears (see Fig. 1) to be marked by higher evening cortisol levels. This finding provides only preliminary insight into the impact of sleep problems on cortisol dysregulation in the context of caregiving stress; the nature of these relationships remains inconclusive. More careful examination of overall sleep duration in caregivers may be warranted. A study of dementia caregivers also pointed to sleep duration as being associated with dynamic cortisol patterns [44]. Future research should seek to identify modifiable psychological, behavioral, or biological factors that perpetuate or interrupt this pattern.

Our findings are, in part, consistent with literature suggesting decrements in sleep quality potentiate HPA axis reactivity to stressful demands [45]. Our pattern of results supports the notion that poor sleep quality might deplete coping resources rendering caregivers more vulnerable to poor stress regulation. Our finding that ethnic minority caregivers may be both more likely to be caregivers and more likely to experience poorer sleep when caregiving, compared with their White counterparts, might be pointing to an important health disparity. Buckhalt and colleagues [46] suggest that an accumulation of stressors is most impactful on sleep outcomes. Ethnic minority caregivers likely experience heightened daily stressors independent of caregiving, further depleting coping resources [47]. An important research direction will be to identify modifiable, explanatory factors underlying such disparities. It is important to note that caregivers were more likely to identify as ethnic minority than White in the current sample. Moreover, ethnic minority caregivers report an average of more than 4 hours of caregiving per week compared with White caregivers. It may be that familial or cultural expectations around informal caregiver vary by ethnic group as might reliance on the informal caregiving for higher level needs. Future studies should also seek to identify critical resilience factors that foster better sleep that might vary by ethnicity.

A strength of this study is the focus on dimensions and not simply overall sleep quality. This provides insight into more nuanced aspects of sleep. At the same time, it should be noted that although results provided some support for the hypotheses, results did not hold across sleep quality dimensions. Other study limitations should be noted. This study examined relationships at one moment in time. Future studies should include multiple assessments over time to identify dynamic patterns of relationships and more insight into temporal patterns. Also, although the current study carefully screened to identify caregivers, participants represented caregiving in different contexts. It might be that caregiving within certain contexts (e.g., for a parent with cancer) could vary greatly from others (e.g., caregiving for a depressed spouse). We also note that our recruitment method yielded a limited variation in education level; however, racial diversity of the sample was a strength. Finally, we note that actigraphy was measured over 3 days, which might not reflect fluctuations that would be detected if the monitoring period was of a longer duration. It is also possible that actigraph monitoring over a longer duration would better ensure stability in our estimations [48]. We do note that all monitoring occurred on weekdays and on self-reported “typical” days.

This study demonstrated the importance of attending to both subjective and objectively measured sleep. This may be particularly important in caregivers, as they have been known to poorly estimate their sleep quality. One study reported that caregivers of cancer patients tended to overestimate their total sleep time [12], while another study reported that caregivers underestimate their sleep quality [49]. Objective sleep measurement provides important information about sleep behaviors that may not be captured by self-reported measures administered during the laboratory visit. Additionally, the impact of chronic general stress in young adults should be considered in future work. Building from our finding on sleep fragmentation, future work should more carefully examined nighttime or “on-call” caregiving duties, as well as the possible impact of nighttime studying demands or nighttime use of mobile devices.

Taken together, the physical and emotional demands associated with caregiving contribute to and may exacerbate sleep problems and dysregulation in stress processes among young informal caregivers. Studies of caregivers should, however, not assume that caregiving is universally burdensome. The inclusion of a non-caregiving comparator group provides evidence of caregiving burden in this sample. Moreover, our post hoc analyses mirrored our findings that caregivers experiencing high levels of caregiver burden and a shortened sleep duration are at heightened risk for dysregulation in stress hormones.

The need to assist caregivers in improving sleep quality is paramount. Sleep impairment is associated with an elevated risk for a host of physical and psychiatric disorders [50,51,52], which are also associated with chronic stress. Despite the well-documented insomnia and related distress experienced by caregivers and caregiving-specific risk factors for insomnia, there are no empirically supported treatments to improve sleep among this vulnerable group. There is, however, emerging evidence for the benefits of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia (CBT-I) [53, 54] tailored to the unique setting of cancer caregiving. Future studies are therefore needed to evaluate the efficacy and effectiveness of CBT-I in addressing the unique experience of sleep disturbance in caregivers as well as to further identify dimensions of problem sleep affected by caregivers.