Introduction

Regular physical activity has been linked to numerous health benefits [1, 2], and increasing physical activity is an important goal in Healthy People 2010 [2]. The Surgeon General [1] recommends use of theoretical models, including social cognitive theory (SCT) and the transtheoretical model (TTM), to help design interventions for physical activity promotion. The TTM posits that ten processes of change, adapted from multiple behavior change theories, are critical in helping formulate intentions, adopt a behavior, and maintain the behavior over time [3]. Also critical to behavior change efforts are anticipated benefits (pros) and costs (cons) of physical activity (i.e., decisional balance). Specifically, the TTM posits that decisional balance and cognitive processes of change are more important during behavior adoption, whereas behavioral processes are more important to behavior maintenance [4]. TTM constructs have been shown to be predictive of physical activity behavior [e.g., 5], and numerous physical activity promotion interventions have been based on the TTM [e.g., 6, 7].

SCT emphasizes the importance of self-efficacy, defined as one’s confidence that he or she can regularly engage in physical activity in the face of salient impediments [8]. Other constructs, such as expected outcomes of physical activity, social support, and environmental access to physical activity facilities, also fit within the larger social–cognitive framework [8] and have been shown to predict physical activity behavior [e.g., 9, 10]. Although SCT has been used extensively in studies examining predictors of physical activity behavior (for reviews, see [11, 12]) and as the foundation for interventions promoting physical activity [e.g., 13], it has been criticized for not distinguishing between the determinants of behavior adoption versus behavior maintenance [14]. For example, SCT posits that personal (i.e., self-efficacy) and environmental (i.e., social support) factors are critical to behavior maintenance; however, these factors do not differ from those that are posited to influence behavior adoption [8, 15, 16]. Rothman and colleagues [14, 17] have posited a framework, grounded in SCT, which distinguishes between determinants of behavior adoption and behavior maintenance. In the context of this framework, they hypothesize that self-efficacy and outcome expectancies are most critical to behavior adoption, but social support and perceived satisfaction with the outcomes of initial attempts at behavior change are more important as determinants of behavior maintenance.

As indicated by Rothman et al. [17], “One way to discern whether a construct’s impact shifts as a function of phase is to separate individuals into subgroups according to their phase, then test its relative ability to predict behavior prospectively within each subgroup” (p. 140). Previous studies examining predictors of physical activity adoption and maintenance have, in predicting behavior maintenance, included all participants who reached a specific time point in the study (e.g., 6 or 12 months) regardless of whether they were active or inactive at that time point [1820]. This approach does not allow for a comparison of the predictors of adoption and maintenance. Other studies, consistent with the suggestion of Rothman et al. [17], have divided participants into active versus inactive subgroups and then examined predictors of physical activity adoption in one subgroup and physical activity maintenance in the other subgroup [2125]. However, these studies have not directly compared predictors of physical activity adoption and maintenance by examining initial physical activity status as a moderator of relationships between the predictors and subsequent physical activity levels. Moreover, some of these studies used retrospective data to divide participants into active versus inactive subgroups at baseline [2225]. The disadvantage of a retrospective approach is that it results in heterogeneity within the active subgroup, such that participants who were active at baseline may have been regular exercisers who had been exercising for a period of several years or more or novice exercisers who recently adopted exercise. Finally, no studies that we know of have compared the SCT constructs of perceived satisfaction, enjoyment, or environmental access as predictors of physical activity adoption versus maintenance within the same study.

The purpose of the present study was to directly compare theoretical predictors of physical activity adoption versus maintenance among healthy adults enrolled in a physical activity promotion trial. All participants were sedentary at baseline. After 6 months in the trial participants had either adopted regular physical activity or remained inactive. Psychosocial variables were measured at 6 months and used to predict 12-month physical activity status. Thus, 12-month physical activity status was an indicator of physical activity adoption among those who were inactive at 6 months and an indicator of physical activity maintenance among those who were regularly active at 6 months (Fig. 1). In order to directly compare predictors of adoption and maintenance, 6-month physical activity status was examined as a moderator of relationships between each psychosocial predictor measured at 6 months and 12-month physical activity status.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participant flow chart and plan for predicting physical activity maintenance (a) and adoption (b)

Consistent with the framework of Rothman et al. [14, 17], we hypothesized that self-efficacy and outcome expectations would be more important for physical activity adoption, but that social support and perceived satisfaction (including perceived enjoyment) would be more important for physical activity maintenance. Additionally, consistent with the TTM, we hypothesized that decisional balance and cognitive process of change would be more important for physical activity adoption, whereas behavioral processes would be more important for physical activity maintenance [4]. Finally, because neither SCT [8, 15, 16] nor Rothman and colleagues [17] provide clear hypotheses, we explored the relative role of environmental access in physical activity adoption versus maintenance.

Methods

Participants

Participants were healthy, previously sedentary adults enrolled in a randomized controlled physical activity promotion trial, recruited primarily through local newspaper advertisements (for a full description of recruitment procedures see [26]). A majority of participants were recruited from the Providence, RI area; however, in order to obtain a more diverse sample, 25% of participants were recruited from the Pittsburgh, PA area. For the purposes of this study, of the 249 participants randomized at baseline, we retained the 205 participants who provided physical activity data at both 6 and 12 months. The study was approved by The Miriam Hospital and University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Boards, and all participants provided written consent to participate.

Measures

Physical Activity

The 7-day Physical Activity Recall (PAR) interview was used to assess physical activity. This interview asks participants to recall sitting, sleep, and physical activity frequency and duration during the previous 7 days. Several studies have demonstrated both the reliability and validity of the PAR [for reviews, see 27, 28]. We took a number of steps to reduce bias and increase consistency of interview administration, including (a) having one Masters-level staff member, who was blinded to participant condition, conduct all of the 7-day PARs; (b) audio-recording all 7-day PAR interviews with independent scoring of a random subsample, and quarterly feedback sessions in which feedback was given based on interview discrepancies; and (c) demonstrating moderate intensity at baseline via a practice brisk walk and asking participants to refer to that demonstration when reporting their activity.

Self-efficacy

We measured self-efficacy using a five-item questionnaire examining confidence in participating in physical activity in five different situations (i.e., bad weather, vacation, bad mood, feeling tired, and not having enough time; [29]). This measure has shown test–retest reliability of 0.90 over 2 weeks [29] and has been significantly predictive of physical activity behavior in previous studies [5, 21]. Internal consistency was 0.86 in the present study at 6 months.

Decisional Balance

Decisional balance was assessed using a 16-item questionnaire [30] that assesses beliefs about the pros (i.e., benefits) and cons (i.e., costs) of physical activity. A decisional balance index score is computed based on the number of pros and cons endorsed. Decisional balance has significantly predicted physical activity behavior in previous studies [5, 21]. In the present study, the internal consistency for the Pros subscale was 0.94 and 0.85 for the Cons subscale at 6 months.

Processes of Change

We used a 40-item scale to assess cognitive and behavioral processes of change [31]. Both cognitive and behavioral processes of change have been significantly predictive of physical activity behavior in previous studies [5, 21]. Cognitive processes were derived from the following five subscales: (1) environmental reevaluation (caring about consequence to others); (2) self reevaluation (comprehending benefits); (3) social liberation (increasing healthy opportunities); (4) consciousness raising (increasing knowledge); and (5) dramatic relief (warning of risks). Behavioral processes were derived from the following five subscales: (1) Committing yourself; (2) reminding yourself; (3) substituting alternatives; (4) enlisting social support; and (5) rewarding yourself. In the present study, the average internal consistency for the subscales was 0.86 at 6 months.

Outcome Expectations

We examined outcome expectations using a 19-item measure, in which participants are asked to rate their agreement with potential benefits of physical activity such as: “A major benefit of physical activity for me is good health” [32]. In the validation study, the scale significantly predicted physical activity behavior in college and worksite samples [32]. The internal consistency was 0.91 in the present study at 6 months.

Physical Activity Enjoyment

The 18-item Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES) was used to measure perceived attributes and enjoyment of physical activity. In the validation study the test–retest reliability was 0.60 for bicycling and 0.93 for jogging and the level of physical activity enjoyment was significantly related to type of physical activity chosen [33]. This scale has also been significantly predictive of physical activity behavior [34]. The internal consistency was 0.95 in the present study at 6 months.

Perceived Satisfaction

Perceived satisfaction was measured with a 24-item scale adapted from Sears and Stanton [35]. In that study, perceived satisfaction with physical activity as part of a weight-loss program was significantly predictive of adherence to the program. In the present study, internal consistency was 0.96 at 6 months.

Social Support

We assessed social support with a 13-item scale examining social support for physical activity from family and friends (calculated separately). In the validation study, the test–retest reliability ranged from 0.55 to 0.79, and the social support score significantly correlated with vigorous activity [36]. Internal consistency in the present study ranged from 0.91 to 0.92 at 6 months.

Environmental Access

The environmental access measure examines environmental barriers and facilitators to physical activity [9]. The 1-week test–retest reliability of this measure ranges from 0.68–0.89, and the measure was shown to significantly correlate with access to convenient facilities and home equipment [9]. In the present study, the internal consistency was 0.84, 0.70, and 0.30 for the facilities, home, and neighborhood subscales at 6 months.

Procedures

Interested participants responded to recruitment advertisements by calling our study telephone number. Eligibility and baseline assessments were then conducted over three sessions prior to randomization into one of three physical activity promotion conditions [26]. As part of the assessment protocol, participants completed psychosocial questionnaires, and the PAR was administered at baseline, 6, and 12 months. All psychosocial questionnaires were completed at home and returned in-person at the 6-month follow-up appointment. We used last value carried forward to impute missing data from the 6-month psychosocial predictor variables for all participants who had reported physical activity data at both 6 and 12 months (N = 205; 15 cases imputed for enjoyment; two to six cases imputed for all other predictor variables). The perceived satisfaction measure was added after the start of the study, and therefore, because of the much smaller sample size, we did not impute data, but simply analyzed the observed cases (n = 111).

Data Analyses

The goals of the analyses were to (a) examine 6-month psychosocial predictors of 12 month physical activity status among the full sample; and (b) compare 6-month predictors of 12-month physical activity status among participants who were active versus inactive at 6 months. We conducted preliminary analyses to examine the differences in psychosocial variables at 6 months among participants who were active versus inactive at 6 months, and to examine bivariate relationships among the 6-month psychosocial variables. These analyses provided a preliminary test of construct validity, as it was expected that active participants would be higher than inactive participants on each of the psychosocial variables and that there would be numerous interrelationships among the 6-month psychosocial variables. Additionally, we sought to examine whether response tendencies on the 6-month psychosocial variables would differentially influence—across participants who were active versus inactive at 6 months—the ability of these variables to predict 12 month physical activity status. Thus, we examined the number of occurrences of extreme responses on each of the predictor variables among the two subgroups.

For this study, we were not interested in the effects of the intervention. Therefore, data were pooled across treatment arms, and the effects of treatment were controlled in the analyses. We used logistic regression analyses, with 12-month physical activity status (i.e., active versus not active) as the dependent variable. Active was defined as participating in at least 150 min of at least moderate intensity physical activity per week or at least 60 min of vigorous intensity physical activity per week [1]. We first examined the main effects of each psychosocial variable measured at 6 months on physical activity status at 12 months among all participants. Second, we examined physical activity status at 6 months as a moderator of the relationship between the psychosocial predictors measured at 6 and 12-months after controlling for the main effects of 6-month physical activity status and the corresponding predictor variable. Third, we examined simple effects for each significant moderator model by dividing participants into active and inactive subsamples based on their physical activity status at 6 months and examining predictors of 12-month physical activity status. In all analyses, predictors were converted to z scores, such that the resulting odds ratios (ORs) reflect the odds of being physically active at 12 months for each standard unit increase in the predictor variable.

Results

Table 1 summarizes characteristics of the study sample (i.e., participants from the main trial who reported physical activity data at both 6 and 12 months; N = 205). Not included in the study sample (n = 44) were 15 participants missing 6-month data, of whom seven were active at 12 months and eight were not; 17 participants missing 12-month data, of whom six were active at 6 months and 11 were not; and 12 participants missing both 6- and 12-month physical activity data. Participants included in the analyses (N = 205) were significantly lower at baseline on social support from friends (mean = 9.71, SD = 9.01; versus mean = 13.89, SD = 10.06; p = 0.014; Cohen’s d = 0.44) and social support from family (mean = 7.12, SD = 8.32; versus mean = 10.20, SD = 8.26; p = 0.028; d = 0.37) than participant who were excluded (n = 44). Additionally, a greater proportion of the subsample of participants who responded to the perceived satisfaction measure (n = 111)—versus those who did not respond (n = 138)—were African Americans (23.0% versus 7.3%; p < .01) because the perceived satisfaction measure was added after the start of the study, and there was more targeted recruitment of minority participants later in the study. There were no other significant differences on demographics or baseline psychosocial variables between participants who were included versus not included in the present analyses (alpha = 0.05).

Table 1 Characteristics of the study sample

Preliminary Analyses

Table 2 shows descriptive statistics for the psychosocial variables measured at 6 months among the full sample (N = 205), participants active at 6 months (n = 98), and participants inactive at 6 months (n = 107). As expected, participants who were active at 6 months were significantly higher on nine of the 12 psychosocial variables, with nonsignificant trends in the expected direction for the other three variables and effect sizes (i.e., Cohen’s d) ranging from 0.17 to 0.73 (Table 2). Table 3 illustrates the numerous bivariate relationships among the 6-month psychosocial variables. Regarding extreme responses on the 6-month psychosocial variables, cell sizes were too small to conduct chi-square analyses; however, for most of the predictor variables, there were few participants (i.e., ≤5) whose 6-month scores were at either the lowest or highest possible value. Similarly, for most of the predictor variables, there were small differences (i.e., ≤2) between the number of participants in each subgroup (i.e., active versus inactive at 6 months) whose 6-month scores were at either the lowest or highest possible value. Exceptions were: (a) outcome expectations, for which 17 participants who were inactive at 6 months versus nine participants who were active at 6 months scored the highest possible value; (b) social support for family, for which 23 participants who were inactive at 6 months versus ten participants who were active at 6 months scored the lowest possible value; and (c) social support for friends, for which 40 participants who were inactive at 6 months versus 22 participants who were active at 6 months scored the lowest possible value.

Table 2 Mean (SD) for psychosocial predictors measured at 6 months for the full sample, participants active at 6 months, and participants inactive at 6 months
Table 3 Bivariate relationships among the 6-month psychosocial variables (N = 205)

Main Effects of 6-Month Psychosocial Variables on 12-Month Physical Activity Status

Table 4 shows main effects of each predictor variable on 12-month physical activity status among the full sample (N = 205) when controlling for treatment condition. Following a Holm step-down procedure [37] self-efficacy, decisional balance, behavioral processes, outcome expectations, and enjoyment retained significance. Self-efficacy was the strongest predictor, with a 139% increase in odds of being physically active at 12 months with each one standard unit increase in self-efficacy.

Table 4 Odds ratios (95% CI) for 6-month predictors of 12-month physical activity status among the full sample of participants (N = 205)

Moderator and Simple Effects Analyses

When controlling for treatment condition, 6-month physical activity status (i.e., active or inactive) moderated the relationship between home access to physical activity equipment measured at 6 months and 12-month physical activity status (p = .049). Similarly, the interaction between 6-month physical activity status (i.e., active or inactive) and 6-month measures of self-efficacy (p = .086) and perceived satisfaction (p = .062) approached significance. No other moderator effect was found. Simple effects analyses revealed that home access to physical activity equipment was significantly predictive of physical activity adoption (OR = 1.73; 95% CI: 1.05, 2.85), but not physical activity maintenance (OR = .88; 95% CI: 0.58, 1.35), whereas self-efficacy was significantly predictive of physical activity maintenance (OR = 2.65; 95% CI: 1.55, 4.52), but not physical activity adoption (OR = 1.50; 95% CI: 0.87, 2.57). Similar to self-efficacy, perceived satisfaction was nearly significantly predictive of physical activity maintenance (OR = 1.95; 95% CI: 0.93, 4.06), but not physical activity adoption (OR = 0.82, CI: 0.44, 1.52).

Discussion

This study compared psychosocial predictors of physical activity adoption and maintenance. All participants were sedentary at baseline and had either successfully adopted physical activity or remained inactive over an initial 6-month period. Scores on 6-month psychosocial variables were then used to predict 12-month physical activity status (i.e., active versus inactive). Consistent with SCT [8] and the TTM [3], a number of variables measured at 6 months were predictive of 12-month physical activity status among the full sample of participants, including self-efficacy, decisional balance, behavioral processes, outcome expectations, and enjoyment. Similarly, differences in means on each psychosocial variable for participants who were active at 6 months versus inactive at 6 months were in the expected direction, with weak effects (d < 0.2) for two of 12 variables, small–medium effects (d = 0.2–0.5) for 5 of 12 variables, and medium-large effects (d = 0.5–0.8) for 5 of the 12 variables [38]. However, when predictors of adoption and maintenance were compared through moderator analyses, self-efficacy and perceived satisfaction were more important in predicting physical activity maintenance than physical activity adoption, whereas access to home exercise equipment was more important in predicting physical activity adoption than physical activity maintenance.

In general, the results of the moderator analyses were not consistent with Rothman and colleagues’ [17] model, which proposes that self-efficacy and outcome expectancies are critical for behavior adoption, while perceived satisfaction and social support are more important predictors of behavior maintenance. Perceived satisfaction was more important during physical activity maintenance than during physical activity adoption, which is consistent with the model; but, self-efficacy was also more important for physical activity maintenance than physical activity adoption, which is contrary to the model. The latter finding may have been influenced by lack of experience with the target behavior among the inactive group when completing the self-efficacy measure at 6 months, as Bandura [8] has noted that participants must have some level of experience with the target behavior in order to have a basis for judging their self-efficacy. Also, contrary to the model, neither outcome expectations nor decisional balance was a stronger predictor of physical activity adoption compared to physical activity maintenance. It should be noted, however, that the measure of outcome expectations grouped together the physical, social, and self-evaluative outcomes theorized to influence behavior [8]. Thus, future research should examine the relative influence of these expectancy subtypes on physical activity adoption and maintenance.

Findings also did not support the TTM’s principle that cognitive processes of change are more important during behavior adoption, and behavioral processes are more important during behavior maintenance [4]. In the current study, both cognitive and behavioral processes were predictive of 12-month physical activity status, but there was no difference in these relationships among those who were inactive at 6 months and attempting physical activity adoption compared to those who were active at 6 months and attempting to maintain physical activity. Though contrary to the TTM, the present findings are consistent with previous research in the physical activity domain [39].

Finally, results also suggest that home access to exercise equipment may be more important during physical activity adoption than maintenance. According to SCT, the environment is important to both exercise adoption and maintenance [8]. Its relative importance in predicting physical activity adoption versus maintenance is a novel finding that requires further study.

The strengths of the study include its longitudinal design and direct, statistical comparison of predictors of physical activity adoption versus maintenance. Additionally, unlike previous studies, which have relied on retrospective data to determine who was active or inactive at baseline [2225], in this study, all participants were sedentary at baseline and were followed for 6 months to determine their initial physical activity status. Moreover, previous studies have looked at predictors of physical activity “maintenance” by examining all participants, regardless of their initial physical activity status [1820]. As can be seen from the present results, separating out active and inactive participants can lead to different conclusions about the predictors of physical activity maintenance.

There were a number of limitations to the study. First, in order to operationally define adoption and maintenance, continuous physical activity data, as measured by the PAR, were used to classify participants into active versus inactive categories consistent with national guidelines [1]. Thus, despite the multiple strategies used to reduce measurement error, there is the possibility of misclassification error [40, 41]. Second, there was a large amount of missing data for the perceived satisfaction measure, resulting in a small sample size for analyses involving this variable. However, as noted above, the missing data resulted from late addition of the measure to the study protocol, instead of participant nonresponse. Nonetheless, the small sample size for this variable resulted in reduced power in the corresponding analyses. Third, the internal consistency for the neighborhood environmental access measure was low (i.e., .30); thus, further investigation into the influence of this variable on adoption and maintenance of physical activity is warranted. Fourth, although there were numerous bivariate relationships among the psychosocial predictor variables, the small sample size, relative to the number of predictor variables, precluded more sophisticated modeling techniques that are needed to understand more complex interrelationships among key variables in the adoption and maintenance processes. Fifth, due to the time elapsed between measurement of the predictors and the dependent variable (i.e., 6 months), the present analyses provide only a snapshot of the potential processes involved in physical activity adoption and maintenance, including potential changes in the predictors and/or the dependent variable. Sixth, despite recruitment of men and women, the sample was predominantly female (83.9%). Although this is typical for physical activity promotion trials [6, 7, 42], more research is needed among men to ensure generalizability of the findings.

Additionally, after conducting multiple tests for moderation, only home access to physical activity equipment was a statistically significant moderator (alpha = 0.05). Moreover, in conducting the moderator analyses, it was necessary to examine all participants at the same time point. As a result, the analysis compares predictors of physical activity maintenance to predictors of what might be considered delayed physical activity adoption among a group of participants who did not immediately adopt physical activity over the first 6 months of an intervention program. The predictors of immediate physical activity adoption may be different from predictors of delayed adoption. Thus, an alternative approach would be to examine predictors of initial adoption among a sedentary sample, and then to examine predictors of maintenance among a subsample that has successfully adopted the behavior. In this approach, however, the two sets of predictors cannot be statistically compared through moderator analysis due to the different time frames. Thus, a combination of research approaches may be necessary to uncover differences between predictors of physical activity adoption and maintenance.

Finally, as with all observational research, the findings cannot be used to draw causal conclusions concerning determinants of physical activity adoption and maintenance. Instead, the findings may be viewed as a means for generating hypotheses to be tested in experimental research [43]. Importantly, the present findings provide initial indication that different variables may operate in predicting physical activity adoption versus maintenance and highlight the need for separate examination of psychosocial predictors among those who are initially active versus inactive.