Introduction

Rather than reflect extremes of normal behavior, it is becoming increasingly recognized that excessive Internet, gaming, or social media use can be associated with symptoms and phenomenology consistent with addiction [1]. These symptoms include urges and craving, cue salience, mood dysregulation, sleep disturbances, as well as effects analogous to tolerance and withdrawal [2]. Regardless, of the classification of people as “addicted” or not, the mechanics of which are still debated [3] and not yet finalized in the DSM [4], Internet users present varying degrees of addiction-like symptoms and these symptoms, even at low-medium levels, may be bothering and reduce various aspects of users’ performance and quality of life [5]. We hence take a two-pronged approach in this study. For theory development and testing, we treat addiction symptoms as a continuous concept the level of which, regardless of classification, can lead to reduced wellbeing [6]. This approach is consistent with prior research and allows for theorizing without assuming the so far inexistent scientific knowledge needed for Internet addiction classification [7]. Next, for adding a secondary layer of confidence in the proposed theory, we test it using an established classification of people as at-risk for technology addiction [8]. This classification is not meant to represent a clinical diagnosis nor does it meant to replace clinical interviews. It just detects at-risk individuals and can only serve as an initial basis for clinical interviews.

In this study we focus specifically on symptoms of addiction to social networking site (SNS) use. This addiction is defined as a state of maladaptive dependency on the use of SNS which manifests in an obsessive-compulsive pattern of seeking and engaging in SNS use to the extent that it produces addiction-like symptoms, such as withdrawal, tolerance, salience, conflict, relapse and mood modification [9]. Estimates of maladaptive or pathological use patterns for SNS have ranged between 1.6% (Nigeria) up to 34% (China/Hong Kong) in various international samples focusing mostly on adolescents and young-adults [10]. A recent study of 5961 Hungarian adolescents demonstrated a prevalence rate of at-risk for SNS addiction people to be 4.5%, but also showed that many in the rest of the population present varying levels of addiction-like symptoms in relation to SNS use [8].

Prior research has pointed to unresolved sex-based differences, not only in prevalence of addiction symptoms, but also possibly in the neurobiological and psychosocial roots of such addictions and the way the sexes respond to and deal with addiction symptoms. For example, women are more prone than men to SNS addiction [11] and present stronger addiction symptoms [9]. In the abovementioned Hungarian sample, 58.8% of the at-risk group included women. In addition, females presented a higher association between Internet addiction and depression compared to males [12]. Other sex-based differences in addiction correlates include parental alcohol abuse, ADHD, personality traits, stress reactivity, and mescorticolimbic function [13]. However, possible differences in the way the sexes react to SNS addiction symptoms have not been fully explored.

Moreover, personality traits, and specifically neuroticism [a tendency to experience negative emotions, especially in stressful situations, 14] appear to play a prominent role in excessive Internet applications consumption. A meta-analysis of big-five personality association with Internet addiction found a large positive effect size for neuroticism [14]. Personality disorders, which correlate positively with neuroticism [15] are also associated with maladaptive Internet use. Neuroticism increases the likelihood of developing addictive behaviors, potentially as a maladaptive coping strategy, in addition to conferring a greater risk of developing Axis I disorders such as depression, a negative indicator of personal wellbeing [16]. On average, women have slightly, but significantly higher mean neuroticism scores relative to men [17]. However, it is not entirely clear how sex and neuroticism interact to influence the way users cope with and subjectively feel when addiction symptoms emerge in relation to social media use.

Collectively, findings regarding possible sex and neuroticism-based differences regarding Internet-related addictions suggest that more research on how the sexes differ, under different neuroticism conditions, in their responses to Internet, and specifically SNS addiction, is warranted. Findings from such studies can guide the development of more nuanced interventions. In this study, we seek to address this gap and examine one mechanism that can account for sex-neuroticism-based differences in the association between SNS addiction symptoms and wellbeing. Wellbeing is defined as the evaluation of one’s own quality of life, health, and satisfaction [18]. When people use the Internet (and SNS) compulsively, they often incur a range of adverse consequences, such as mood swings, conflict with other activities, reduced health, poor sleep, diminished academic and job performance, complaints from other people, difficulties in emotion regulation, and negative emotions such as guilt [5, 19]. Under such circumstances, it is reasonable to assume that their wellbeing is diminished. This view received support in numerous studies on addictive Interment use [20,21,22]. Even outside the context of problematic use, engagement with social media is associated with reduced wellbeing; the more people use Facebook, the lower their subsequent wellbeing was (alternative models ruled out the possibility that poor wellbeing predicted increased Facebook use) [23]. Taken together, we hypothesize that (H1) SNS addiction will be negatively associated with wellbeing.

Neuroticism is a key determinant of the way humans respond to negative emotional stimuli; individuals with high neuroticism generate stronger negative reactions to stimuli including visceral pain [24], stress induction [25] and negative affective priming [26, 27]. As such, it is reasonable to assume that people high in neuroticism subjectively assess SNS addiction symptoms as more problematic, stressful, emotionally draining and painful compared with people with low levels of neuroticism. Given that such responses diminish one’s wellbeing [28], people high in neuroticism should have reduced wellbeing compared with people low in neuroticism, in association with the same level of SNS addiction symptoms. We hence hypothesize that (H2) neuroticism will moderate (augment) the negative association between SNS addiction and wellbeing.

Lastly, it is reasonable to assume that the abovementioned moderated association is sex-dependent and that it is stronger for females. This idea is rooted in the notion that women can respond differently than men to stressors [29], including at the hormonal level [30], and have lower coping ability with stressors, compared with men [31]. On average, women see stressors as threats that require avoidance and elicit strong negative emotional response, while men, more often, see them as challenges that elicit weaker negative emotional response [32, 33]. Moreover, endocrinological processes may contribute to the differences in expression of poor wellbeing in males versus females- cortisol levels were negatively related to neuroticism in females, but positively related in men, reflecting biological differences in stress responses [34]. In addition, a study on the etiological pathways to major depression (a negative indicator of wellbeing) using opposite-sex twin pairs found that for women, neuroticism was 30% more potent in its influence on major depression as compared to men. Consequently, neuroticism was among the significant factors leading to major depression in women, whereas it was not a significant etiological factor for men [35]. Given that as per H2, the addiction symptoms- wellbeing association depends on one’s level of neuroticism, it is reasonable to assume a three-way interaction involving sex-based differences. We therefore hypothesize that (H3) sex moderates the neuroticism effect on the negative association between SNS addiction and wellbeing, such that this effect is stronger (more negative) for women.

Ultimately, this theory is in line with the “telescoping effect” developed in the substance and gambling addiction domains [36]. According to this perspective, while women are generally less likely than men to become addicted or present addiction-like symptoms, when they do, they present faster progression and a more severe clinical profile, including stronger psychological problems, compared to men. Our theory suggest that in the case of SNS, one reason for these differences is the way that women associate symptoms and wellbeing is influenced by their level of neuroticism, and is different from the way men deal with this association. Testing this model can therefore shed light on possible similarities between SNS addiction and substance addictions and point to the need for possible differential treatments for men and women who present addiction-like symptoms in relation to SNS use.

Methods

Participants

Israeli college students who use SNS (Facebook) were invited to participate in this study (n = 260); 215 completed the online survey (82.7% response rate). Inclusion criteria included active usage of Facebook at the time of the survey and signing a consent form that was approved by the Institutional Review Board of a research University. Exclusion criteria included age < 18. No exclusions were made. All participants gave an online informed consent when they started the survey. The university student segment of SNS users was deemed to be appropriate for this study because it presents varying degrees of SNS addiction symptoms [9] that are often associated with reduced wellbeing [37].

Procedure

Minimum sample size for an effect size of 0.15, desired power of 0.8, twelve predictors (one main effect, five controls, two direct effects of moderators, three two-way interactions, and one three-way interaction term), and probability level of 0.05 was calculated a-priori to be 127. An invitation to participate in a social media study was published on a bulletin board of a psychology department. Those who decided to participate completed the consent form and the subsequent online survey. No compensation was provided. There were no missing data.

Measures

Appendix 1 describes scale sources; it also provides the English version of measurement scales, and their descriptive and psychometric properties.

Statistical Analysis

Details regarding analyses are given in Appendix 2.

Results

The sample included 215 Facebook users (158 women). Their average age was 26.99 (20–65, SD = 6.02). Their average Facebook friends score was 7.36 (1–9, SD = 2.05). Through extrapolation, the average number of Facebook friends is estimated to be 336. Their average Facebook daily use frequency score was 4.93 (1–6, SD = 1.22). Through extrapolation, the average Facebook daily use frequency is estimated to be 2.4 times per day. The sample’s average Facebook daily use time score was 3.08 (1–6, SD = 1. 47). Through extrapolation, the average Facebook daily use is estimated to be 1.1 h per day. Based on WHO-5 scores and recommended cutoffs, the sample included 73 people (34%) who were at-risk for low mood (or mild depression) and 13 people (6%) at-risk for major depression. Based on addiction symptoms scores and suggested cutoffs, the sample included nine people (all women; 4.2% of the population) who may be at-risk of SNS addiction. This is comparable to results obtained in prior research, using the English version of the scale in adolescents [4.5% in 8], which strengthens the confidence in the validity of the translated scale.

The indices provided in Appendix 1 combined with the CFA results reported in the “Measures” section and pilot test results demonstrated that all scales were sufficiently valid and reliable. The AIC and BIC indices of models including the four ordinal controls treated as continuous predictors of wellbeing were consistently better (much smaller) than these indices in models operationalizing the four ordinal controls with dummy variables (i.e., as discrete ordinal variables). It was therefore reasonable to treat the ordinal controls as continuous variables.

The correlations matrix is provided in Appendix 1. Results of the hierarchical regression models are presented in the left columns of Table 1 and depicted in Fig. 1. These results lend support to all three hypotheses. The three-way interaction model explained 64.4% of the variance in wellbeing. Additional analyses are reported in the two rightmost columns of Table 1. A model that uses a binary addiction variable produced similar results to those obtained by using a continuous addiction symptoms variable; the three way interaction was significantly associated with wellbeing.

Table 1 Path coefficients and model indices†, ††, †††
Fig. 1
figure 1

Estimated research model

The three-way interaction is plotted in Fig. 2. It first shows that men and women were similar in that those with high neuroticism, regardless of level of addiction, presented lower wellbeing compared to those low in neuroticism. The plot further shows that while the slopes of addiction symptoms’ effects on wellbeing are similar for men in high and low neuroticism conditions, they differ for women. Women high in neuroticism seem to bear more negative SNS addiction consequences, as manifested in reduced wellbeing, compared to others. Slope difference tests support this observation and indicated that there is a significant difference between slopes (1) and (3) (t = −0.3125, p < 0.002). This means that while men with low and high neuroticism have statistically similar slopes of addiction symptoms association with wellbeing, women with high levels of neuroticism present much steeper addiction-to-wellbeing slopes compared to women with low neuroticism. In addition, slope difference tests pointed to marginal difference between slopes (3) and (4) (t = 1.691, p < 0.092). This means that that among participants low in neuroticism; men actually have a somewhat steeper association between addiction symptoms and wellbeing. The simple slope test indicated that addiction symptoms predict wellbeing when the moderators are set to 0 (simple slope = −0.462, t = −0.531, p < 0.000). Overall, this analysis points to sex-based and neuroticism-level based differences in the association between SNS addiction symptoms and wellbeing.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Three-way interaction plot

The logistic regression results with WHO-5 ≤ 50 classification as the outcome are provided in the fifth column of Table 1. They show that addiction symptoms, neuroticism and being a female are associated with increased likelihood of being at-risk for low mood. The significant three-way interaction term indicates that women, high in neuroticism and addiction, have higher (17 fold) odds of being at-risk for low mood, compared to men, low in neuroticism and with low addiction symptoms. The model reached 85.1% general accuracy, and classified at risk for low mood correctly in 72.6% of the cases.

Focusing on at-risk for major depression classification (WHO-5 ≤ 28), we had only 13 positive cases and hence ran a model with only the main predictor (SNS addiction). The coefficient for addiction (β = 1.962, exp.(β) = 7.12, p < 0.001) indicated that an increase of one unit in SNS addiction symptoms is associated with a seven-fold increased likelihood of being at risk for major depression. Nevertheless, it failed to classify people as possibly being at risk for major depression.

Discussion

This study examined how neuroticism and sex interact to influence the established negative association between SNS addiction symptoms and subjective wellbeing in a sample of college-age Israeli participants. Firstly, we confirmed H1 and showed that higher scores on SNS addiction symptoms are related to lower subjective wellbeing. We also observed that higher neuroticism was associated with lower wellbeing. Lending support to H2, we showed that high neuroticism was associated with a stronger negative relationship between SNS addiction symptoms and wellbeing. Although neuroticism was associated with lower wellbeing independent of sex and SNS addiction symptoms score, the context of high SNS addiction had stronger negative association with female wellbeing: the association between addiction symptoms and wellbeing was similar for men regardless of their level of neuroticism, whereas for females, high neuroticism magnified the negative association between SNS addiction symptoms and wellbeing. This result confirmed our third, critical hypothesis. Support for this hypothesis was also provided when using a binary coded addiction variable that classified people to at-risk and low/no risk of SNS addiction categories. Another observation based on the three-way interaction was that for individuals low in neuroticism, men actually have a slightly steeper association between addiction symptoms and wellbeing. This suggests moderating factors apart from neuroticism are more relevant to men as compared to women in explaining associations between SNS addiction symptoms and wellbeing.

The model ultimately supports the key ideas on which we built the hypotheses, including the notions that (1) SNS addiction symptoms can be conceived as a persistent stressor that is associated with reduced wellbeing, (2) neuroticism alters the way people interpret and deal with such stressors, and (3) women differ from men in the way they subjectively associate combined SNS addiction symptoms-neuroticism states with wellbeing. There are, to our knowledge, no studies linking sex differences in neuroticism and wellbeing in the context of addiction and especially SNS addiction symptoms. Taking a broader addiction perspective, the findings demonstrate that the “telescoping effect”, which is common in substance abuse and gambling contexts, can be relevant to SNS addiction and there are possible similarities between the way sex is associated with SNS addiction and the way it is associated with other addictions.

Ultimately, the preliminary results of this study point to a need for further inquiries into how (and why) males and females differ in associations with behavioral addictions in order to identify effective treatments. From a practical standpoint, they suggest that tailored treatments should be developed based on one’s sex and neurotic traits; special attention to wellbeing improvement techniques should be given in the case of women high in neuroticism who seek treatment for Internet-related addictions. The efficacy of this approach should be tested in future research.

Several limitations of this study that point to interesting future research are noteworthy. First, this study was cross-sectional in nature. Longitudinal designs can help in establishing better causality. Second, this study did not account for possible comorbidities, even though such comorbidities are common with Internet addictions [38]. Future research can better account for potential effects of additional co-present disorders and states. Third, we employed a simple set of suggested cutoffs for creating at risk classifications for addiction and low-mood. Future research can employ more robust depression and addiction screening. Fourth, the chosen statistical techniques are limited; future research can supplement them with qualitative data, clinical interviews and larger samples. Fifth, future research may use continuous control variables rather than ordinal one. Lastly, this study used subjective measures. Future research can extend our findings with experimental contexts that employ physiological measures, such as psychophysiological or hormonal indices and neuroimaging.

Conclusions

This study suggests that sex-based differences exist in the way SNS addiction symptoms, neuroticism and wellbeing are associated. This finding is theoretically significant because it develops a new theory that captures sex-neuroticism profile associations, points to additional possible similarities between behavioral addictions on the spectrum of Internet addiction disorders and more established substance and gambling addictions, and provides initial support to the extension of the “telescoping effect” from substance and gambling addictions to the case of SNS addiction symptoms. The findings are practically meaningful because they pave the way for the development of more tailored interventions aiming to improve the wellbeing or alleviate the suffering of people who present addiction symptoms in relation to SNS use. We call for future research to further examine sex-based differences in people who present addiction symptoms in relation to the use of various technologies.