Introduction

Boston received national acclaim for its innovative approach to preventing youth violence in the 1990s (see, e.g. Butterfield 1996; Witkin 1997). The well-known Operation Ceasefire initiative was an interagency violence prevention program that focused enforcement and social service resources on a small number of gang-involved offenders at the heart of the city’s youth violence problem (Kennedy et al. 1996). The Ceasefire “pulling levers” focused deterrence strategy was associated with a near two-thirds drop in youth homicide in the late 1990s (Braga et al. 2001; Piehl et al. 2003) and was soon embraced by the U.S. Department of Justice as an effective approach to crime prevention. In his address to the American Society of Criminology, former National Institute of Justice Director Jeremy Travis (1998) announced “[the] pulling levers hypothesis has made enormous theoretical and practical contributions to our thinking about deterrence and the role of the criminal justice system in producing safety.” Subsequently, the basic elements of the Boston Ceasefire framework has been applied in many American cities through federally sponsored violence prevention programs such as the Strategic Alternatives to Community Safety Initiative and Project Safe Neighborhoods (Dalton 2002).

The evaluation of Boston’s Operation Ceasefire, however, has been greeted with both a healthy dose of skepticism (Fagan 2002; Rosenfeld et al. 2005) and some support (Cook and Ludwig 2006; Morgan and Winship 2007). The relatively weak quasi-experimental evaluation design of the original implementation leaves some uncertainty about the size of Ceasefire’s effect on gang violence in Boston and does not provide any direct evidence that Boston gangs subjected to the Ceasefire intervention actually changed their offending behaviors (Ludwig 2005; Wellford et al. 2005). Given the influence of the Operation Ceasefire experience on policing and violence prevention policy, a more rigorous examination of the intervention’s effects on street gang behavior in Boston is sorely needed.

In this paper, we take advantage of unique data on gangs and gang-involved gun violence in Boston in a quasi-experimental evaluation of the group-level violence prevention effects of a reconstituted Operation Ceasefire strategy implemented in 2007. As compared to previous evaluations of Operation Ceasefire that focused solely on aggregate rates of violence, our quasi-experimental evaluation focuses squarely on the gangs that were targeted for treatment. Propensity score matching techniques were used to develop matched Ceasefire treatment gangs and comparison gangs. Growth-curve regression models were then used to estimate the impact of Ceasefire on gun violence trends for the treatment gangs relative to comparisons gangs. We find that the Ceasefire intervention was associated with statistically significant reductions in gun violence trends for treatment gangs relative to gun violence trends for the comparison gangs. A supplementary analysis examined the specific timing of the Ceasefire intervention as applied to each matched treatment gang and found that sharp reductions in gun violence immediately followed the intervention.

Literature Review

The Boston Gun Project and Operation Ceasefire

The Boston Gun Project was a problem-oriented policing enterprise expressly aimed at taking on a serious, large-scale crime problem—homicide victimization among young people in Boston. Like many large cities in the United States, Boston experienced a large sudden increase in youth homicide between the late 1980s and early 1990s. The Project began in early 1995 and implemented what is now known as the “Operation Ceasefire” intervention, which started in the late spring of 1996 (Kennedy et al. 1996). Led by the Boston Police Department (BPD), a working group of law enforcement personnel, youth workers, and Harvard University researchers diagnosed the youth violence problem in Boston as one of patterned, largely vendetta-like (“beef”) hostility amongst a small population of chronic offenders, and particularly among those involved in loose, informal, mostly neighborhood-based gangs. These gangs represented less than 1 % of the city’s youth between the ages of 14 and 24, but were responsible for more than 60 % of youth homicide in Boston.

The focused deterrence strategy behind Operation Ceasefire was designed to prevent violence by reaching out directly to gangs, saying explicitly that violence would no longer be tolerated, and backing up that message by “pulling every lever” legally available when violence occurred (Kennedy 1997, 2011). The chronic involvement of gang members in a wide variety of offenses made them—and their groups—vulnerable to a coordinated criminal justice response. The authorities could disrupt street drug activity, focus police attention on low-level street crimes such as trespassing and public drinking, serve outstanding warrants, cultivate confidential informants for medium- and long-term investigations of gang activities, deliver strict probation and parole enforcement, seize drug proceeds and other assets, ensure stiffer plea bargains and sterner prosecutorial attention, request stronger bail terms (and enforce them), and bring potentially severe federal investigative and prosecutorial attention to gang-related drug and gun activity. Rather than simply dealing with individual offending, groups were held accountable for outbreaks of serious gun violence.

Simultaneously, youth workers, probation and parole officers, and later churches and other community groups offered gang members services and other kinds of help. These partners also delivered an explicit message that violence was unacceptable to the community and that “street” justifications for violence were mistaken. The Ceasefire Working Group delivered this message in formal meetings with gang members (known as “forums” or “call-ins”), through individual police and probation contacts with gang members, through meetings with inmates at secure juvenile facilities in the city, and through gang outreach workers. The deterrence message was not a deal with gang members to stop violence. Rather, it was a promise to gang members that violent behavior would evoke an immediate and intense response. If gangs committed other crimes but refrained from violence, the normal workings of police, prosecutors, and the rest of the criminal justice system dealt with these matters. But if gang members persisted in their violent behaviors, the Working Group concentrated its enforcement actions on their gangs.

The idea of the Ceasefire “crackdowns” specifically but the focused deterrence model more generally was not to eliminate gangs or stop every aspect of gang activity, but rather to control and deter serious violence among specified groups (Kennedy 1997). To do this, the Working Group explained its actions against targeted gangs to other gangs, as in “this gang did violence, we responded with the following actions, and here is how to prevent anything similar from happening to you.” The ongoing Working Group process regularly watched the city for outbreaks of gang violence and framed any necessary responses in accord with the Ceasefire strategy. As the strategy unfolded, the Working Group continued communication with gangs and gang members to convey its determination to stop violence, to explain its actions to the target population, and to maximize both voluntary compliance and the strategy’s deterrent power.

Operation Ceasefire Deterrence Mechanisms

Deterrence theory posits that crimes can be prevented when the costs of committing the crime are perceived by the offender to outweigh the benefits (Gibbs 1975; Zimring and Hawkins 1973). Most discussions of the deterrence mechanism distinguish between “general” and “special” deterrence (Cook 1980). General deterrence is the idea that the general population is dissuaded from committing crime when it sees that punishment necessarily follows the commission of a crime. Special deterrence involves punishment administered to criminals with the intent to discourage them from committing crimes in the future. Much of the literature evaluating deterrence focuses on the effect of changing certainty, swiftness, and severity of punishment associated with certain acts on the prevalence of those crimes (see, e.g. Apel and Nagin 2011; Blumstein et al. 1978; Cook 1980; Nagin 1998; Paternoster 1987).

In addition to any increases in certainty, severity, and swiftness of sanctions associated with gun violence, the Operation Ceasefire strategy sought to gain deterrence through the advertising of the law enforcement strategy, and the personalized nature of its application. The effective operation of general deterrence is dependent on the communication of punishment threats to the public. As Zimring and Hawkins (1973) observe, “the deterrence threat may best be viewed as a form of advertising” (p. 142). A key element of the strategy was the delivery of a direct and explicit “retail deterrence” message to a relatively small target audience regarding what kind of behavior would provoke a special response and what that response would be.Footnote 1

The available research suggests that deterrent effects are ultimately determined by offender perceptions of sanction risk and certainty (Nagin 1998). As described above, Operation Ceasefire was targeted on very specific behaviors by a relatively small number of chronic offenders who were highly vulnerable to criminal justice sanctions. The approach directly confronted violent gang members and informed them that continued offending will not be tolerated and how the system will respond to violations of these new behavior standards. Face-to-face meetings with offenders are an important first step in altering their perceptions about sanction risk (Horney and Marshall 1992; Nagin 1998). As McGarrell et al. (2006) suggest, direct communications and affirmative follow-up responses are the types of new information that may cause offenders to reassess the risks of committing crimes.

In their recent essay on the limits of lengthy prison stays to deter crime, Durlauf and Nagin (2011, p. 40) suggest that “strategies that result in large and visible shifts in apprehension risk are most likely to have deterrent effects that are large enough not only to reduce crime but also apprehensions.” Focused deterrence strategies, such as Boston’s Operation Ceasefire, are identified by Durlauf and Nagin (2011) as having this characteristic. Moreover, they suggest that these “carrot and stick approaches” to crime prevention creatively use positive incentives, such as social services and job opportunities, to reward compliance and facilitate nonviolent behavior. Durlauf and Nagin (2011) conclude their discussion of the promise of focused deterrence strategies with a call for additional research and evaluation on the crime reduction benefits of these new approaches.

Evaluation Evidence

A large reduction in the yearly number of Boston youth homicides followed immediately after Operation Ceasefire was implemented in mid-1996. A U.S. Department of Justice (DOJ)-sponsored evaluation of Operation Ceasefire revealed that the intervention was associated with a 63 % decrease in the monthly number of Boston youth homicides, a 32 % decrease in the monthly number of shots-fired calls, a 25 % decrease in the monthly number of gun assaults, and, in one high-risk police district given special attention in the evaluation, a 44 % decrease in the monthly number of youth gun assault incidents (Braga et al. 2001). The evaluation also suggested that Boston’s significant youth homicide reduction associated with Operation Ceasefire was distinct when compared to youth homicide trends in most major U.S. and New England cities (Braga et al. 2001). In a companion paper to the main impact evaluation, Piehl et al. (2003) developed an econometric model that evaluated all possible monthly break points in the time series to identify the maximal monthly break point associated with a significant structural change in the trajectory of the time series. Controlling for trends and seasonal variations, the timing of the “optimal break” in the monthly counts of youth homicides time series was in the summer months after Ceasefire was implemented in 1996.

Given the high profile of the Boston experience, the Ceasefire evaluation has been reviewed by a number of researchers and the relationship between the implementation of Ceasefire and the trajectory of youth homicide in Boston during the 1990s has been closely scrutinized. Fagan (2002) suggested that some of the decrease in homicide may have occurred without the Ceasefire intervention in place as violence was decreasing in most major U.S. cities. In support of this perspective, Fagan (2002) presented a simple time-series graph on youth gun homicide in Boston and in other Massachusetts cities that suggested a general downward trend in gun violence may have existed before Ceasefire was implemented. Using growth-curve analysis to examine predicted homicide trend data for the 95 largest U.S. cities during the 1990s, Rosenfeld et al. (2005) found some evidence of a sharper youth homicide drop in Boston than elsewhere but suggest that the small number of youth homicide incidents precludes strong conclusions about program effectiveness based on their statistical models. However, in his review of their analysis, Berk (2005) raised a number of statistical and methodological concerns with the analysis developed by Rosenfeld and his colleagues.

Other reviewers, however, have been more supportive of a program effect in their reviews of the Ceasefire impact evaluation (see Cook and Ludwig 2006). Ludwig (2005) suggested that Ceasefire was associated with a large drop in youth homicide but, given the complexities of analyzing city-level homicide trend data, there remained some uncertainty about the extent of Ceasefire’s effect on youth violence in Boston. Morgan and Winship’s (2007) review of the Ceasefire evaluation concluded that the analysis was a “very high-quality example” of how to conduct an interrupted time series analysis of program impact and further noted “they offer four types of supplemental analysis … which can be used to strengthen the warrant for causal assertion” (p. 252).

The National Academies’ Panel on Improving Information and Data on Firearms (Wellford et al. 2005) concluded that the Ceasefire evaluation was compelling in associating the intervention with the subsequent decline in youth homicide. However, the Panel also suggested that many complex factors affect youth homicide trends and it was difficult to specify the exact relationship between the Ceasefire intervention and subsequent changes in youth offending behaviors. The Panel further observed that the Ceasefire evaluation examined aggregate citywide data and did not provide any empirical evidence that treated gangs modified their violent behaviors after being exposed to the intervention. In a recent article in The New Yorker (Seabrook 2009, p. 37), well-respected deterrence scholar Professor Franklin Zimring echoed the concerns raised by the Panel by stating:

Ceasefire is more of a theory of treatment rather than a proven strategy … It’s odd that no one has ever said, O.K., here are the youths who were not part of the Ceasefire program in Boston, let’s compare them to the youths who were. And no one has ever followed up any long range studies of the criminal behavior of the group that was in the program, either. We just don’t have the evidence, and until we do we can’t evaluate how effective Ceasefire really is.

The Current Study

While the existing evidence is strong enough to suggest an association between the implementation of Ceasefire and the subsequent drop in Boston youth homicides, we agree with the concerns raised by the National Academies’ Panel and Professor Zimring that it is difficult to determine whether Ceasefire actually changed violent gang behaviors in Boston based on the analysis of aggregate citywide trend data during the 1990s—a period known for sudden and surprising decreases in violent crime in the United States (see, e.g. Cook and Laub 2002). A more rigorous test of Ceasefire would compare pre-test and post-test trends in gun violence outcomes by treated Boston gangs to pre-test and post-test trends in gun violence outcomes for an equivalent group of untreated Boston gangs. In this study, we take advantage of new data on gangs and gang-involved gun violence in Boston to conduct a stronger quasi-experimental evaluation of a reinvigorated version of Operation Ceasefire implemented during the late 2000s.

Despite the national acclaim, the BPD discontinued the Ceasefire strategy as its primary response to outbreaks of gang violence in January 2000 (see Braga and Winship 2006). Yearly counts of gang homicides, unfortunately, increased linearly after Ceasefire was halted in Boston (Braga et al. 2008a). In 1999, the last full year of Ceasefire intervention, there were only 5 gang-motivated homicides in Boston. By 2006, this number had increased more than seven-fold to 37 gang-motivated homicides in Boston. During this time period, the BPD experimented with alternative approaches to violence prevention by adapting certain Ceasefire tactics to a broader range of problems such as investigating unsolved shootings, facilitating the re-entry of incarcerated violent offenders back into high-risk Boston neighborhoods, and addressing criminogenic families in hot spot areas (Braga and Winship 2006). Unfortunately, the slate of new approaches seemed to diffuse the ability of the City of Boston to deal with gang violence as no one group was focused exclusively on addressing ongoing conflicts among street gangs (Braga et al. 2008a).

At the beginning of December 2006, Edward F. Davis III, former Chief of the Lowell, Massachusetts, Police Department, was sworn in by Mayor Thomas M. Menino as the new Commissioner of the BPD and was immediately charged with reducing gun violence in the city. Drawing on his past experience with a pulling levers strategy to control gang violence in Lowell (Braga et al. 2008b), Davis announced that Operation Ceasefire would once again be the BPD’s main response to outbreaks of serious gang violence. He promoted Gary French, who led many of the BPD’s Ceasefire efforts during the 1990s, to Deputy Superintendent with oversight of the Youth Violence Strike Force (YVSF, known informally as the “gang unit”), school police unit, and the tactical bicycle unit. With the support of Davis and his command staff, French reinstated the Ceasefire approach as a citywide, interagency effort to disrupt ongoing cycles of gang violence. Between January 2007 and December 2010, 19 Boston gangs were subjected to the Ceasefire pulling levers focused deterrence strategy.

Analytical Approach

We used a non-randomized quasi-experimental design to compare serious gun violence trends for Boston gangs subjected to the Ceasefire intervention to serious gun violence trends for a matched comparison group of Boston gangs that did not receive the Ceasefire intervention (Shadish et al. 2002; Rossi et al. 2006). This section describes the development of the data and units of analysis in our quasi-experiment, the identification of comparison gangs, and the specification of appropriate statistical models to estimate the effect of the Ceasefire intervention on serious gun violence trends for treated gangs relative to serious gun violence trends for comparison gangs.

Data and Units of Analysis

In this study, we measured serious gun violence by using computerized records of BPD official reports of Homicide by Firearm and Assault and Battery by Means of a Deadly Weapon—Firearm (ABDW—Firearm) incidents between January 1, 2006 and December 31, 2010. Incident reports are generated in the BPD by detectives or police officers after an initial response to a request for police service. In the State of Massachusetts, ABDW—Firearm incidents essentially represent shooting events where guns were fired and victims were physically wounded by the fired bullets.Footnote 2 The availability of non-fatal incident data has the significant advantage of allowing us to include a wider range of gang-involved gun violence. More importantly, the difference between a gun homicide and a non-fatal shooting event, as one police officer related to us, “is often only a matter of inches and luck—a lot of times a non-fatal shooting is just a failed homicide.” The officer’s sentiment suggests that whether or not an event becomes lethal is contingent on several uncontrollable factors—the aim of the shooter, the distance to the target, a rapid call to the police, the response time of medical assistance, and so on. In fact, Zimring’s (1968, 1972) studies of wounds inflicted in gun and knife assaults demonstrate considerable overlap between fatal and non-fatal attacks and suggest that the difference between life and death is just a matter of chance. In the text that follows, we use “shooting” as a term of convenience to represent both fatal and non-fatal shooting incidents.

It is well known that police incident data, such as the Federal Bureau of Investigation’s Uniform Crime Reports, have shortcomings. For instance, crime incident data are biased by the absence of crimes not reported by citizens to the police and by police decisions not to record all crimes reported by citizens (see Black 1970). Although incident reports have flaws, careful analyses of these data can yield useful insights on crime (Schneider and Wiersema 1990). Moreover, official police incident data are widely used for assessing trends and patterns of gun crime (Blumstein 1995; Cook and Laub 2002) and the evaluation of gun violence reduction programs (see, e.g. Sherman and Rogan 1995; McGarrell et al. 2001; Cohen and Ludwig 2003).

To determine whether a shooting event involved a gang member as a suspect, victim, or both, the “crime incident review” process was used (see Klofas and Hipple 2006). Between 2006 and 2010, the BPD’s Boston Regional Intelligence Center (BRIC) convened separate quarterly shooting review meetings for the four policing districts (B-2, B-3, C-11, and D-4) that experience the bulk of gun violence in Boston and one quarterly shooting review meeting for the remaining policing districts. For each district meeting, detectives and officers with detailed knowledge on gangs and gang violence problems were required to attend; this included district detectives, plainclothes Anti-Crime district officers, Drug Control Unit detectives and officers, Homicide Unit detectives, Special Investigations Unit detectives, and YVSF detectives and officers. In each quarterly shooting review meeting, BRIC detectives and civilian analysts presented the objective characteristics of each shooting event (date, location, victim information, and, if arrested, offender information) and the available gang intelligence on the event based on their computerized data systems. The meeting participants shared their working knowledge on circumstances of the shooting event, the relationships between victims and suspects, and, if the event involved gang members, details on the gangs involved in the shooting.

Researchers attended the quarterly shooting review meetings and partnered with the BRIC in collecting, coding, entering, and analyzing the qualitative insights on the nature of each shooting event. Figure 1 presents the yearly counts of gang-involved shootings in Boston between 2006 and 2010. Gang-involved shootings were relatively stable between 2006 (N = 263) and 2007 (N = 253), decreased over the course of 2008 (N = 232) and 2009 (N = 172), and, despite a small increase over the previous year, remained relatively low in 2010 (N = 183). Between 2006 and 2010, gang-involved shootings in Boston decreased by 30.4 %.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Gang-involved shootings in Boston, 2006–2010

The units of analysis in this evaluation are quarterly counts of shootings by and against specific Boston gangs between 2006 and 2010. Since shootings by and against any particular gang were relatively rare events, we aggregated specific shootings into quarterly counts to provide more stable estimates of any measurable impacts of Ceasefire on gang shooting behaviors. There were N = 123 gangs in Boston involved in at least one shooting between 2006 and 2010. We analyzed three quarterly outcomes for each gang included in the evaluation: victim gang-involved shootings, suspect gang-involved shootings, and total gang-involved shootings (victim and suspect summed).

Matching Treatment Gangs with Comparable Control Gangs

It is important to note here that evaluating Ceasefire is a particularly difficult task. The Ceasefire intervention was explicitly designed to deter continued gun violence by gangs not subjected to the treatment. As Kennedy et al. (1996, p. 181) describe in their discussion of evaluating Ceasefire in Boston during the 1990s:

…rather than trying to protect certain areas or groups from the intervention, as in the traditional experimental design, the working group went to considerable effort to design an intervention that would create “spillover” effects onto other gangs and neighborhoods – through the communications strategy, interfering in active or nascent gang vendettas, fear reduction, and the like. Thus, a traditional evaluation would find no impact—youth homicide would fall in the targeted areas … and in all other areas of the city…

Kennedy et al. (1997, p. 240) describe how social network analysis concepts were used to assist the diffusion of the deterrence message across Boston’s gang landscape:

We used structural network analysis in pursuit of support for an effective communications strategy. Here, [social network analysis software] was employed to identify naturally existing subgroups, or “cliques,” such that talking to one member would effectively be talking to all members [of that clique] … for clique identification, conflict and alliance networks were combined and analyzed.

The post-2007 version of Boston Ceasefire attempted to create these spillover effects onto other gangs that were socially connected to targeted gangs through rivalries and alliances. As Ceasefire interventions were completed on targeted gangs, the Ceasefire Working Group directly communicated to their rivals and allies that “they would be next” if these groups decided to retaliate against treated rival gangs or continue shootings in support of treated allied gangs. These messages were delivered to members of socially-connected gangs via individual meetings with gang members under probation supervision and through direct “street conversations” with gang members by BPD officers and gang outreach workers.

One key assumption that underlies all controlled program evaluations is the “stable unit treatment value assumption” (SUTVA). This assumption requires that the treatment or control condition to which a unit is assigned has no impact on the response of another unit (Rubin 1990). Including untreated Boston street gangs that were socially connected to Ceasefire gangs as comparison groups in our impact evaluation would violate SUTVA. The Ceasefire program was explicitly designed to ensure that knowledge of Ceasefire actions taken against their immediate rivals and allies would diffuse into these untreated groups and influence their subsequent gun violence behaviors. To minimize SUTVA violations, we excluded all untreated Boston street gangs that were known to have a rivalry or alliance with Ceasefire gangs from consideration as comparison groups in our quasi-experimental evaluation. This process resulted in N = 82 gangs that were not socially connected to the N = 19 Ceasefire gangs as possible comparison groups.Footnote 3

We recognize that our strategy to address possible SUTVA violations is limited. The gun violence behaviors of untreated gangs with second- and third-order social connections to treated gangs may have been impacted through the indirect transmission of knowledge on the consequences experienced by treated gangs. The Ceasefire intervention could have also affected the gun violence behaviors of untreated gangs located in proximate neighborhoods that were not socially connected to treatment gangs through local non-gang social networks. In essence, these social dynamics introduce a potential bias against establishing a statistically-significant Ceasefire treatment effect. Our analyses would then represent a very conservative test of program impacts.

Using Stata 12.1 statistical software, we executed PSMATCH2 propensity score matching routines (Leuven and Sianesi 2003) to develop matched comparison and treatment groups from the untreated gangs and the Ceasefire gangs. Propensity score matching techniques attempt to create equivalent treatment and comparison groups by summarizing relevant pre-treatment characteristics of each subject into a single-index variable (the propensity score) and then matching subjects in the untreated comparison pool to subjects in the treatment group based on values of the single-index variable (Rosenbaum and Rubin 1983, 1985). As such, we drew upon detailed information on the characteristics of Boston gangs from a recent investigation of the relative importance of prior conflicts and the proximity of gang turf on gun violence outcomes. The propensity score matching routine included the following nine characteristics:

  1. 1.

    Number of total shootings committed by each gang in 2006 (pre-Ceasefire). Gun violence among Boston gangs has been previously described as perpetuated by vendettas and ongoing series of retaliations (Kennedy et al. 1996). Gangs with higher levels of gun violence have an increased risk of persisting in their shooting behaviors over time.

  2. 2.

    Gang membership size. Gangs with larger memberships have an increased number of members who can commit or be victimized by shootings.Footnote 4

  3. 3.

    Adjacency to another gang’s turf. Research suggests that gang violence is more likely to erupt at the boundaries where gangs’ turf meet (Papachristos 2009; Tita and Greenbaum 2009; Tita and Radil 2011). Boston gangs with turf adjacent to the turf of another gang are more likely to be involved in serious gun violence.Footnote 5

  4. 4.

    Gang longevity. Gangs that have been in existence since the 1990s will have a more stable set of rivalries and a longer history of death and injury at the hands of their rivals; the longevity of these gangs and their ongoing disputes with rivals may increase the likelihood of a violent dispute during the study period.Footnote 6

  5. 5.

    Number of rivalries with other gangs. Gangs with larger numbers of rivalries with other gangs have an increased risk that one or more of these rivalries could turn into an active violent dispute that would generate a string of retaliatory shootings. Retaliation and retribution are perhaps the most frequently cited mechanisms of gang violence (Decker 1996; Hughes and Short 2005; Papachristos 2009).

  6. 6.

    Number of alliances with other gangs. Similar to alliance systems in international relations, some gangs form alliances for the benefit of mutual protection. For instance, in a unique study on gang finances and strategy in Chicago, Levitt and Venkatesh (2000) describe how one gang parlayed and negotiated such alliances to rally other groups to their aid during a gang war.

  7. 7.

    Gang located in housing project. Research has found that housing project areas are associated with increased levels of gang homicide relative to other city areas without housing projects (Smith 2012).

  8. 8.

    The concentration of social disadvantage in each gang turf area. We included an index that measured concentrated social disadvantageFootnote 7 in the 2000 US Census block group(s) surrounding gang turfs to make certain that comparison gangs were selected from neighborhoods that were similar to the neighborhoods in which the Ceasefire gangs were located. Research reveals that the degree of concentrated social disadvantage in a neighborhood is strongly correlated with the concentration of violent crime (Morenoff et al. 2001; Sampson and Wilson 1995) and gang crime in these areas (Papachristos and Kirk 2006; Rosenfeld et al. 1999).

  9. 9.

    Number of gang members arrested in 2006 (pre-Ceasefire). Finally, local police departments traditionally use arrest-based enforcement strategies to suppress gang violence (Klein 1993). Arrests of gang members could plausibly impact the likelihood that a particular gang engages in gun violence through the removal of likely “shooters” from the street.

We recognize that it would have been ideal to include a greater number of covariates in our final propensity score matching model. Indeed, the ability to balance treatment and comparison groups on as many covariates as possible is the main strength of propensity score methods. Unfortunately, these nine covariates represented the only group-level descriptors for Boston gangs available at the time of this analysis. Nevertheless, we believe our parsimonious propensity score model captures the gang-level covariates most directly associated with gun violence behaviors that would influence the selection of particular gangs for Ceasefire treatment. As we describe in detail below, our impact analysis was not affected by unobserved variables that could simultaneously affect gang assignment to the Ceasefire treatment and gun violence outcomes.

The broader propensity score matching literature identifies a wide variety of matching algorithms with different choices that need to be made when each approach is used (see, e.g. Apel and Sweeten 2010; Heckman et al. 1997; Imbens 2004; Smith and Todd 2005). We selected radius matching with a caliper = 0.01 as our primary propensity score matching algorithm. According to Dehejia and Wahba (2002), the basic idea of this variant is to use not only the nearest neighbor within each caliper but all of the comparison members within the caliper. A benefit of radius matching is that the approach uses only as many comparison units as are available within the caliper and therefore allows for usage of additional units when good matches are available or fewer units when good matches are not available (Caliendo and Kopeinig 2005). As such, the approach minimizes the risk of bad matches.

Table 1 reports the results of the propensity score radius matching with a caliper = 0.01. The table presents the pre- and post-matching t tests and the standardized bias statistics which represents the mean difference as a percentage of the average standard deviation between the groups (Rosenbaum and Rubin 1985). In the matched sample, all p values are higher than 0.05, and all bias statistics are less than 20.0 (a general ‘rule of thumb’ for balanced groups; see also Austin et al. 2007).Footnote 8 This confirmed that we achieved balanced treatment and comparison groups. PSMATCH2 radius matching (caliper = 0.01) routine revealed that the 16 matched Ceasefire treatment gangs and 37 matched comparison gangs were in the common support region. This ensures that gangs with the same X values have a positive probability of being both treated and untreated (Heckman et al. 1999).

Table 1 Balancing treatment and comparison gangs through propensity score matching

Growth-Curve Regression Model Specification

We use a variation of a multi-level negative binomial regression model in order to analyze the quarterly change in gang-involved shootings for treatment and comparison gangs over a 5-year observation period (2006–2010, N = 20 quarters).Footnote 9 More specifically, we developed individual growth curve models to estimate street gang changes in violent index crime incidents over the observation period (Gelman 2005; Singer and Willet 2003). Here we used a longitudinal negative binomial model where we predict within unit variation at level 1 and between unit variation at level 2 using level 1 intercepts and slopes as outcomes. In non-technical terms, we are interested in accurately analyzing the overall shooting trend of each of the street gangs during the observation period. Each street gang is also allowed to have its own slope and intercept in order to model different starting levels of shootings as well as different rates of change. This is consistent with the variation observed in shootings by gangs—some groups are highly active and others are less active.

Formally, the model is specified as follows where \( y_{it} \) is the count for the tth observation in the ith group. The model begins with \( y_{it} |\sim Poisson\left( {\gamma_{it} } \right) \) where \( \gamma_{it} |\sim gamma\left( {\lambda_{it} ,\delta_{i} } \right) \) with \( \lambda_{it} = \exp (x_{it} \beta + offset_{it} ) \) and \( \delta_{i} \) represents the dispersion parameter. This produces the following equation:

$$ \Pr \left( {Y_{it} = y_{it} |x_{it} ,\delta_{i} } \right) = \frac{{\Upgamma \left( {\lambda_{it} + y_{it} } \right)}}{{\Upgamma \left( {\lambda_{it} } \right)\Upgamma \left( {y_{it} + 1} \right)}}\left( {\frac{1}{{1 + \delta_{i} }}} \right)^{{\lambda_{it} }} \left( {\frac{{\delta_{i} }}{{1 + \delta_{i} }}} \right)^{{y_{it} }} $$
(1)

Following Gelman (2005) and others (Long and Freese 2006; Singer and Willet 2003), this specification yields a negative binomial model for the ith group with dispersion equal to 1 + δ, in other words, a constant dispersion within groups. Thus, we feel that such a specification fits the observed distribution of our data.

For a random-effects over-dispersion model, δ varies randomly across observational units. We therefore assume that \( \left( {\frac{1}{{1 + \delta {}_{{_{i} }}}}} \right)\sim Beta(r,s) \). Accordingly, the joint probability of the counts for the ith group is:

$$ \begin{aligned} \Pr \left( {Y_{i1} = y_{i1} , \ldots ,Y_{{in_{i} }} = y_{{in_{i} }} |X_{i} } \right) = & \int_{0}^{\infty } {\prod\limits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\Pr \left( {Y_{it} = y_{it} |x_{it} ,\delta_{i} } \right)f\left( {\delta_{i} } \right)d\delta_{i} } } \\ = &\frac{{\Upgamma \left( {r + s} \right)\Upgamma (r + \sum {_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} \lambda_{it} )\Upgamma (s + \sum\nolimits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {y_{it} )} }}}{{\Upgamma (r)\Upgamma (s)\Upgamma (r + s + \sum\nolimits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\lambda_{it} + \sum\nolimits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {y_{it} )} } }}\prod\limits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\frac{{\Upgamma (\lambda_{it} + y_{it} )}}{{\Upgamma (\lambda_{it} )\Upgamma (y_{it} + 1)}}} \\ \end{aligned} $$
(2)

For \( X_{i} = (x_{i1} , \ldots ,x_{in} ) \) and where f is the probability density for \( \delta_{i} \). This yields the following log likelihood:

$$ \begin{gathered} \ln L = \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n} {w_{i} } \left[ {\ln \Upgamma (r + s) + \ln \Upgamma \left( {r + \sum\limits_{k = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\lambda_{ik} } } \right) + \ln \Upgamma \left( {s + \sum\limits_{k = 1}^{{n_{i}^{{}} }} {y_{ik} } } \right) - \ln \Upgamma (r) = \ln \Upgamma (s)} \right. \hfill \\ \left. { - \ln \Upgamma \left( {r + s + \sum\limits_{k = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\lambda_{ik} } + \sum\limits_{k = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {y_{ik} } } \right) + \sum\limits_{t = 1}^{{n_{i} }} {\left\{ {\ln \Upgamma \left( {\lambda_{it} + y_{it} } \right) - \ln \Upgamma \left( {y_{it} + 1} \right)} \right\}_{{}} } } \right] \hfill \\ \end{gathered} $$
(3)

Following these equations, our final model is as follows:

$$ \begin{gathered} Y_{ij} = \alpha_{i} + \beta_{1i} (Ceasefire) + \beta_{2i} (period) + \beta_{3i} (impact) + \beta_{4i} (trend) + \beta_{5i} (trend2) + \beta_{6i} (quarter2) \hfill \\ + \beta_{7i} (quarter3) + \beta_{8i} (quarter4) + \beta_{9i} (iptw) \hfill \\ \end{gathered} $$
(4)

where the quarterly counts of total gang-involved shooting incidents over the 5-year study time period was our primary outcome measure (Y ij ). However, in addition to our simple effect size analyses, we also analyzed changes in the quarterly counts of victim gang-involved shootings and the quarterly counts of suspect gang-involved shootings. To estimate the effect of the Ceasefire treatment, we created a dichotomous dummy variables indicating whether a street gang was in the treatment group (1) or in the comparison group (0) (Ceasefire) and whether the quarter was pre-intervention (0) or during the intervention period (1) (period). We then created a differences-in-differences (DID) estimator by inter-acting these two dummy variables (impact).

To account for secular linear and nonlinear quarterly trends in the dependent variable, we included a variable that was measured as the simple linear additive progression for each quarter over the course of the 5-year observation period (trend) and a variable that squared this simple linear additive progression for each quarter (trend2). We also controlled for seasonal variations in the quarterly counts of shootings by including a polychotomous dummy variable (quarter2, quarter3, and quarter4).Footnote 10 We estimated the growth curve regression models with the inverse-weighted propensity score value (1/p) for each of the treatment and comparison gangs (represented in the above equation by iptw). The inclusion of this covariate controlled for observable differences between the gangs in the treatment and comparison groups given the covariates used to calculate the propensity score (Imbens and Wooldredge 2009).

The XTNBREG command in Stata 12.1 statistical software was used to calculate the maximum likelihood estimate of the parameters for the DID estimator and to compute the associated probability values; this provided estimates of the effects of the Ceasefire intervention on the treatment gangs as relative to the comparison gangs. The parameter estimates were expressed as incidence rate ratios (i.e., exponentiated coefficients). Incidence rate ratios are interpreted as the rate at which things occur; for example, an incidence rate ratio of 0.90 would suggest that, controlling for other independent variables, a one unit increase in the selected independent variable was associated with a 10 % decrease in the rate at which the dependent variable occurs. Following social science convention, the two-tailed 0.05 level of significance was selected as the benchmark to reject the null hypothesis of “no difference.”

Results

Simple Pre-Post Analysis of Matched Ceasefire Gangs and Matched Comparison Gangs

Figure 2 presents the yearly mean total gang-involved shootings between 2006 and 2010 for the 16 matched Ceasefire gangs and the 37 matched comparison gangs. During the study time period, the yearly mean total gang-involved shootings per Ceasefire gang decreased by 57.3 % from 9.6 shootings in 2006 to 4.1 shootings in 2010. In contrast, the yearly mean total gang-involved shootings per comparison gang decreased by only 20.2 % from 8.4 shootings in 2006 to 6.7 shootings in 2010. Consistent with the trends in yearly mean total gang-involved shootings, the Ceasefire gangs experienced larger decreases in both yearly mean suspect and victim gang-involved shootings relative to the comparison gangs. Between 2006 and 2010, yearly mean suspect gang-involved shootings per Ceasefire gang decreased by 60.7 % (from 5.6 to 2.2) and yearly mean victim gang-involved shootings per Ceasefire gang decreased by 52.5 % (from 4.0 to 1.9); in contrast, yearly mean suspect gang-involved shootings per comparison gang decreased by 23.3 % (from 4.3 to 3.3) and yearly mean victim gang-involved shootings per comparison gang decreased by 17.1 % (from 4.1 to 3.4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean gang-involved shootings for matched ceasefire gangs and matched comparison gangs

Standardized mean difference effect size statistics were used to determine whether the shooting reductions observed for the treated Ceasefire gangs were significantly larger than the shooting reductions observed for the comparison gangs. The standardized mean-difference effect size (d) is designed for contrasting two groups on a continuous dependent variable (Lipsey and Wilson 2001). For this simple analysis, we calculated the mean Time 2 (year 2010) minus Time 1 (year 2006) gain score, the SD of the gain score, and the correlation between the Time 1 and Time 2 scores for the matched 16 Ceasefire gangs and the 37 matched comparison gangs. These statistics were entered into David B. Wilson’s Practical Meta-Analysis Effect Size Calculator to estimate the standard mean difference effect sizes.Footnote 11 For total gang-involved shootings, the Ceasefire intervention was associated with a large, statistically-significant standardized mean difference effect size favoring treatment conditions over control conditions (d = −0.7678; 95 % CI = −1.4221, −0.1136; v = 0.1114). For suspect gang-involved shootings, the Ceasefire intervention was associated with a larger statistically-significant standardized mean difference effect size favoring treatment conditions over control conditions (d = −0.869; 95 % CI = −1.6022, −0.1358; v = 0.1339). While the statistic suggested a beneficial impact on victim gang-involved shootings, the standardized mean difference effect size was modest and not statistically significant (d = −0.4799; 95 % CI = −1.1807, 0.2209; v = 0.1278).

Growth Curve Regression Model and Sensitivity Analysis Results

Table 2 presents the results of the growth curve regression models. Controlling for the other covariates, the Ceasefire intervention was associated with a statistically-significant 30.8 % reduction (p < 0.05) in quarterly total gang-involved shootings, a statistically-significant 34.7 % reduction (p < 0.05) in quarterly suspect gang-involved shootings, and a statistically-significant 26.9 % reduction (p < 0.05) in quarterly victim gang-involved shootings for the treatment gangs relative to the comparison gangs. The Ceasefire dummy variable was not statistically significant (p < 0.05) for all three outcome variables, confirming that the matched groups were comparable on the gun violence outcome measures controlling for the other covariates. For all three outcome variables, the growth curve regression models revealed that Boston gang-involved shootings had statistically-significant seasonal variations; relative to January through March quarterly gang-involved shooting counts (Quarter 1), April through June (Quarter 2) and July through September (Quarter 3) experienced higher counts of gang-involved shootings (p < 0.01). As expected, the inverse propensity score had a statistically-significant negative association with the three gang-involved shooting outcome variables (p < 0.01).Footnote 12 This suggests that Boston gangs with higher levels of shootings were more likely to be included in the quasi-experimental analysis.

Table 2 Ceasefire impacts on gang-involved shooting incidents: growth curve regression models

While the propensity score matching process ensures balance on observed confounders, unobserved variables could simultaneously affect assignment into treatment and the outcome (Rosenbaum 2002). This hidden bias would alter our inferences about Ceasefire treatment effects. For instance, our propensity score model did not include information about the organizational structure of Boston gangs. A recent study by Decker et al. (2008) demonstrates that even modest increases in organizational structure are correlated with increases in patterns of victimization and offending. To examine the robustness of our results against possible hidden bias, we used the bounding approach proposed by Rosenbaum (2002) via the RBOUNDS user-written routine in Stata 12.1 (DiPrete and Gangl 2004). The Rosenbaum bounds techniques allows researchers to determine how strongly an unobserved variable must influence the selection process to alter inference about treatment effects. No hidden bias is represented when bound estimate Γ = 1. Underestimated or overestimated treatment effects that may be due to unobserved confounding are represented by Γ bound estimates higher than 1. A scenario of Γ = 1.50 suggests that hidden bias would increase the odds of receiving Ceasefire treatment for gangs actually receiving Ceasefire treatment by 50 % relative to gangs that did not receive Ceasefire treatment.

Table 3 presents the results of our Rosenbaum bounds sensitivity analysis. Given the direction of the estimated Ceasefire treatment effect, our analysis focused on negative self-selection of gangs into the treatment. Positive self-selection of gangs would simply cause our findings to be conservative. The p-critical values represent the bound of the significance level of the treatment effect in the case of endogenous selection into treatment status (DiPrete and Gangl 2004). The results show that the critical level of Γ at which the estimated Ceasefire treatment effect would no longer be statistically significant at the 5 % level is 1.45 for total gang shootings, 1.55 for suspect gang shootings, and 1.40 for victim gang shootings.Footnote 13 Our conclusion that gun violence involving gangs that received the Ceasefire treatment was significantly lower than gun violence involving gangs that did not receive Ceasefire treatment would be challenged if an unobserved variable increased the odds that Ceasefire gangs received the Ceasefire treatment by 45 % for total shootings, by 55 % for suspect shootings, and by 40 % for victim shootings.

Table 3 Sensitivity analysis: Rosenbaum bounds for Ceasefire treatment effect

Table 3 also presents the magnitude of the hidden bias that would cause us to revise our findings of the causal effects of Ceasefire on gang shootings. Hidden bias equivalents were calculated at the mean of the covariates for 2010 gang shootings. For total gang shootings in 2010, the critical level of Γ = 1.45 is attained at a difference of 4.57 shootings per gang. The Ceasefire average treatment effect on treated (ATT) is −5.21 (SE = 2.41, p < 0.05) for 2010 total gang shootings.Footnote 14 The unobserved variable would have to produce a difference of similar magnitude to the Ceasefire treatment effect in order to alter our conclusions. While these results convey important information about the level of uncertainty contained in matching estimators by showing how large a confounding variable must be to undermine the conclusions of our matching analysis, it is important to note that Rosenbaum bounds represent a “worst case” scenario (DiPrete and Gangl 2004). As such, these analyses suggest that our propensity score matching estimators are robust to hidden bias caused by an unobserved confounder.

Supplementary Analysis of the Timing of Treatment and Observed Reductions in Gang Shootings

We selected January 2007 as the start date of the reinvigorated Operation Ceasefire strategy because it represented the first full month of a regime change in the BPD that delivered a fully-implemented program. Given the complex and intensive work required to implement a focused deterrence intervention on an individual gang, it was simply not possible for the Ceasefire Working Group to address the persistent violent behavior generated by all treated gangs at the same point in time. The Ceasefire intervention was applied to 9 gangs in 2007, 6 gangs in 2008, and 1 gang in 2009. As such, the actual delivery of the intervention to treated gangs occurred in a staggered manner during the post-intervention time period. The overall dosage of Ceasefire intervention to Boston gangs increased during the post-intervention period as suggested by the linear decrease in yearly total shootings by treated gangs in Fig. 2.

To make a direct link between the application of the Ceasefire treatment and subsequent changes in violent gang behavior, we conducted an exploratory analysis to identify abrupt statistically-significant reductions, known as structural breakpoints, in quarterly total gang-involved shootings for each of the 16 matched Ceasefire gangs. Using the NBREG command in Stata 12.1, we ran a series of 18 negative binomial regressions for each Ceasefire gang with a varying quarterly intervention point between Quarter 2 and Quarter 19 that included controls for secular trends and quarterly seasonal variations. Dummy variables (0 = pre-intervention, 1 = intervention) were used to estimate the adjusted pre-post mean difference in total shootings by and against each Ceasefire gang for each of the 18 quarters between Quarter 2 and Quarter 19.Footnote 15 A sharp and sustained break in the quarterly shooting time series will lead to significant before and after differences for several time periods around the intervention. This is because these structural breakpoint analyses involve, in essence, comparisons of two means adjusted for other factors (see Piehl et al. 2003). However, if Ceasefire did produce the desired impact, the maximal structural breakpoint in each time series should coincide with the quarter when treatment was applied or in the quarter immediately following the treatment application.

We reviewed official records maintained by the BPD on Ceasefire actions during the study time period to determine the specific quarter that the treatment was fully implemented. Ceasefire was considered fully implemented for a targeted gang when three components were present: (1) direct communications with the gang had occurred, (2) social services and opportunities were available to gang members who wanted them, and (3) a customized law enforcement response was delivered. We illustrate our structural breakpoint analyses by presenting the details of this exercise for the first gang to receive the full Ceasefire treatment under the new regime.

The Lucerne Street Doggz was the first group selected for Ceasefire intervention because it was the most violent gang in Boston at the beginning of the study time period. The Doggz were a loosely-organized gang based in the disadvantaged Lucerne Street area of the Mattapan section of Boston (District B-3). In 2006, the Lucerne gang had roughly 50 members and was involved in violent disputes with eight rival gangs—Big Head Boys, Morse Street, Norfolk, Greenwood, Heath Street, Orchard Park, H-Block, and Winston Road. Lucerne was the suspect group in 30 gang-involved shootings and the victim group in 7 gang-involved shootings in 2006. BRIC intelligence suggested that most of the Lucerne shootings, which accounted for nearly 10 % of all Boston shootings in 2006, were carried out by no more than 6 or 7 members of the gang.

In late 2006, BPD District B-3 detectives and officers decided to implement a Ceasefire intervention to address the persistent shootings generated by Lucerne. They partnered with the U.S. Attorney’s Office, Suffolk County District Attorney’s Office, Boston School Police, Massachusetts Department of Youth Services, Massachusetts Department of Probation, Boston Ten Point Coalition, Boston Centers for Youth and Families streetworkers, Youth Service Providers Network (social work program) and Youth Opportunities Boston (non-profit employment development agency) on a “call-in” to deliver the Ceasefire anti-violence message. On November 14, 2006, 22 members of the Lucerne Street Doggz attended the call-in; 11 members made appointments with Youth Opportunities Boston to explore job placement options and 7 members requested follow-up meetings with Youth Service Providers Network counselors. Unfortunately, since the BPD was not fully invested in the Ceasefire approach, Lucerne did not face any enhanced enforcement response to their continued violent behavior after the call-in. BPD participation in the Lucerne Street effort was limited to a handful of B-3 detectives and officers; the citywide YVSF and the Drug Control Unit were not involved in this initiative. After a relatively quiet winter period, Lucerne continued its torrid involvement in shootings and, by the end of May 2007, was the suspect group in another 21 gang-involved shootings and the victim group in another 6 gang-involved shootings.

As described earlier, in December 2006, newly-appointed Commissioner Davis mandated that Ceasefire needed to be the BPD’s marquee response to ongoing gang violence. In January 2007, then-Deputy Superintendent Gary French, who was charged by Davis to coordinate the citywide implementation of Ceasefire, started regular meetings of the interagency Operation Ceasefire working group. It was critical to establish the credibility of the Ceasefire anti-violence message on the streets of Boston again. Since Lucerne had been subjected to a call-in and continued on its violent path, the Ceasefire working group needed to make good on the promise that a strong enforcement response would soon follow. With the support of the Drug Control Unit and District B-3 personnel, the YVSF worked with the U.S. Attorney’s Office, Suffolk County District Attorney’s Office, Drug Enforcement Administration and Bureau of Alcohol, Tobacco, Firearms, and Explosives in a focused investigation of the Lucerne Street Doggz. On May 24, 2007, 25 Lucerne Street gang members were taken into custody and charged with federal and state drug and firearms offenses (Ellement 2007). As Fig. 3 reveals, the impact of the Ceasefire intervention on their gun violence behavior was noteworthy. In 2006 and 2007, Lucerne gang averaged 33.5 total shootings per year. Their yearly average plummeted by 87.2 % to 4.3 per year between 2008 and 2010.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Total shootings involving Lucerne Street Doggz, 2006–2010

Table 4 presents a summary assessment of the timing of Ceasefire interventions and maximum quarterly total shooting reductions for the 16 matched treatment gangs. Since this was an exploratory analysis of only 20 quarterly observations for each gang, we relaxed our benchmark to reject the null hypothesis of “no difference” to the less restrictive p < 0.10 level. The key components of Ceasefire intervention on the Lucerne Street Doggz—direct communications with the gang, offers of services and opportunities, and the delivery of an enhanced enforcement response—were in place in Quarter 6 (April–June 2007). The table shows that the maximum statistically-significant reduction (p < 0.05) in the quarterly counts of total shootings for Lucerne occurred in Quarter 7 (Jul–Sep 07) of the time series.

Table 4 The timing of ceasefire interventions and maximum shooting reductions for 16 matched treatment gangs

As Table 4 reveals, 13 of the 16 matched treatment gangs experienced their largest statistically-significant reduction in total shootings in the same quarter as or the quarter immediately following the full implementation of Ceasefire. To test whether this distribution of “successes” relative to “failures” was significantly different than what would be expected by chance, we used an application of the binomial distribution known as the sign test (Blalock 1979). This test examines the probabilities of getting an observed proportion of successes from a population of equal proportions of successes and failures. The observed distribution binomial sign test proportion = 0.8125 (13/16) with a two-tailed p = 0.0213. This suggests that the observed relationship between the implementation of Ceasefire and the timing of the largest statistically-significant reductions was not generated by a random process. In other words, Ceasefire generated noteworthy changes in the gun violence behaviors of targeted gangs during the post-intervention time period.

Conclusions

There is a growing body of evidence that focused deterrence strategies, such as the pulling levers approach pioneered by Operation Ceasefire in Boston, generate significant crime reduction benefits. A recently completed Campbell Collaboration review of 11 controlled evaluations found that focused deterrence strategies were associated an overall statistically significant, medium-sized crime reduction effect (Braga and Weisburd 2012). This review considered replications of the Boston Ceasefire program in five other jurisdictions, including Cincinnati (Engel et al. 2011), Indianapolis (Corsaro and McGarrell 2009; McGarrell et al. 2006), and Los Angeles (Tita et al. 2004). Indeed, the available scientific evidence suggests that cities suffering from gang and criminally-active group violence should experiment with pulling levers focused deterrence strategies.

Our quasi-experimental evaluation estimated that the reconstituted Boston Ceasefire intervention generated a 31 % reduction in total shootings for treated gangs relative to total shootings for matched comparison gangs. Relative to matched comparison gangs, matched treatment gangs committed significantly fewer shootings and experienced significantly lower levels of violent gun victimization. However, it is important note that this evaluation yielded a much more conservative violence reduction estimate when compared to the two-thirds reductions in youth homicides reported in the original Ceasefire quasi-experimental evaluation (Braga et al. 2001; Piehl et al. 2003). While the biases in quasi-experimental research are not clear (e.g. Campbell and Boruch 1975; Wilkinson and Task Force on Statistical Inference 1999), recent reviews in crime and justice suggest that weaker research designs often lead to more positive outcomes (e.g. see Weisburd et al. 2001; Welsh et al. 2011).Footnote 16

Importantly, this study also provides some much needed evidence to address some of the well-thought out concerns over the original Boston Ceasefire evaluation raised by the National Academies’ Panel on Improving Information and Data on Firearms (Wellford et al. 2005) and by Professor Zimring. Our analyses showed that Boston gangs subjected to the post-2007 Ceasefire treatment did indeed change their gun violence behaviors relative to Boston gangs that did not receive Ceasefire treatment. Our study also represents an important advance over other focused deterrence evaluations that examined aggregate citywide changes in group behavior. In Indianapolis (Corsaro and McGarrell 2009) and Cincinnati (Engel et al. 2011), evaluators compared citywide gang and criminally-active group homicide trends, respectively, to citywide non-gang and non-criminally-active group homicide trends, respectively. These evaluations did not distinguish post-intervention homicide trends for treated groups relative to post-intervention homicide trends for untreated groups.

Some readers may wonder whether this evaluation can comment on the “true” impact of Ceasefire on serious gun violence in Boston. Indeed, this evaluation focused on addressing a key question posed by the National Academies’ Panel and Zimring—whether treated Ceasefire gangs actually changed their violent behavior. Kennedy (1997), however, suggests that the Ceasefire focused deterrence strategy was intentionally designed to deter the violent behavior of gangs not directly exposed to the intervention. In essence, our statistical models estimated the effect of treatment on the “directly” treated gangs but not on the “indirectly” treated gangs. A full accounting of Ceasefire violence reduction effects in Boston would also examine these second-order impacts. An important avenue of future research would be to determine whether focused deterrence strategies created “spillover” violence reduction effects onto other gangs and neighborhoods. Indeed, building upon this study, we are pursuing analyses to examine whether untreated gangs changed their gun violence behaviors after their rivals and/or allies were subjected to the Ceasefire intervention.

The available research on Ceasefire and its replications has thus far provided scant empirical evidence on the ways individuals nested within targeted groups and social networks may change their criminal decision making processes. The Ceasefire mechanism of putting gangs “on notice” is designed to increase the certainty of punishment for the group as a whole, but it does so through (a) the diffusion of the message among individual group members and (b) reliance on the group members, as a collective, to modify behavior accordingly. Unfortunately, our study was not able to analyze data on individual behavior. However, the next generation of research on focused deterrence strategies should take advantage of an important opportunity to understand how changing the certainty of punishment for group-level criminal activity may affect individual as well as group behavior. A recent study by Loughran et al. (2011b) offers evidence of a “tipping” effect, whereby perceived risk deters only when it reaches a certain threshold, and a substantially accelerated deterrent effect for individuals at the high end of the risk continuum (Loughran et al. 2011b). Yet, another study found diminishing ambiguity of certainty had no observed deterrent effect for crimes involving contact between offender and victim (Loughran et al. 2011a). It is possible that Ceasefire’s unambiguous face-to-face meetings with gang members, coupled with demonstrated increases in the swiftness, certainty, and severity of punishment for gun violence, exceeds the threshold for a tipping effect and substantially accelerates the deterrent effect of the intervention for high-risk gang members. Or, it is possible that some such tipping points operate in unexplored ways when collectivities such as gangs are involved. Future research on Ceasefire-like interventions would do well to consider how individual decision making processes operate in the context of group accountability and interventions.

While determining whether a program generates the desired outcomes remains an important task, we strongly believe that the next wave of research on focused deterrence strategies needs to understand why these strategies seem to work and how these strategies can be sustained over time. A growing number of scholars suggest that that there seems to be additional crime control mechanisms at work in these strategies beyond straight-up deterrence (Braga 2012; Corsaro et al. 2012; Papachristos et al. 2007). Other prevention frameworks, such as community social control and procedural fairness, might help explain the observed impacts of focused deterrence programs on crime. There is also a growing body of literature suggesting that it is very difficult in practice to sustain these initiatives over an extended time period. Beyond the cessation of Ceasefire in Boston noted earlier, replication programs in Baltimore and Minneapolis unraveled rapidly after some encouraging initial crime control success stories (see Kennedy 2011). The Cincinnati Initiative to Reduce Violence, however, has been able to institutionalize and sustain its focused deterrence interventions through the establishment of a comprehensive organizational structure and a governing board (Engel et al. 2011). Clearly, jurisdictions interested in implementing focused deterrence strategies need to understand how to keep these programs on track for the long-term.