Psychopathy has become important in the identification of a more serious and persistent subgroup of antisocial youth. Juveniles with high psychopathic traits begin their criminal activities earlier in life, commit more serious crimes, have higher predisposition to use weapons, and their victims sustain greater injury (Frick et al. 2003; Murrie et al. 2004; Pechorro et al. 2014a; Salekin et al. 2004; Vitacco et al. 2007). Psychopathic traits in offenders are associated with more institutional violence (Brandt et al. 1997), higher rates of recidivism, and a shorter time between release and re-offense compared to offenders without these traits (Gretton et al. 2004; Hildebrand et al. 2004; Leistico et al. 2008; Rice and Harris 2013).

In turn, juvenile psychopathy has gained importance in forensic theory and practice (Häkkänen-Nyholm and Nyholm 2012; Salekin and Lynam 2010; Vitacco and Salekin 2013). Researchers, borrowing from the established nomological network of adult psychopathy, have modified assessment instruments to make them developmentally appropriate for use with youth (e.g., Forth et al. 2003; Frick and Hare 2001). Such efforts provide the possibility of a more vast understanding of the etiology of the disorder and of early identification and treatment (Lindberg 2012; Pardini and Byrd 2013; Salekin 2010).

Clinical assessments of psychopathy such as the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL:YV; Forth et al. 2003) are valid and useful measures of psychopathy, however, they tend to be time-consuming, expensive, and difficult to use (e.g., require intensive training). Additionally, such measures are less applicable among community samples because they require access to institutional file information in order to calculate a total score (Colins et al. 2012). Another suggested limitation of such measures has to do with tautology. That is, antisocial behavior can be considered a consequence, not a core feature, of the psychopathy construct (e.g., Weizmann-Henelius et al. 2010). Alternatively, parent, teacher, and self-report measures, such as the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick and Hare 2001), have proven useful for circumventing such issues associated with clinical measures and avoid the inclusion of items reflecting antisocial behavior. However, the APSD itself has some limitations (e.g., low reliability, response bias; Andershed et al. 2002b; Falkenbach et al. 2003).

The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI; Andershed et al. 2002a), on the other hand, addresses some of the limitations of other self-report psychopathy measures, in addition to those of clinical measures (e.g., Kotler and McMahon 2005; Vaughn and Howard 2005). The YPI was modeled after Cleckley’s (1976) theory and Hare’s (1991, 2003) operationalization of psychopathy and was intended to capture the core personality features of psychopathy: interpersonal (e.g., glib, manipulative) and affective (e.g., callous, shallow affect), and behavioral (e.g., fearless, impulsive) components. However, the authors intentionally avoided items that overlapped with behavioral outcomes of these traits (e.g., criminal behavior; Andershed et al. 2002a, b). The YPI is comprised of 10 subscales that combine into a three-factor structure consistent with recent psychopathy theoretical models (e.g., Cooke and Michie 2001). Each subscale consists of 5 items enabling trait-level assessment and enhancing its reliability. Items were also intended to capture feelings and opinions as competences or neutral traits, rather than obvious deficiencies in order to avoid socially desirable responding.

The psychometric properties of the YPI have been examined among community, clinical, and forensic samples (Andershed et al. 2002a, b, 2007; Cauffman et al. 2009; Dolan and Rennie 2007; Poythress et al. 2006; Salekin et al. 2010; Skeem and Cauffman 2003). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses have shown that the YPI subscales form the expected three-factor structure conceptualization of the psychopathy construct (i.e., Grandiose-Manipulative, Callous-Unemotional, and Impulsive-Irresponsible dimensions; Andershed et al. 2002a, b, 2007; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010). The internal consistencies measured by Cronbach’s alpha of the three YPI dimensions and the total scores have generally been good to very good: the YPI total ranging from 0.87 to 0.92, the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension from 0.82 to 0.90, the Callous-Unemotional dimension from 0.74 to 0.81, and the Impulsive-Irresponsible dimension from 0.68 to 0.85 (Andershed et al. 2002a, b, 2007; Larsson et al. 2006; Skeem and Cauffman 2003).

The YPI has demonstrated that it can identify a severe and aggressive subgroup of antisocial adolescents (e.g., Andershed et al. 2002a, b; Dolan and Rennie 2006a) and is associated with criminal or antisocial behavior (e.g., Declercq et al. 2009; Dolan and Rennie 2006a, b, 2007; Larsson et al. 2006; Poythress et al. 2006; Salekin et al. 2010; Skeem and Cauffman 2003) and drug and alcohol use (e.g., Hillege et al. 2010; Poythress et al. 2006). The validity of the YPI has further been demonstrated by significant moderate to moderate-high correlations in samples of boys and girls with the PCL: YV (r = 0.29–0.51; Andershed et al. 2007; Dolan and Rennie 2006b; Chauhan et al. 2012; Skeem and Cauffman 2003) and even higher with the APSD (r = 0.76–0.77; Seals et al. 2012; Poythress et al. 2006).

A shortened version of the YPI was recently developed in order to reduce administration time. The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory – Short version (YPI-S; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010) is a brief 18-item version of the YPI developed through a stepwise selection process using a series of exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, and content related arguments. It is organized in three factors, without the subscales composing the original YPI that are supposed to capture the same interpersonal, affective, and behavioral traits of the psychopathy construct. According to Colins et al. (2012), the YPI-S, by not including subscales, may have solved a concern that relates to an inappropriate use of parceling (using subscale scores rather than raw items) in factor analyses, namely not justifying the assumption of unidimensionality.

The only two YPI-S studies published to date (Colins et al. 2012; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010), both use participants from community samples, revealed the expected three-factor structure, namely: the Grandiose-Manipulative (Interpersonal), the Callous-Unemotional (Affective), and the Impulsive-Irresponsible (Behavioral) dimensions. However, Colins et al. (2012) found that one item (i.e., “I have probably skipped school or work more than most other people”) showed a low standardized loading of 0.11, but decided to retain it in order to test whether the original YPI-S factor structure could be replicated. The internal consistencies measured by Cronbach’s alpha of the YPI-S total and its three dimensions were generally modest to good: the YPI-S total ranging from 0.78 to 0.85, the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension from 0.76 to 0.81, the Callous-Unemotional dimension from 0.66 to 0.75, and the Impulsive-Irresponsible from 0.66 to 0.68.

The YPI-S showed high convergence with the original YPI, and the two shared similar correlations to external criterion measures (Van Baardewijk et al. 2010). Additionally, youth with high scores on all three dimensions showed more conduct problems and committed more offenses than those with low scores (Colins et al. 2012). Positive and statistically significant associations with externalizing problems, conduct problems, peers problems, and diverse types of offending were also found. Unfortunately, the psychometric properties of the YPI-S have yet to be examined using clinic-referred or forensic (i.e., justice involved juveniles) samples, and there is still no data on the convergent validity of the YPI-S with other psychopathy measures (e.g., APSD), on the concurrent validity with psychiatric diagnostics (e.g., DSM-5’s Conduct Disorder), or on criterion-related criminal variables (e.g., crime seriousness).

The literature regarding juvenile psychopathy has largely centered on the debate about the utility and stability of the North American construct of psychopathy, overlooking measurement and construct validity among non-North-American subjects (Dolan and Rennie 2006a, b). Because the expression of personality characteristics could vary across ethnic groups, it is crucial to examine the cross-ethnic generalizability of the YPI (Cooke et al. 2004; Veen et al. 2011). The main aim of the present study is to examine the psychometric properties of the YPI and of the YPI-S among a forensic sample of incarcerated Portuguese juvenile delinquents. To our knowledge this is the first study attempting to investigate the psychometric properties of both the YPI and of the YPI-S among incarcerated juvenile delinquents. Consistent with prior research we hypothesized that a tridimensional factor structure would be identified for both instruments among the current forensic sample. Next, several external criteria were included in the current study to test the convergent and discriminant validity of the YPI and YPI-S. Based on prior research, it was hypothesized that both measures would show convergent validity with existing measures of psychopathy (e.g., APSD) and discriminant validity with a social anxiety measure. It was also expected that the YPI and the YPI-S scores would be significantly associated with conduct disorder, age of crime onset, increased crime severity, use of physical violence, alcohol abuse, drug use, and risky sexual behavior.

Method

Participants

Male inmates from the eight nation-wide juvenile detention centers managed by the Portuguese Ministry of Justice voluntarily agreed to participate in the current study (N = 221). They were all detained by the court’s decision. Incarceration into juvenile detention centers is the hardest measure a Portuguese court can decide. Seven of the detention centers are considered low to medium security, and one is considered maximum security (exclusively used for youths tried as adults).

The participants (mean age = 16.75 years; SD = 1.41 years; age range = 13–20 years) were white Europeans (54.3 %), black Africans (20.5 %), mixed race South-Americans (18.6 %), and members of other ethnic minorities (6.8 %). Most of the participants came from an urban background (92.8 %). Their first criminal problems had been early in their lives (mean = 11.33 years, SD = 2.24 years), most were detained before they were 16 years old (mean = 15.46 years, SD = 1.31 years), and had been convicted to an average of 21 months in detention (mean = 20.67 months, SD = 6.69 months). Most of them (87.6 %) were convicted of having committed serious and/or violent crimes (e.g., homicide, robbery, assault, rape).

Measures

The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI; Andershed et al. 2002a, b) is a 50-item self-report measure designed to assess the core personality traits of the psychopathic personality constellation in youth aged 12 years old and up. Each item is scored on an ordinal 4-point Likert scale ranging from “Does not apply at all” to “Applies very well”. The YPI consists of 10 subscales (with 5 items each) designed in line with Cooke and Michie’s (2001) three-dimensional conceptualization of the psychopathy construct, namely: the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension, the Callous-Unemotional dimension, and the Impulsive-Irresponsible dimension. More specifically, the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension consists of the Dishonest charm, Grandiosity, Lying, and Manipulation subscales; the Callous-Unemotional dimension consists of the Callousness, Unemotionality, and Remorselessness subscales; the Impulsive-Irresponsible dimension consists of the Impulsivity, Thrill-seeking, and Irresponsibility subscales. Higher scores reflect an increased presence of the characteristics associated, namely psychopathic traits.

The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory-Short (YPI-S; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010) is an 18-item self-report shorter version of the original YPI (Andershed et al. 2002a, b) designed to measure psychopathic-like traits in adolescents. All the items of the YPI-S came from the original YPI. Stepwise parallel reduction using principal components analysis and content-related arguments were used to develop the YPI-S. A three-factor structure almost identical to the factor structure of the YPI (see above) was demonstrated in boys and girls. The YPI-S showed strong convergence with the original YPI and similar correlations to external criterion. Each item in the YPI-S is scored on a 4 point Likert scale ranging from “Does not apply at all” to “Applies very well”. The YPI can be scored by simply adding the items. Higher scores indicate an increased presence of the characteristics associated.

The APSD-Self-Report (APSD-SR; Frick and Hare 2001; Caputo et al. 1999) is a multidimensional 20-item measure designed to assess psychopathic traits in adolescents. It was modeled after the PCL-Revised (PCL-R; Hare 2003). Each item is scored on a 3-point ordinal scale ranging from “Never” to “Often”. The total score, as well as each dimension score, is obtained by adding the respective items. Some studies (e.g., Frick et al. 1994) reported two main factors (Callous-Unemotional and Impulsivity/Conduct problems), while others (e.g., Frick et al. 2000) reported three main factors: Callous-Unemotional, Narcissism and Impulsivity. Higher scores indicate higher psychopathic traits. The Portuguese validation of the APSD-SR (Pechorro et al. 2013) was used. The internal consistency for the current study, estimated by Cronbach’s alpha, was: APSD-SR total = 0.81; APSD-SR Callous-Unemotional dimension = 0.68; APSD-SR Impulsivity dimension = 0.60; APSD-SR Narcissism dimension = 0.74.

The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU; Essau et al. 2006; Kimonis et al. 2008) is a 24-item self-report scale designed to assess callous and unemotional traits in youth derived from the callous-unemotional (CU) subscale of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick and Hare 2001). Each item is scored on a four-point scale ranging from “Not at all true” to “Definitely true”. Scores are calculated by reverse-scoring the positively worded items and then summing the items to obtain a total score. Using confirmatory factor analysis it was possible to identify three independent factors, namely: Callousness, Unemotional, and Uncaring. All items also loaded onto a general callous-unemotional factor. Higher scores indicate an increased presence of CU traits. The Portuguese validation of the ICU (Pechorro et al. 2015) was used. The internal consistency for the current study, estimated by Cronbach’s alpha, was: ICU total = 0.90; ICU Callousness dimension = 0.88; ICU Uncaring dimension = 0.86; ICU Unemotional dimension = 0.87.

The Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca and Lopez 1998) is an 18-item self-report scale designed to assess subjective experience of social anxiety in adolescents. Each item is rated on a five-point scale ranging from “Not at all” to “All the time”. Three distinct subscales have been identified: the Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE) subscale has eight items and reflects fears, concerns, or worries regarding negative evaluations from peers; the Social Avoidance and Distress – New (SAD-New) subscale has six items and reflects social avoidance and distress with new social situations or unfamiliar peers; the Social Avoidance and Distress – General (SAD-General) subscale has four items and reflects more generalized or pervasive social distress, discomfort, and inhibition. Scores are obtained by summing the ratings for the items comprising each subscale. The Portuguese validation of the SAS-S (Pechorro et al. 2014b) was used. Internal consistency for the present study, estimated by Cronbach’s alpha, was: SAS-A total = 0.91; SAS-A FNE dimension = 0.91; SAS-A SAD-New dimension = 0.89; SAS-A SAD-General dimension = 0.75.

The Child and Adolescent Taxon Scale (CATS; Harris et al. 1994; Quinsey et al. 2006) is an actuarial rating scale developed from variables related to childhood and adolescent antisocial and psychopathic characteristics. Harris et al. (1994) established that eight childhood variables could identify members of the antisocial/psychopathic class. These variables include the following: (a) elementary school maladjustment, (b) teen alcohol abuse, (c) childhood aggression, (d) childhood behavior problems, (e) parental alcohol problems, (f) suspended or expelled from school, (g) separated from parents before age 16, and (h) arrested before age 16. The eight variables scored either 0 (No) or 1 (Yes), and summed to form a scale that ranges from 0 to 8. Higher scores indicate an increased presence of the characteristics associated.

The Sellin-Wolfgang Index of Crime Seriousness (ICS; Wolfgang et al., as cited in White et al. 1994) guided the delinquency seriousness classification of the official court reports. Level 0 consists of no delinquency. Level 1 consists of minor delinquency committed at home such as stealing minor amounts of money from mother’s purse. Level 2 consists of minor delinquency outside the home including shoplifting something worth less than 5 euros, vandalism and minor fraud (e.g. not paying bus fare). Level 3 consists of moderately serious delinquency such as any theft over 5 euros, gang fighting, carrying weapons, and joyriding. Level 4 consists of serious delinquency such as car theft and breaking and entering. Level 5 consists of having performed at least two of each of the behaviors in level 4.

In addition, a questionnaire was constructed to describe the socio-demographic and criminal characteristics of the participants, to offer a descriptive account of the sample, and to explore the association of some of these variables (e.g., age of onset) with YPI scores. This questionnaire included variables such as participants’ age, nationality, ethnic group, origin (rural vs. urban), level of schooling completed, age of crime onset, length of the conviction, use of physical violence in committing crimes, alcohol use, cannabis use, cocaine/heroin use, and having unprotected sex (i.e., sex without using condoms). DSM-5’s Conduct Disorder (CD; American Psychiatric Association 2013) was assessed using the official diagnostic criteria (i.e., the standard method described in the DSM-5).

Procedures

Authorization to validate the YPI and the YPI-S among Portuguese youth was obtained from the first author of the inventory (Andershed et al. 2002a, b). Appropriate procedures (e.g., avoiding item bias or differential item functioning) were followed during the translation and retroversion (Hambleton et al. 2005). The initial translation from English into Portuguese was completed by the first and last authors of this article, who made sure that young people would be able to properly understand the meaning of the items. The questionnaire was then independently translated back into English by a native English speaker with considerable professional experience in translating psychology-related scientific texts. No significant differences were found between the back-translation and the original version, demonstrating that the translated items had the same or very similar meanings as the original English items.

Authorization to assess youths was obtained from the General Directorate of Reintegration and Prison Services – Ministry of Justice (Direção-Geral de Reinserção e Serviços Prisionais – Ministério da Justiça). The detainees, from the eight existing Portuguese Juvenile Detention Centers that admit male youths, were informed about the nature of the study and asked to voluntarily participate. The participation rate was approximately 92 %. Not all young people agreed or were able to participate; reasons for this included refusal to participate (5 %), inability to participate due to not understanding the Portuguese language (2 %) and inability to participate due to security issues (1 %). The measures were administered by means of individual face-to-face interviews in an appropriate setting. It was stressed that there were no right or wrong answers and that for each item the youth should consider what he generally thinks or feels. Institutional files were also used to complement the information obtained (e.g., prior criminal activity and detentions, psychiatric diagnosis). Some of the information (e.g., socio-demographic variables) was obtained from self-reports. The first author made the diagnosis of Conduct Disorder (American Psychiatric Association 2013), after interviewing each youth and taking into consideration the institutional files (which also included the official psychiatric and psychological assessments of each youth).

Data-Analytical Strategy

The data was analyzed using SPSS v22 (IBM SPSS, 2013) and EQS 6.2 (Bentler and Wu 2008). The factor structure of the Portuguese language version of the YPI was assessed with Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) performed in EQS 6.2 (Bentler and Wu 2008; Byrne 2006), with the robust estimation methods. Goodness of fit indices were calculated, including chi-square/degrees of freedom or Satorra-Bentler chi-square/degrees of freedom, comparative fit index (CFI), incremental fit index (IFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). A chi-square/degrees of freedom value ≤ 5 is considered acceptable, a value ≤ 2 is considered good, and = 1 very good (Maroco 2014; West et al. 2012). A CFI ≥ 0.90 and RMSEA ≤ 0.10 indicate adequate fit, whereas a CFI ≥ 0.95 and RMSEA ≤ 0.06 indicate good model fit (Byrne 2006; West et al. 2012). The incremental fit index, also known as Bollen’s IFI, is relatively insensitive to sample size; values that exceed 0.90 are regarded as acceptable and above 0.95 are regarded as good (Maroco 2014).

Regarding the YPI, the CFA was performed on the subscale scores, not on the items per se, the same methodology used by Andershed et al. (2002a, b), while for the YPI-S the CFA was performed on the original scale items. The minimum value considered for an adequate loading was a standardized loading above 0.30. Our models were a 1st order factorial models and covariance between factors were freely estimated. Modification indexes were also considered but did not provide any suggestion of model modification that would significantly improve the measurement model. Polychoric correlations were used to perform the CFAs on the ordinal items of the YPI-S because they provide more accurate estimates for the true correlations between ordinal items than Pearson correlations (Holgado-Tello et al. 2010). Pearson correlations were used to analyze associations between scale variables, Spearman correlations were used with ordinal variables, and point-biserial correlations were used to analyze associations between nominal dichotomous variables and scale variables (Leech et al. 2008). Correlations were considered low if below 0.20, moderate if between 0.20 and 0.50, and high if above 0.50. Mean inter-item correlations were considered good if between 0.15 and 0.50 (Clark and Watson 1995). Corrected item-total correlations were considered satisfactory if above 0.20 (Nunnally and Bernstein 1994). Cronbach’s alphas were considered good if above 0.70 (Cortina 1993).

Results

Our first step in examining the psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the YPI and YPI-S was to attempt to replicate, by means of CFA, the tridimensional factor structures of these instruments. As seen in the Table 1, the three factor structure of both the YPI and the YPI-S fitted the data well.

Table 1 Goodness of fit indexes for different factor models of the YPI and YPI-S using Maximum Likelihood with robust estimation methods

Presented in Table 2 are the subscale loadings of the YPI and item loadings of the YPI-S for the three-factor model drawn for the CFAs. As seen in the table, all loadings were above 0.30 in the intended factor, with the exception of item 5 of the YPI-S, even though this item was still statistically significant.

Table 2 YPI subscale loadings for the confirmatory three-factor structure and YPI-S item loadings for the confirmatory three-factor robust structure

The next step was the estimation of internal consistency reliability statistics in the form of Cronbach’s alpha, mean inter-item correlations and corrected item-total correlation range for the YPI and the YPI-S. The results are presented in Table 3 and shows that the majority of the subscales and factors exhibited good to excellent internal consistency. Only the YPI Callousness subscale, YPI Unemotionality subscale, YPI Remorselessness subscale, YPI Irresponsibility subscale, and the YPI-S Affective dimension were below 0.70. The YPI Callousness subscale and the YPI Unemotionality subscale exhibited Cronbach’s alpha values below 0.60.

Table 3 Cronbach’s Alpha, mean inter-item/subscale correlation, and corrected item/subscale-total correlation range for the YPI and the YPI-S

Presented in Table 4 are the correlations between the YPI-S (total and its dimensions) and the original YPI (total and its dimensions). As seen in the table, these correlations were, as expected, moderate to high.

Table 4 Correlations matrix between YPI-S and YPI factors and totals

Correlations between the factors and total scores of the YPI and YPI-S and convergent validity variables are presented in Table 5. As shown in the table, the YPI total (and its dimensions) and of the YPI-S total (and its dimensions) with the APSD-SR, the ICU, and the CATS revealed moderate to high statistically significant positive correlations, while the discriminant validity with the SAS-A revealed mostly the expected low non-significant correlations. The correlations with DSM-5’s Conduct Disorder diagnostic (coded No = 0, Yes = 1), age of crime onset, crime seriousness, and use of physical violence (coded No = 0, Yes = 1) revealed mostly moderate statistically significant correlations. Regarding DSM-5’s CD diagnostic, a very high prevalence rate of 94.1 % was found in the present sample. The correlations with alcohol use, cannabis use, cocaine/heroin use, and having unprotected sex (coded as five-point ordinal scales) revealed mostly moderate and moderate-low statistically significant correlations; the exception was the unprotected sex (i.e., sex without using condoms) variable which was always non-significant with the YPI-S (see Table 5).

Table 5 Correlations of YPI / YPI-S and its dimensions with other variables

Discussion

The present study aimed to test the psychometric properties of the YPI and the YPI-S among incarcerated male juvenile delinquents. The issue of factor structure is important due to the ongoing debate of how best to define and measure psychopathy. Evidence obtained in our study shows that the three-factor model is the most acceptable one for the Portuguese version of the YPI, whereas a two-factor model did not fit the data well. Other studies (e.g., Andershed et al. 2002a, 2007, b; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010) have found supporting evidences for the three-dimensional nature of psychopathy in youths and it does seems to also apply in the current sample of incarcerated Portuguese adolescents. Regarding the YPI-S, the present study confirms that the three-factor model is applicable. Colins et al. (2012) and Van Baardewijk et al. (2010) have also found supporting evidence for the three-dimensional structure of the YPI-S in community youths, which thus also seems to be the case in the current incarcerated sample. However, one item, namely “I have probably skipped school or work more than most other people”, showed a low standardized loading but was still statistically significant. Because this loading was very close to the minimum value considered an adequate loading, we decided to keep it. Colins et al. (2012) found a similar problem with this item in their community sample, indicating that it may need to be revised.

The Pearson correlations between the YPI total with its dimensions showed mostly moderate to high statistically significant positive associations. The same pattern of associations was observable regarding the original YPI-S and its dimensions, and regarding the correlations between YPI-S (and its dimensions) and the original YPI (and its dimensions). These values were similar to the ones found by other authors (Colins et al. 2012; Van Baardewijk et al. 2010).

Some psychometric problems were found. Analyses of the internal consistency of the YPI revealed somewhat low values especially for the Unemotionality subscale and the Callousness subscale, and also for the Remorselessness subscale and the Irresponsibility subscale. Thus, this questions the reliability of these scales, since only a small portion of the variance is attributable to them (Cortina 1993; Kaplan and Saccuzzo 2009). However, these values are not exclusive to our study (e.g., Chauhan et al. 2012; Declercq et al. 2009) and thus may be more general issues of the YPI rather than linked to the Portuguese translation or sample. Thus, it may well be the case that some items of these subscales and dimensions need to be revised. In particular, the only items of the YPI that are reversed (no reversed items are included in the YPI-S), generally show quite low inter-item correlations, which was also the case in the present sample (shown by complementary analyses – not shown in tables). Thus, future studies should examine whether not reverse coding items may improve the reliability of YPI subscales that underperformed. The YPI total, the three dimensions and rest of the subscales presented good or very good values (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70–0.87). Regarding the YPI-S, the analysis of the internal consistency revealed a somewhat low value (i.e., 0.67) for the Affective dimension questioning the reliability of measurements for this dimension (Cortina 1993; Kaplan and Saccuzzo 2009), but the YPI-S total and the remaining dimensions presented good values. Similar findings have been found in previous studies (e.g., Colins et al. 2012) and are not easy to explain. It is worth pointing out that the Affective dimension of the YPI-S does not include any reversed items.

In terms of the mean inter-item correlations of the YPI, no problems were found because the total scale, the three dimensions and the subscales all reached a minimum 0.15 mean inter-item correlation (Clark and Watson 1995; Domino and Domino 2006). However, the mean inter-item correlations for the Dishonest charm subscale, Manipulation subscale, Grandiose-Manipulative dimension, and Impulsive-Irresponsible dimension were above the maximum recommended value of 0.50, indicating that there might be some excessive homogeneity present. The corrected item/subscale-total correlation range of the YPI total, of the three dimensions and of the subscales were all above the minimum recommended value of 0.20 (Kaplan and Saccuzzo 2009; Nunnally and Bernstein 1994). Regarding the YPI-S, the mean inter-item correlations revealed no problems. That is, YPI-S total and its three dimensions were all within the recommended value range of 0.15–0.50 (Clark and Watson 1995; Domino and Domino 2006), showing adequate homogeneity between the items. In terms of the corrected item-total correlation range, the three dimensions of the YPI-S were all above the minimum recommended value of 0.20 (Kaplan and Saccuzzo 2009; Nunnally and Bernstein 1994), but the YPI-S total did not reach that value although it was very close.

The convergent validity of the YPI and its dimensions with the APSD-SR, the ICU and the CATS revealed mostly moderate-high statistically significant correlations demonstrating the expected overlap in line with previous studies (e.g., Seals et al. 2012; Poythress et al. 2006), while the discriminant validity with the SAS-A revealed mostly the expected null correlations (American Psychological Association 1999; Kaplan and Saccuzzo 2009). The concurrent validity of the YPI and its dimensions with DSM’s Conduct Disorder (American Psychiatric Association 2013) showed moderate associations that were somewhat lower than the typical correlation range provided by youth psychopathy instruments (e.g., Forth et al. 2003; Pechorro et al. 2013) but still statistically significant; the exception was the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension with a lower correlation. Regarding the YPI-S, basically the same pattern of correlations emerged, but these were somewhat lower. The very high prevalence rate of conduct disorder in the current sample (94.1 %) was higher than what is typical of some forensic samples (Sevecke and Kosson 2010).

The correlations of the YPI and its dimensions with the criminal variables revealed mostly the existence of moderate-low negative associations with age of crime onset, and the existence of moderate-low positive associations with crime seriousness and the use of physical violence when committing crimes, with the exceptions of the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension which revealed mostly non-significant correlations. Regarding the YPI-S, basically the same pattern of correlations emerged, but these were somewhat lower. Negative associations between psychopathy scores and the age of crime onset have been consistently reported in the literature (e.g., Forth et al. 2003; Pechorro et al. 2014a, b), although the one we obtained was somewhat lower but still statistically significant. Positive associations between psychopathy scores and antisocial and criminal behavior variables similar to the ones we obtained have been consistently reported in the literature (e.g., Dolan and Rennie 2006a, b, 2007; Larsson et al. 2006; Poythress et al. 2006; Salekin et al. 2010; Skeem and Cauffman 2003).

As expected, the positive correlations of the YPI (and its dimensions) and of the YPI-S (and its dimensions) with alcohol use, cannabis use, and cocaine/heroin were mostly moderate to low (e.g., Colins et al. 2012); the exception was the Affective dimension of the YPI-S which revealed a non-significant association with cocaine/heroin use. Statistically significant associations were found regarding the unprotected sex variable, although smaller in strength than previously reported (e.g., Rucevic 2010). The exceptions were the Grandiose-Manipulative dimension and the Callous-Unemotional dimension of the YPI which revealed non-significant correlations, while regarding the YPI-S no significant associations were found. Overall, the YPI-S presented somewhat weaker relations to external correlates, but less problems with reliability.

Our findings provide some additional support for the extension of the psychopathy construct to adolescents and its potential generalization across different cultures and ethnic groups. We were able to demonstrate some appropriate psychometric properties that justify the future use of the YPI and the YPI-S. However, some caution is advised since the Portuguese validation of these important instruments is still ongoing. Further psychometric procedures are needed and should be done in the near future (e.g., cross-validation using other samples, discriminant validity, test-retest reliability, known-groups validity). Another serious limitation was the relatively small sample size, which is an important issue given that CFA was used (both Type I and II errors are much more likely with small samples, and this concern is elevated when the data are skewed).

To our knowledge this is the first study attempting to investigate the psychometric properties of both the YPI and of the YPI-S among incarcerated male juvenile delinquents. Considering the enormous costs that persons with psychopathic traits create in terms of their crimes and collateral effects on victims and society as a whole, we hope that our study may guide future research/use of these instruments with Portuguese youth, promote research of the psychopathy construct in southern European countries, contribute to the betterment of treatment programs of serious and violent juvenile offenders in Portugal, and promote future research and a more generalized use of the YPI-S. In conclusion, the present study generally lends support to the criterion, external, and divergent validity of both the YPI and YPI-S among incarcerated males. The correlations to external validity correlates of the YPI are however generally somewhat stronger than those of the YPI-S. Another important conclusion is that the affective part of both the YPI and YPI-S exhibit relatively low internal consistency reliability underlining the need for further development of the YPI and YPI-S.