It is well-known that individuals with autism have difficulties perceiving and recognizing faces, including deficits in the categorization of facial gender (e.g., Behrmann et al. 2006; Best et al. 2010), discrimination of facial expressions, (e.g., Celani et al. 1999; Rump et al. 2009), and face recognition (e.g., Klin et al. 1999; Lahaie et al. 2006; Newell et al. 2010). Traditional explanations for these deficits have suggested that individuals with autism focus more on discrete facial features rather than processing configural information and perceiving faces in a holistic manner (for review see Dawson et al. 2005). In particular, it has been found that individuals with autism are less affected by the face inversion effect than typically developing individuals (e.g., Boucher and Lewis 1992; Klin et al. 1999). Since the viewing of inverted faces disrupts configural and holistic processes, it has been argued that individuals with autism rely more on featural processing.

Researchers have traditionally used bottom-up, perceptual explanations to account for face processing difficulties, arguing that individuals with autism are biased toward processing local features and are less likely to perceive global patterns (Frith and Happé 1994; Mottron et al. 2006). These explanations suggest that the difficulties that arise in face processing are caused by underlying differences in how the perceptual aspects of faces are processed. Although differences in these bottom-up processes may exist, it is also clear that the development of expertise in face processing requires extensive learning that does not reach full maturity until adolescence or adulthood in typically developing individuals (e.g., Rump et al. 2009; Scherf et al. 2009). With development, children encounter an increasing number of people in their environment and learn about faces and how facial dimensions vary. Therefore, it is critical to consider the impact that top-down processes may have on the face processing abilities of both typically developing individuals and individuals with autism.

One of the most useful models for understanding how typically developing individuals organize their developing knowledge of facial information is Valentine’s (1991) multidimensional experience-based framework for representing and storing faces. This framework explains how faces are recognized with top-down processing through the development of an organizational structure that guides face perception. Valentine suggested that exemplars and prototypical information about faces are stored in an n-dimensional “face space” representing all possible features used to encode a face, including both featural and configural information, as well as information used to discriminate faces (e.g., age, gender, and race). The values of the n-dimensions encoded in the face space depend on an individual’s experience with faces. The center of this multi-dimensional framework represents the central tendency of all facial information (prototype), and the distribution of facial features and facial information is normally distributed around this central tendency. For example, in human faces, there are variations in dimensions such as the distance between the eyes, the height of the forehead, the width of the mouth, and so on. The prototype would thus reflect the combined central values across all of these varying dimensions. As an individual gains experience with faces, these faces are represented in the face space framework according to the values of facial information. With experience, the distributions become more refined as more subtle variations are included in the face space, and the central tendencies of facial dimensions become more accurate. When presented with a new face, individuals compare it to their learned prototype and use this information to guide perception, categorization, and recognition.

Valentine’s (1991) face space theory provides a framework for understanding many face perception and recognition effects, including the recognition advantage for distinctive faces and caricatures (e.g., Best and Strauss 2007; Humphreys 2003; Rhodes et al. 1987), the classification advantage of gender-typical faces in gender classification tasks (e.g., O’Toole et al. 1998), and the preference for attractive faces over unattractive faces (e.g., Rubenstein et al. 1999). Thus, the ability to form a face space and a prototype is critical for multiple aspects of face perception and recognition. Studies have demonstrated that within the first year, typically-developing infants can form prototypes of faces (Rubenstein et al. 1999; Strauss 1979), objects (Younger 1990) and dot patterns (Younger and Gotlieb 1988). Evidence of prototype formation in children and adults comes from studies of the prototype effect—the tendency to falsely remember a prototype as previously seen despite never actually seeing it. In a classic study by Posner and Keele (1968), adults trained on dot patterns varying in distortion levels from a prototype tended to falsely remember the unseen prototype and considered it to be as familiar as previously seen dot patterns.

Although there has been relatively little research on prototype formation in autism, a few studies have suggested that individuals with autism are unable to abstract a prototype and do not exhibit the prototype effect. Klinger and Dawson (2001) found that low-functioning children with autism were unable to abstract a prototype of simple animal-like categories, a finding that has recently been replicated with high-functioning children and adults with autism (Klinger et al. 2006; Plaisted 2000). In contrast, Molesworth et al. (2005, 2008) did not find evidence of a lack of prototype formation in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder; however, their results may not reflect intact prototype formation abilities. Aspects of the study design (e.g., the use of obvious features, lacking subtle variation between feature values, the use of the exact same feature values in the familiarization and test phase) may have permitted individuals with autism to show a prototype effect due to memorization of specific features or by focusing on the variations in one feature rather than forming a prototype.

Only one study to date has examined the ability of individuals with autism to form a prototype of faces. Gastgeb et al. (2009) tested high-functioning children and adults with autism and matched controls on a face prototype task using schematic drawings of faces. This task was patterned after a study originally conducted on both adults and 10-month-old infants (Strauss 1979). Results indicated that 78% of the adults in the control group chose the prototype face as more familiar than the face comprised of features that were more frequently seen, while only 55% of the adults with autism chose the prototype. These results add to those of Klinger and colleagues (2001, 2006) suggesting that in addition to having difficulty with forming a prototype of animal-like categories, individuals with autism also have difficulty forming prototypes of facial information.

An inability to form a face space and prototype may contribute to the well-known deficits in face processing and recognition in autism that were discussed earlier. Therefore, research on prototype formation and other top-down processes may impact how interventions to improve face processing and memory are developed. There is growing recognition that interventions aimed at improving the abilities of individuals with autism need to be tailored to the specific deficits associated with autism. Interventions aimed at improving the face processing ability of individuals with autism are beginning to address the specific face processing deficits that appear to be associated with the disorder (e.g., Tanaka et al. 2010). Thus, the extent to which we can better understand these deficits, particularly whether they involve basic perceptual bottom-up processes or higher learning top-down processes, will be critical to the continued development of effective intervention strategies.

The current study aimed to replicate the findings of previous prototype studies using natural faces in a group of high-functioning adults with autism. The only previous study on face prototype formation in autism (Gastgeb et al. 2009) used schematic line drawings of faces rather than natural faces. The current study utilized natural faces in order to more closely replicate the facial information that is abstracted in real life categorization. Participants were familiarized with sets of faces, and after each set, they chose which face was more familiar, the prototype face or a face comprised of features that were previously seen (mode face). If individuals with autism are unable to abstract a prototype of facial information, they should not show a prototype effect and should not choose the prototype faces as more familiar. It is also possible that individuals with autism who perform well on the face prototype task may differ from those who perform poorly on the task. Therefore, the current study also explored the distribution of performance in both groups and the relationship between performance and measures of intelligence or behavioral symptoms of autism in the autism group.

Participants’ eye movements were also recorded in order to gather vital information about which areas of the faces individuals with autism and control individuals looked at when viewing the familiarization stimuli (learning). If there are between group differences in prototype formation ability, one potential explanation for these differences could be that the groups differed in the way in which they distributed their attention to the faces and/or facial features. For example, if the autism group spent less time looking at the faces and more time looking at the background, they would be less likely to form a prototype during the familiarization phase. Similarly, if the autism group spent less time looking at relevant facial features such as the eyes, nose, mouth, and forehead and more time looking at irrelevant features such as the cheeks or hairline, they would be less likely to form a prototype. Finally, if the autism group focused solely on one facial feature, this would affect their ability to form a prototype of the entire face. Therefore, unlike previous prototype formation studies, the current study addressed both if individuals with autism have difficulty forming prototypes or categories in addition to why they may have difficulty.

Method

Participants

Participants consisted of 20 high-functioning, adult males with autism and 20 healthy, control adult males recruited by the Autism Center for Excellence (ACE) at the University of Pittsburgh. Control participants were matched with participants in the autism group on age, full scale IQ (FSIQ), verbal IQ (VIQ), and performance IQ (PIQ). All participants had IQ scores greater than 80 as determined by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler 1999). Table 1 summarizes the participants’ demographic characteristics. No significant differences were found between the two groups on age, FSIQ, VIQ, or PIQ.

Table 1 Participants’ diagnostic and demographic characteristics

Individuals with autism were recruited through informational visits to service providers throughout the state of Pennsylvania and the surrounding states, fliers at autism meetings, advertisements in autism newsletters, and posters. All participants with autism met criteria for autism on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord et al. 1989) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Lord et al. 1994), which was verified by expert clinical opinion. Participants with Asperger’s disorder or Pervasive Developmental Disorder were excluded. Potential participants with autism were excluded if found to have a history of seizures or evidence of an associated neurologic, genetic, infectious, or metabolic disorder. Exclusions were based on physical examination, neurologic history and examination, and chromosomal analysis.

Control participants were volunteers recruited from the community through advertisements. Potential control participants were screened by completing family and personal history questionnaires of medical, neurological, and psychiatric disorders (Adult Symptom Inventory-4, Gadow et al. 1999; Family History Screen, Weissman et al. 2000). Exclusion criteria included a personal history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, learning disability, brain injury prior to or after birth; loss of consciousness; poor school attendance; a medical disorder with implications for the central nervous system or requiring regular medication usage; a family history in first-degree relatives of learning disability, mood disorder, or anxiety disorder; and a family history of autism in first-, second-, or third-degree relatives.

Apparatus

Testing occurred in a quiet, dark laboratory room that simulated a small movie theater and provided maximum comfort. Each participant was seated in a modified desk chair in front of a large rear projection movie screen (69 × 91 cm). The testing area was surrounded by black curtains to reduce distractions. A stand-alone eye-tracker that required no attachments to the participant was positioned on a table in front of the participant. Stimuli were rear projected onto the screen using Tobii Studio software, and eye movements were recorded by a Tobii X120 stand-alone eye tracker at a sampling rate of 60 Hz, accuracy of 0.5 degrees of visual angle, spatial resolution of 0.2 degrees, and drift of 0.3 degrees. The eye-tracker sat 81 cm in front of the projection screen, and the participants were positioned approximately 162 cm from the screen. A Dell Dimension 9200 displayed experimental stimuli and recorded eye-movement and behavioral accuracy data. Responses were recorded by keypad response using a two button Ergodex DX1 input system response pad. Eye-tracking and behavioral data were processed using Tobii Studio software, Version 2.0.6.

Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of six sets of faces (three sets of male faces and three sets of female faces) that were approximately 12 × 19 degrees of visual angle. Each set included 20 stimuli (16 familiarization stimuli and four test stimuli) that were created by manipulating specific features and spatial distances between features of a photograph of a natural face with average features. The features and spatial distances that were manipulated included nose/mouth distance, nose width, forehead height, and lip thickness. The non-manipulated original face was designated as the “Prototype” (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Prototype and mode stimulus examples

Faces were generated by manipulating each of the four facial aspects or distances to either be larger or wider than the original (values 3 and 4) or smaller or narrower than the original (values 1 and 2) using the Face Fun facial morphing program. No face had the same feature values as the original, and the facial aspects were manipulated by equal amounts from one value to the next. For each set of faces, the familiarization stimuli consisted of 16 stimuli and within these stimuli, each value for each facial aspect or distance was seen four times. For example, for nose width, four faces had a nose width value of 1, four had a value of 2, four had a value of 3, and four had a value of 4. Two “Mode” faces, which were comprised of values that were seen an equal number of times during the familiarization trials but were perceptually the most different from the prototype values, were also created for each set of faces. One mode face (Mode 1) was comprised of all of the smallest values of each facial aspect (i.e., all values of 1) and the other mode face (Mode 2) was comprised of all of the largest values (i.e., all values of 4).

Finally, four possible test stimuli were created for each set of faces in which the prototype face (original non-manipulated face) was paired with one of the mode faces. For two test stimuli, the prototype was on the right side of the pair, and for the other two stimuli, the prototype was on the left side of the pair. Table 2 shows example values of the modified facial aspects for all stimuli (familiarization, mode, prototype) and includes a note that defines the test stimuli.

Table 2 Example values of modified facial aspects for face stimuli

Familiarization and test stimuli were programmed into four different presentation orders using Tobii Studio. For each presentation order, the six sets of faces were presented in a different predetermined order in blocks. In each block, the familiarization stimuli for one set of faces were presented in randomized order followed by the test trial. This was repeated for a total of six blocks. Each test stimulus was presented an equal number of times across the four different presentation orders, and the prototype was presented on the left and on the right an equal number of times within each presentation order.

Procedure

Participants were told that they would be viewing a series of faces and asked to answer questions about the faces by making button-press responses. Participants were familiarized with the eye-tracking equipment and seated in front of the eye-tracker and projection screen. During the calibration, participants were required to look at the calibration points on the screen in front of them. The calibration procedure was repeated until it was successful. After the calibration, participants were instructed to look at the faces on the screen but were not given any other instructions.

Participants were then shown the first block of familiarization trials consisting of 16 manipulated face stimuli in a randomized order. Based on prior face prototype research (Gastgeb et al. 2009), the familiarization faces remained on the screen for two seconds with an interstimulus interval of one second in which a plain white screen was presented. At the end of the familiarization period, participants were given a response pad with two buttons. Above the left button was an arrow pointing to the left side of the screen, and above the right button was an arrow pointing to the right side of the screen. Participants were instructed to press the button corresponding to the face that looked most familiar to them. Once the participants were ready, the test trial was presented. This was done to ensure that the participants were attending when the test trials were presented. There was minimal variation in the time between the familiarization trials and test trial within and across participants. Each test trial remained on the screen until the participants responded by pressing a button. This procedure was repeated until all six blocks of trials were completed. During the entire procedure, the participants’ eye movements and responses were recorded by Tobii Studio.

Eye-Tracking Data Preparation (Areas of Interest—AOIs)

All familiarization stimuli were partitioned into areas of interest (AOIs) corresponding to the following areas: eyes (Eyes), nose (Nose), mouth (Mouth), forehead (Forehead), face (Face), and whole stimulus (All). Figure 2 shows an example of all AOIs.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Example areas of interest (AOIs)

Results

Percent Prototype Selection Data

Between Group Analyses

A Mean Prototype Score (MPS) was calculated to reflect the average number of times that the prototype was chosen as familiar across the six test trials. The MPS data is presented in Fig. 3. As can be seen, the autism (M = 50.83%) and control (M = 72.5%) groups significantly differed in their MPSs, with the control group selecting the prototype faces as familiar more often than the autism group (t = − 2.96, p < 0.01). While the autism group did not select the prototype faces as familiar more often than chance (50%) (t = 0.17, p = 0.87), the control group showed clear familiarity for the prototype faces (t = 4.13, p < 0.01).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean prototype scores by diagnosis (*p < 0.01)

In addition to these overall group differences, analyses were conducted to address potential differences in the range or distribution of scores between the two groups. Figure 4 presents the distribution of performance across groups. While 75% of the participants in the control group chose the prototype faces as familiar in at least four out of six trials, only 40% of the autism group performed at this level. In order to analyze the distribution of performance across groups, the groups were divided into subgroups of “good performers” and “poor performers.” Good performers were defined as participants who performed as well as the majority of the control group and chose the prototype face as familiar in at least four out of six trials while poor performers were defined as those who chose the prototype face as familiar in three or fewer trials. A chi-square analysis comparing the distribution of good performers (n = 8 in the autism group, n = 15 in the control group) and poor performers (n = 12 in the autism group, n = 5 in the control group) revealed a significant association between diagnosis and prototype formation, χ2(1) = 5.01, p < 0.05. Based on the odds ratio, individuals in the control group were 4.50 times more likely to show clear evidence of prototype formation than were individuals in the autism group.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Distribution of performance across groups

Within Autism Group Analyses

Participants with autism who were able to successfully form a prototype (good performers) were compared to those who performed poorly on the face prototype task (poor performers) on measures of intelligence (VIQ, PIQ, FSIQ) or behavioral symptoms of autism as measured by the ADOS (ADOS Social Interaction Total Score, ADOS Communication Total Score, ADOS Social Interaction and Communication Total Score, and ADOS Stereotyped Behavior and Restricted Interests Total Score). The means and standard deviations for the measures of intelligence and symptoms of autism are presented in Table 3. Independent samples t tests showed that the only significant difference between the two subgroups was that the good performers had significantly lower Stereotyped Behavior and Restricted Interests Total Scores (M = 1.13) on the ADOS than the poor performers (M = 3.00), t = 3.04, p < 0.01. No other between-group comparisons were significant.

Table 3 Means and standard deviations for measures of intelligence and symptoms of autism for good performers and poor performers

Eye-Tracking Results

Of the 20 individuals with autism and the 20 control individuals, 14 individuals in each group were included in the eye-tracking analyses. Six participants in each group were excluded due to poor eye-tracking data (e.g., poor calibration or lack of accurate eye-tracking). As with the full participant set, no significant differences were found between the two groups on age, FSIQ, VIQ, or PIQ. There were also no significant differences between the participants who were included in the eye-tracking analyses and those that were excluded on age, FSIQ, VIQ, PIQ, or MPS.

Face Versus Background of Stimulus

The first question is whether the autism and the control groups differed in the proportion of time that they spent looking at the face versus the background of the stimulus (% Face). The % Face was calculated by dividing the total amount of time that participants spent looking at the Face AOI (across all familiarization trials) by the total amount of time that they spent looking at the Stimulus AOI (across all familiarization trials) and multiplying the result by 100. An independent samples t test determined that the autism group (M = 93.03%) and the control group (M = 91.21%) did not differ in the percentage of time that they spent looking at the faces, t = 1.14, p = 0.26.

Relevant Versus Irrelevant Aspects of the Face

The second question is whether the autism and control groups differed in the amount of time that they spent looking at relevant aspects of the face versus irrelevant aspects of the face (% Relevant). The % Relevant score was calculated by dividing the total amount of time that the participants spent looking at the Eyes, Mouth, Nose, and Forehead AOIs (across all familiarization trials) by the total amount of time that they spent looking at the Face AOI (across all familiarization trials) and multiplying the result by 100. An independent samples t test indicated that there was not a significant difference between the autism group (M = 95.09%) and control group (M = 98.69%) in the amount of time that they spent looking at the relevant aspects of the face, t = − 1.75, p = 0.09. However, a possible trend emerged, such that the control group spent slightly more time looking at the relevant aspects of the faces than the autism group.

Individual Features

Finally, the proportion of time that each group spent looking at individual features was analyzed in order to examine differences in the distribution of time spent looking at the relevant features between the autism group and the control group. The proportion of time spent looking at each feature (e.g., % Eyes) was calculated by dividing the total amount of time each participant spent looking at each AOI (e.g., Eyes) by the total amount of time the participant spent looking at all of the relevant features (i.e., Eyes + Nose + Mouth + Forehead) and multiplying the result by 100. This data is presented in Fig. 5. A 2 (Group) × 4 (Feature) ANOVA indicated a significant main effect of Feature, F (3, 78) = 33.23, p < 0.01. Post-hoc comparisons (Holm-Bonferroni) resulted in significant differences between all of the features indicating that both groups spent the largest proportion of time looking at the eyes (M = 48.33%) followed by the nose (M = 32.81%), mouth (M = 16.99%), and forehead (M = 1.87%) (p < 0.01 for all comparisons except Eyes vs. Nose, p < 0.05). There was no significant main effect for Group, (F (1, 26) = 1.74, p = 0.20) nor a significant interaction between Group and Feature (F (3, 78) = 1.90, p = 0.14). In general, the control group (M = 25.00%) did not differ from the autism group (M = 25.00%) in the mean percent of time that they spent looking at the features.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean percent looking time by feature (*p < 0.05)

Also of interest was determining whether there were any between-group differences in the percentage of time spent looking at each feature. Independent samples t tests were performed on the data for each feature. As can be seen in Fig. 5, the groups did not differ in the percentage of time spent looking at noses or foreheads (t = − 0.26, p = 0.80 for % Nose and t = 1.34, p = 0.19 for % Forehead). The autism group (M = 42.65%) spent a smaller percentage of time looking at eyes than the control group (M = 54.01%), but this difference did not reach significance, t = − 1.31, p = 0.20. In contrast, the autism group (M = 22.95%) spent a larger percentage of time looking at mouths than the control group (M = 11.02%). This difference reached statistical significance, t = 2.16, p < 0.05.

Discussion

One objective of the current study was to investigate whether individuals with autism, in contrast to typically developing individuals, experience difficulty abstracting prototypes of facial information. As expected, typically developing individuals were able to distinguish the prototype faces from the mode faces and found the prototype faces to be more familiar than faces comprised of features that were previously seen. In contrast, the individuals with autism did not choose the prototype faces as familiar at a level greater than chance, indicating a deficit in face prototype formation. A closer examination of individual participant’s data in the autism group indicated that there was a subset of individuals with autism (n = 8) who performed well and appeared to form a prototype of facial information. These individuals had lower Stereotyped Behavior and Restricted Interests Total Scores on the ADOS than those who performed poorly.

An equally important objective of the current study was to use eye-tracking to address potential reasons why the autism group did not choose the prototype faces as more familiar. Examination of eye fixation patterns indicated that the autism and control groups did not differ in the amount of time that they spent looking at the faces or relevant facial features. Even though the autism group spent more time looking at the eyes than any other facial feature, they spent a smaller percentage of time looking at eyes and a larger percentage of time looking at mouths than the control group. However, the general pattern of attention to the faces for both groups was similar, suggesting that differential attention to faces or features does not explain the difficulty that the individuals with autism had in abstracting facial prototypes.

In general, the results of the current study are consistent with past research that has found a deficit in prototype formation in individuals with autism (Gastgeb et al. 2009; Klinger and Dawson 2001; Klinger et al. 2006; Plaisted 2000). While these results are inconsistent with prior research by Molesworth et al. (2005, 2008), methodological differences between the current study and Molesworth’s studies are likely responsible for these divergent findings. Specifically, the current study improved on prior studies by using more subtle, quantitative spatial variations when designing the stimuli in addition to never showing the mean prototype values during the familiarization phase, making it less likely that participants would have selected the prototype face as familiar without having truly abstracted the prototype.

In addition to determining whether individuals with autism have a deficit in prototype and category formation, it is important to determine why individuals with autism have difficulty with prototype formation. The results of the current study could be explained as a generalized difficulty in processing faces, since it is well known that individuals with autism have deficits in face recognition, gender categorization, and emotion recognition (e.g., Behrmann et al. 2006; Klin et al. 1999; Newell et al. 2010; Rump et al. 2009). However, since prior research has demonstrated prototype deficits with objects (Klinger and Dawson 2001; Klinger et al. 2006; Plaisted 2000), more general explanations need to be explored. Another possibility is that individuals with autism do not pay sufficient attention to faces. However, the eye-tracking data suggests that this is not the case since the individuals with autism did not differ from the typically developing individuals in the percentage of time they spent looking at the faces in general or to the relevant features.

Even though there were no overall differences in attention to the stimuli, there were some interesting differences in the way in which individuals with autism distributed their attention to the facial information during the familiarization phase. Despite the fact that the individuals with autism spent more time looking at the eyes than any other feature of the face, they spent less time looking at the eyes and more time looking at the mouths compared to typically developing individuals. These results are consistent with other eye-tracking studies that suggest individuals with autism devote less attention to the eye region than do control individuals (e.g., Klin et al. 2002; Norbury et al. 2009; Pelphrey et al. 2002). However, the general pattern of attention to the faces for both groups was similar, suggesting that differential attention to features does not explain the difficulty that individuals with autism had in the abstraction of facial prototypes. Thus, prototype formation difficulties cannot solely be accounted for by differential attention to features or different attentional patterns to the faces.

Difficulties in prototype formation may also be related to differences in the way in which individuals with autism cognitively process information. Two theories that address potential differences in perceptual processing are weak central coherence (Frith and Happé 1994) and enhanced perceptual functioning (Mottron et al. 2006). According to these theories, individuals with autism prefer parts over wholes, have a local processing bias, and focus on details. If individuals with autism have weak central coherence or enhanced perceptual functioning, this would likely affect their ability to form a prototype. The differences between individuals with autism who performed well on the task and those who performed poorly provide some support for these explanations. Individuals with autism who performed poorly had higher Stereotyped Behavior and Restricted Interests Total Scores on the ADOS than those who performed well. The Stereotyped Behavior and Restricted Interests Total Score is a summary score made up of subscores including unusual sensory interest in play materials or people, hand and finger and other complex mannerisms, excessive interest in unusual or highly specific topics or objects, and compulsions or rituals. This result suggests that individuals with autism who tend to focus intensely on details, parts or irrelevant aspects of objects, or topics of interest may be more likely to focus on specific aspects of the face rather than the whole face, which would negatively affect prototype formation.

As discussed in the introduction, traditional theories of face perception deficits in autism are bottom-up, perceptual explanations that argue that individuals with autism are biased toward processing local features and are less likely to perceive global patterns. The current study suggests that individuals with autism may also have difficulty with top-down learning processes. That is, individuals with autism may not develop a well-defined face space, an organizational structure that is critical for face processing. Having a well-defined face space has implications for several aspects of face processing and underlies the ability to recognize distinctive faces (e.g., Best and Strauss 2007), discriminate subtle emotional expressions (Rump et al. 2009), and efficiently process and categorize facial information such as gender and age (e.g., O’Toole et al. 1998). It is known that the development of the face space occurs throughout the lifespan and continues to develop through adolescence (e.g., Rump et al. 2009). If individuals with autism have difficulty developing a face space and abstracting prototypes, they may never be able to process faces at the same level of expertise as a typically developing adult. Due to the important role that face processing plays in social interactions, a lack of a well developed face space and difficulty with prototype abstraction would likely impact the general social abilities of individuals with autism as well.

Even though the current study expands and improves on previous research on prototype formation, there are some limitations. One limitation is that participants in the autism group were all high-functioning males. Therefore, the results may not generalize to the full spectrum of autism disorders or to females with autism. Another limitation that occurs in studies of cognitive abilities in high-functioning individuals with autism is that it is difficult to determine whether successful performance on the task reflects intact ability or whether alternative strategies or compensatory mechanisms are used to perform the task. The results of the current study should be replicated in other large samples of individuals with autism with a wide variety of ability levels using both social and non-social stimuli to determine whether prototype formation deficits are domain general and extend to low-functioning individuals with autism. Future studies should also further examine the relationship between prototype formation and symptoms of autism. Although the current study is an important first step and highlights the importance of research on top-down cognitive processes, many more studies need to be conducted to determine the exact role that prototype formation deficits and other top-down cognitive processes play in the syndrome of autism.