Despite over three decades of research since subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were first specified in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association 1980), the optimal approach to describe heterogeneity among individuals with ADHD remains unclear. DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD defined three nominal subtypes based on differential elevations on two dimensions of nine symptoms of inattention and nine symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association 1994). The Predominantly Inattentive Type (ADHD-I) includes individuals with six or more symptoms of inattention and fewer than six symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity, the Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive Type (ADHD-H) includes individuals with six or more symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity and fewer than six symptoms of inattention, and the Combined Type (ADHD-C) is defined by six or more symptoms on both dimensions.

A comprehensive literature review and meta-analysis of 546 studies was recently completed to evaluate the validity of the DSM-IV model of ADHD (Willcutt et al. 2012). Factor analyses and studies of external correlates overwhelmingly supported the internal and external validity of the inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptom dimensions. In contrast, the review found weak evidence to support the distinction between ADHD-C and ADHD-I in studies of academic and cognitive functioning, longitudinal stability, etiological influences, and treatment response. These results challenged the validity of the DSM-IV subtype model and underscored the need to develop and test new theoretical models that may account for the weak discriminant validity of DSM-IV ADHD-C and ADHD-I.

In DSM-IV and previous editions of the DSM, the inattentive subtypes of ADHD (DSM-III ADD without hyperactivity, DSM-III-R undifferentiated ADD, DSM-IV ADHD-I) have always been defined based on the presence of a subset of symptoms of ADHD (i.e., inattention and impulsivity in DSM-III and inattention in DSM-III-R and DSM-IV) in the absence of clinically significant elevations of hyperactivity or hyperactivity-impulsivity. Based on concerns that this approach might contribute to the weak discriminant validity of ADHD-C and ADHD-I, several authors suggested that the validity of a predominantly inattentive subgroup might be improved by developing positive diagnostic criteria for ADHD-I (e.g., Carlson and Mann 2002; McBurnett et al. 2001; Milich et al. 2001).

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo and its Relation to DSM-IV ADHD

One potential candidate for positive diagnostic criteria for ADHD-I is a cluster of behaviors that was first identified by exploratory factor analyses (EFA) of the Children’s Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS; Neeper and Lahey 1984), a teacher rating scale that included a large pool of items related to developmental psychopathology (Neeper and Lahey 1986). Many of the seven factors that were extracted were similar to dimensions of psychopathology that had been described previously in the clinical literature (Inattention-Disorganization, Hyperactivity, Conduct Disorder, Anxiety/Depression). In addition, the EFA identified a novel Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) factor that included five items related to cognitive sluggishness, drowsiness, and lethargic and apathetic behavior (Neeper and Lahey 1986).

Measures of SCT were then included in several studies that systematically compared the DSM-III ADD subtypes on an extensive range of measures (e.g., Lahey et al. 1988; Lahey et al. 1985; 1987; Stanford and Hynd 1994). Results of these studies indicated that DSM-III ADD with and without hyperactivity were associated with many of the same clinical correlates, but one characteristic that consistently distinguished the subtypes were significantly higher levels of SCT symptoms in children with ADD without hyperactivity.

Based on these results, the DSM-IV field trials tested the utility of two SCT symptoms as part of the provisional diagnostic criteria for ADHD, and confirmed that SCT symptoms were associated most strongly with ADHD-I (Frick et al. 1994). However, due to relatively weak predictive power these symptoms were not included in the final DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD, and ADHD-I was once again differentiated from ADHD-C by the absence of clinically significant levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity. Nonetheless, the results of the field trials spurred additional interest in SCT, and after DSM-IV was published a number of studies continued to examine the relation between SCT and DSM-IV ADHD symptoms.

Factor Analyses of SCT and DSM-IV ADHD

Seven studies have conducted exploratory or confirmatory factor analyses of parent or teacher ratings of SCT and DSM-IV ADHD symptoms (Barkley 2013; Garner et al. 2010; Hartman et al. 2004; Lahey et al. 2004; McBurnett et al. 2001; Penny et al. 2009; Todd et al. 2004; see Willcutt et al. 2012 for a full review). Six of these studies found that at least a subset of putative SCT symptoms had a primary loading on a third factor that was distinct from DSM-IV inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms.

These initial factor analytic studies provided important support for the internal validity of SCT, but also had important limitations. Several SCT items are similar to specific symptoms of major depressive disorder (e.g., psychomotor retardation and fatigue or loss of energy) and generalized anxiety disorder (e.g., mind going blank and easily fatigued), and overall SCT scores are significantly correlated with a range of measures of internalizing and externalizing symptoms (Garner et al. 2010; Hartman et al. 2004; Neeper and Lahey 1986; Penny et al. 2009; Wahlstedt and Bohlin 2010). Despite these correlations only two previous factor analytic studies of SCT and ADHD included items from other dimensions of psychopathology (Lahey et al. 2004; Neeper and Lahey 1986).

As described earlier, the study that first described SCT identified an SCT factor that was distinct from six other factors that were similar to DSM-IV symptom dimensions (Neeper and Lahey 1986). In contrast, a second study did not find an SCT factor in the six-factor solution that emerged from EFA of a parent interview that included items corresponding to ADHD, SCT, and most major domains of DSM-IV psychopathology (Lahey et al. 2004). Instead, five symptoms that were similar to the SCT items that were used in other studies (day-dreamed and lost in thoughts, dawdled and worked slowly, stared into space, took longer to answer than others, difficulty thinking or deciding) had weak loadings (0.43–0.49) on a factor that included items similar to DSM-IV inattention symptoms, and one potential SCT item (sleepy during the day) had a primary loading on a factor that was labeled Depression. These conflicting results underscore the need for additional research to clarify the relations among SCT, DSM-IV ADHD, and other dimensions of psychopathology.

External Validity of SCT

For a dimension of psychopathology, significant associations with important aspects of functional impairment provide the most decisive evidence of the external validity and clinical importance of the construct. A handful of studies have reported significant univariate associations between SCT symptoms and measures of academic, social, and neuropsychological functioning (Barkley 2013; Garner et al. 2010; Hartman et al. 2004; Neeper and Lahey 1986; Wahlstedt and Bohlin 2010). However, only a few measures of impairment were included in these studies, and multiple regression analyses suggest that some associations between SCT and impairment may be explained by covariance between SCT and inattention (Barkley 2013; Wahlstedt and Bohlin 2010). Additional systematic research is needed to clarify whether SCT symptoms are independently associated with significant functional impairment.

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo and the DSM-IV ADHD Subtypes

In an attempt to improve the weak discriminant validity of DSM-IV ADHD-I and ADHD-C, several studies have tested whether the presence of high levels of SCT symptoms identify a subgroup of ADHD-I that is more clearly distinct from ADHD-C (e.g., Carlson and Mann 2002; McBurnett et al. 2001). Consistent with this hypothesis, some initial studies found that the subgroup of ADHD-I with high SCT was significantly different from ADHD-C on some measures of functional impairment (Carlson and Mann 2002) and selective attention (Huang-Pollock et al. 2005), whereas the group with ADHD-I without SCT was not. However, other subsequent studies reported little evidence that levels of SCT moderated the associations between ADHD-I and measures of functional impairment, neuropsychological functioning, or treatment response (e.g., Bauermeister et al. 2005; Harrington and Waldman 2010; Hinshaw et al. 2007; Hinshaw et al. 2002; Ludwig et al. 2009).

The Current Study

As part of the ongoing Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center (CLDRC) twin study, participants who met symptom criteria for DSM-IV ADHD (N = 410) and a comparison group without ADHD (N = 311) completed an extensive battery that included measures of ADHD and SCT, internalizing and externalizing psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. These data were used for a series of analyses designed to test the internal and external validity of SCT and its relation with DSM-IV ADHD. The three primary objectives of the study were as follows:

  1. 1.

    The internal validity of SCT was tested by conducting exploratory factor analyses of nine potential SCT symptoms along with all DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder (CD), major depressive disorder (MDD), and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). We hypothesized that at least a subset of putative SCT symptoms would have primary loadings on a factor that was distinct from factors that included symptoms of DSM-IV inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity or the other dimensions of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology.

  2. 2.

    After identifying the cluster of SCT items with optimal internal validity, the external validity of SCT was examined by testing if SCT symptoms were associated with significant functional impairment or neuropsychological difficulties. The discriminant validity of SCT and DSM-IV ADHD was then tested by including symptoms of SCT, ADHD, and other psychopathology as simultaneous predictors in multiple regression models predicting each external measure. We tentatively predicted that after controlling for the other variables, SCT symptoms would be independently associated with social isolation, academic difficulties, and slow cognitive processing speed, whereas DSM-IV inattention symptoms would be more strongly associated with externalizing symptoms and weaknesses on executive function measures.

  3. 3.

    The final set of analyses tested whether the relatively weak discriminant validity of DSM-IV ADHD-C and ADHD-I was improved by subdividing the group with ADHD-I based on SCT symptoms. We hypothesized that in comparison to the group with ADHD-C, the group with ADHD-I with high levels of SCT would exhibit significantly higher levels of social withdrawal and greater impairment on measures of sustained attention and processing speed. In contrast, we anticipated that the group with ADHD-I with high SCT would exhibit lower levels of externalizing behaviors and overt social rejection in comparison to the group with ADHD-C and the group with ADHD-I without elevated SCT.

Method

Participants

The full recruitment procedures for the CLDRC study are described in detail in previous papers (e.g., Shanahan et al. 2006; Willcutt et al. 2010b; Willcutt et al. 2005). Briefly, families of all twins between the ages of 8 and 16 in 22 local school districts were invited to participate in the initial screening procedures for the study. If either of the twins met symptom criteria for DSM-IV ADHD based on the screening procedures described in the subsequent section, the twin pair was invited to participate in the full study (90 % of selected families agreed to participate). A comparison sample was also recruited from pairs in which neither twin met screening criteria for ADHD. Twins with significant reading difficulties were recruited independently as part of the overall CLDRC study, but twins with reading difficulties alone were not included in the current analyses (reading achievement was free to vary in the ADHD and control groups).

Exclusion Criteria

As part of the larger study, potential participants with a documented brain injury, significant hearing or visual impairment, or a rare genetic or environmental etiology (e.g., Fragile X syndrome, phenylketonuria, Down syndrome or other chromosomal anomalies) were excluded from the sample. In addition, any twins with a previous diagnosis of a pervasive developmental disorder, psychosis, tic disorder, or bipolar disorder were excluded from the study, and participants with a Full Scale IQ score below 75 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Revised (Wechsler 1974) were excluded from the current analyses (this criterion excluded two potential participants who met research criteria for ADHD-C, three who met criteria for ADHD-I, one participant with ADHD-H, and one participant from the comparison group).

The Final Sample

Because twins in a pair are not independent observations, one twin was selected at random from each pair in which both twins met inclusion criteria for the control or ADHD groups. If one twin met criteria for ADHD and one did not, the twin who met criteria for ADHD was included in the study to maximize power for ADHD subtype comparisons. The final sample included 410 individuals with DSM-IV ADHD and a comparison group of 311 individuals (52 % female). Consistent with results obtained in other community samples (e.g., Willcutt 2012), more participants met research criteria for ADHD-I (n = 235; 36 % female) than ADHD-C (n = 135; 25 % female) or ADHD-H (n = 40; 30 % female). The overall sample was 80 % Caucasian, 12 % Hispanic, 3 % African American, 3 % Asian American, and 2 % American Indian/Native American, with no significant differences between groups with and without ADHD.

Procedure

All study procedures were fully approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Colorado, Boulder, and University of Denver, and have therefore been performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Parents gave their informed consent and children and adolescents assented to participate prior to their enrollment in the study.

Measures were administered in four testing sessions at the University of Colorado, Boulder, and University of Denver. All examiners were unaware of the diagnostic status of the child and the results of the testing conducted at the other sites. Parents of participants that were taking psychostimulant medication were asked to withhold medication for 24 h prior to each session of the study to minimize the influence of medication on the results.

Measures

The analyses described in this report include data from over 60 individual measures, including 45 measures of functional impairment and neuropsychological functioning. These measures have been described in detail in previous papers (e.g., Keenan et al. 2008; McGrath et al. 2011; Willcutt et al. 2010b; 2005), and space constraints preclude a full description of all individual measures in the body of the current paper. Therefore, this section includes abbreviated descriptions of the measures of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. Section 1 of the supplemental materials provides a full description and citation for each individual measure, a summary of the procedures used to create each composite score that was included in the current analyses, and information regarding the psychometric characteristics of all individual tasks and composite scores.

DSM-IV ADHD Symptoms

The Disruptive Behavior Rating Scale (DBRS; Barkley and Murphy 1998) was used to obtain parent and teacher ratings of the 18 symptoms of DSM-IV ADHD. Each symptom on the DBRS is rated on a four point scale (never or rarely, sometimes, often, and very often). For analyses of symptom counts items rated as often or very often were scored as positive symptoms and items rated as never or rarely or sometimes were scored as negative symptoms, consistent with the procedure used in previous studies (e.g., Lahey et al. 1998). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms were combined using an adaptation of the or rule algorithm used in the DSM-IV field trials (Lahey et al. 1994). For each symptom the higher of the parent or teacher ratings was used as the score for that item. The mean of the nine items on each DSM-IV symptom dimension was then age-regressed and standardized based on the overall sample to create composite measures of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity. Both composite scores had high internal consistency (α = 0.96 for inattention and 0.93 for hyperactivity-impulsivity) and 12-month test-retest reliability (r = 0.84 for inattention and 0.75 for hyperactivity-impulsivity).

ADHD Subtypes

DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD require an individual to exhibit significant impairment across settings. However, functional impairment was a key dependent variable for many of the analyses described in this paper. Therefore, ADHD subtype classifications were defined for research purposes based on symptom criteria only. Participants with six or more symptoms of inattention and fewer than six symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity were included in the group with ADHD-I, participants with six or more symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity and fewer than six symptoms of inattention were coded as ADHD-H, and individuals with six or more symptoms on both symptom dimensions were categorized as ADHD-C.

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Items

Nine potential SCT items were developed based on theoretical models of SCT and items used in previous studies of ADHD and SCT (e.g., Carlson and Mann 2002; McBurnett et al. 2001; Penny et al. 2009). Each item was added to the DBRS and administered in the same format.

Comorbid Psychopathology

One parent completed modules of the DSM-IV Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (Reich et al. 1997) for oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder (CD), major depressive disorder (MDD), and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). In addition, parents and teachers completed the Achenbach Scale for Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA; Achenbach and Rescorla 2001), a psychopathology screening measure that includes measures of externalizing symptoms (Aggressive Behavior, Delinquent Behavior) and internalizing symptoms (Anxious/Depressed, Withdrawn, and Somatic Complaints).

Functional Impairment

Global Functioning

Parents completed the Child Global Assessment Scale (CGAS; Setterberg et al. 1992), a measure of global impairment that requires the rater to indicate the single number between 1 and 100 that best represents the individual’s functioning. In addition, items on the parent and teacher DBRS were used to assess the extent to which the twin has difficulty managing daily responsibilities.

Academic Functioning

The CLDRC study includes an extensive battery of standardized tests of academic achievement (described in detail in Section 1 of the supplemental materials). Scores on these achievement tests were used to create reliable composite measures of word reading, reading comprehension, math, and written language. In addition to these psychometric measures of academic achievement, parents and teachers rated the twin's understanding of classroom assignments and current academic performance in reading, math, and language arts.

Social Functioning

Multiple measures were also administered to facilitate the assessment of different dimensions of social functioning. Scores on the ASEBA Social Problems scale and items from the DBRS were combined to create a composite measure of overall social impairment. Parent ratings on the Colorado Learning Difficulties Questionnaire (CLDQ; Willcutt et al. 2011) were used to assess social isolation (e.g., isolates self in social situations, feels anxious or out of place in new social settings) and social cognition (e.g., has trouble understanding how others are feeling). Finally, teachers estimated the proportion of children who like, dislike, or ignore the participant using the procedure described by Dishion (1990).

Neuropsychological Measures

The six composite measures of neuropsychological functioning include measures of constructs that were most strongly associated with ADHD or SCT in previous studies. As described in detail in Section 1 the supplemental materials, the individual test scores were combined to create standardized composite scores based on previous exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (e.g., McGrath et al. 2011; Willcutt et al. 2010a).

Response Inhibition

The response inhibition measures assess the ability to inhibit a prepotent response. The Stop-signal Task is a computerized task that provides an estimate of stop-signal reaction time, a measure of the latency of the inhibitory process (e.g., Logan et al. 1997). The Gordon Diagnostic System (Gordon 1983) is a visual continuous performance test (CPT) that requires the participant to respond when a 9 appears immediately after a 1. The primary measure of inhibition is the number of commission errors in response to a sequence of numbers other than the target.

Working Memory

The three verbal working memory tasks require the participant to retain and manipulate verbal information in memory. On the Sentence Span Task (Siegel and Ryan 1989) the participant provides the last word for a set of simple sentences read by the examiner (e.g., “throw the ball up and then it comes…”), then must reproduce the words that they provided after all sentences in that set are completed. Similarly, the Counting Span Task (Case et al. 1982) requires the participant to count aloud the number of dots on a series of cards, then recall the number of dots on each card in the set. Finally, the Digits Backward component of WISC-R Digit Span (Wechsler 1974) requires the participant to repeat in reverse order a series of numbers presented aloud by the examiner.

Processing Speed

A composite processing speed score was created by averaging the standardized scores on four measures that loaded on a latent processing speed factor in our previous analyses (McGrath et al. 2011). The WISC-III Symbol Search (Wechsler 1991) and WISC-R Coding subtests are widely-used psychometric measures of processing speed. On the Colorado Perceptual Speed Test (DeFries et al. 1978) the participant circles one of four possible letter strings to match a target letter string as rapidly as possible. Similarly, the Identical Pictures Test (French et al. 1963) requires the participant to identify as quickly as possible the one picture out of five options that matches a target picture.

Naming Speed

The Rapid Automatized Naming Test is an adaptation of the measure developed by Denckla and Rudel (Denckla and Rudel 1976). On each of the four test trials the participant names as many objects, numbers, letters, or colors as possible in 15 s.

Sustained Attention

As described earlier, we tentatively hypothesized that SCT would be associated with difficulty remaining consistently attentive during lengthy tasks. The primary measure of sustained attention in the current battery was the number of omission errors (failure to respond to the target sequence) during the CPT described previously (Gordon 1983).

Response Variability

Finally, the primary measures of response variability was the intraindividual standard deviation of reaction times on the primary task trials of the stop-signal task.

Data Analyses

Data Cleaning and Adjustments

The distribution of each variable was first assessed for outliers, defined as scores that fell more than three standard deviations (SD) from the mean of the overall sample and more than 0.5 SD beyond the next most extreme score. Most measures had no scores that met these criteria, but a small number of outliers were identified on the CPT (two individuals in the ADHD group and one in the comparison group) and the stop-signal task (three individuals in the ADHD group and one in the control group). After confirming that the outlying score was entered correctly in the data file, each outlier was adjusted to a score 0.5 SD units beyond the next highest score, with multiple outliers rescored to 0.1 SD apart.

The distribution of each variable was then assessed for significant deviation from normality, and a logarithmic transformation was implemented to approximate a normal distribution if skewness or kurtosis was greater than one (parent and teacher ratings on the ASEBA, CPT commission errors). As expected, correlational analyses suggested that several measures of impairment and neuropsychological functioning were significantly correlated with age. Therefore, an age-adjusted score was created for each measure by regressing the variable onto age and saving the standardized residual score.

Analyses to Test the Internal Validity of SCT and ADHD

Factor Analyses of ADHD and SCT

Initial EFA included the 18 DSM-IV ADHD symptoms and nine potential SCT symptoms. Because theoretical models of ADHD and SCT suggested that the extracted factors would be correlated, principal axis factor analyses were conducted with an oblique promax rotation, and all factors with Eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were extracted. Results were similar when analyses were repeated using principal components analysis with an orthogonal rotation, suggesting that the factor structure is robust across methods. Primary loadings were defined as loadings of 0.60 or higher, and items were considered cross-loaded if a secondary loading was greater than 0.30 and within 0.20 of the item’s primary factor loading. To examine whether the factor structure was robust across raters, initial EFA were conducted separately for parent ratings, teacher ratings, and parent and teacher ratings combined using the or rule procedure.

Expanded Factor Analytic Models

As noted earlier, SCT symptoms bear some resemblance to features of internalizing disorders, and previous studies suggest that the factor structure of ADHD may vary depending on the specific symptoms of disorders other than ADHD that are included in the analysis (e.g., McBurnett et al. 2001; Pillow et al. 1998). Therefore, to provide a more definitive test of the internal validity of SCT and ADHD, a second EFA included symptoms of DSM-IV ADHD, ODD, GAD, and MDD along with the six items that loaded on the SCT factor in the first EFA. Symptoms of CD were also initially included in the second analysis, but were dropped due to extremely low rates of endorsement in the current sample (none of the CD items cross-loaded on the SCT or ADHD factors when they were initially included).

Analyses to Test the External and Discriminant Validity of SCT and ADHD

Primary Analyses of SCT and ADHD

The external validity of SCT was first examined by computing zero-order correlations between SCT and the composite measures of functional impairment and neuropsychological functioning. To test whether any significant effects were independently associated with SCT or explained by covariance with ADHD symptoms, SCT, inattention, and hyperactivity-impulsivity composite scores were entered simultaneously as independent variables in multiple regression models predicting each measure of impairment.

Other Potential Covariates

Zygosity and socioeconomic status were included in initial multiple regression models, but were dropped from final models because neither variable had a significant impact on any result. Potential differences in the pattern of results as a function of age or sex were examined by testing for differences in males and females and in subsets of the sample age 12 or younger versus age 13 or older. Because the pattern of results was similar, results are reported for the full sample.

Some researchers have argued that general intelligence and symptoms of concurrent mental disorders should also be statistically controlled to ensure that any associations between SCT, inattention, or hyperactivity-impulsivity and measures of functional impairment cannot be explained more parsimoniously by these correlated variables (e.g., Lahey et al. 1998). In contrast, others have pointed out that the decision to statistically control these variables may potentially remove meaningful variance if ADHD or SCT symptoms directly cause poor performance on standardized tests of intelligence (e.g., Barkley 1997), or if shared etiological influences lead to covariance between symptoms of different disorders (e.g., Willcutt et al. 2010a). Because these issues have not been resolved conclusively, results are presented both ways. The first step of each regression model included symptoms of SCT, inattention, and hyperactivity-impulsivity, and Full Scale IQ and symptoms of ODD, CD, GAD, and MDD were then added simultaneously in a second step to test whether any effects that were significant in the first step of the model were explained by these covariates. Collinearity diagnostics were within the acceptable range for all multiple regression models (Tolerance = 0.31–0.80; VIF = 1.25–2.80).

Analyses to Test the Relation Between DSM-IV ADHD Symptoms and SCT

A final set of analyses was conducted to test whether the presence of elevated SCT symptoms identified a subgroup of ADHD-I that was more clearly distinct from ADHD-C. The group with ADHD-I was divided into subgroups with high and low SCT based on a cutoff score one SD above the estimated population mean on the SCT composite. Analyses of variance with planned comparisons were then conducted to compare the two ADHD-I groups to the groups with ADHD-C and ADHD-H and the comparison group without ADHD on the measures of internalizing and externalizing symptoms, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning.

Results

Internal Validity

EFA of Symptoms of ADHD and SCT

Exploratory factor analyses of parent ratings, teacher ratings, and combined parent and teacher ratings of ADHD and SCT symptoms each yielded three factors with Eigenvalues greater than one. Consistent with expectations based on the DSM-IV model of ADHD, all DSM-IV inattention symptoms had a primary loading on the first factor that was extracted, and all DSM-IV hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms had a primary loading on the second extracted factor. Six of the potential SCT items had a primary loading on a third factor, whereas the other three putative SCT items cross-loaded on the Inattention factor in at least one of the analyses (due to space constraints only the factor loadings for the six final SCT items are provided in Table 1; Supplement Table 1 includes factor loadings for all 27 items). After the three items that cross-loaded on the SCT and Inattention factors were dropped, all ADHD and SCT items had a primary loading on the predicted factor, and none cross-loaded on either of the other factors. The same pattern of results emerged when analyses were restricted to the comparison sample without ADHD.

Table 1 Exploratory factor analyses of DSM-IV ADHD symptoms and potential SCT items

EFA of Symptoms of SCT, ADHD and Other Psychopathology

When symptoms of GAD, MDD, and ODD were also included in a second EFA, all six SCT items continued to have a primary loading on a single factor with no cross-loadings on the other five factors that were extracted (Supplement Table 2). Similarly, all DSM-IV inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity items had a primary loading on their expected factor, although two hyperactivity-impulsivity items also cross-loaded on the inattention factor (fidgets and leaves seat in the classroom). Nearly all symptoms of ODD, GAD, and MDD had a primary loading on their expected factor. Most importantly, none of the inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity, or SCT symptoms loaded on factors with symptoms of ODD, GAD, or MDD, and only one MDD symptom (psychomotor retardation) loaded on the factor with the SCT items.

External Validity

A standardized SCT composite score was created based on the mean of the six items that loaded on the SCT factor (α = 0.93 and 12-month test-retest reliability r = 0.73). Analyses of external validity yielded similar results for parent ratings, teacher ratings, and combined parent and teacher ratings of ADHD and SCT symptoms. To simplify presentation results are reported for the combined parent and teacher ratings, and a detailed summary of the analyses of parent and teacher ratings alone is available from the first author upon request.

Concurrent Mental Disorders

Zero-order correlations indicated that SCT symptoms were significantly associated with both ADHD symptom dimensions and all measures of internalizing and externalizing symptoms (Table 2), and multiple regression analyses indicated that SCT symptoms were independently associated with all measures of internalizing symptoms when inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were controlled. In contrast, SCT was not associated with externalizing symptoms when inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were also included in the multiple regression models.

Table 2 Multiple regression analyses of the relations between symptoms of ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo and symptoms of other psychopathology

Functional Impairment

With the exception of the tendency to be disliked by peers, SCT symptoms were independently associated with all aspects of global, academic, and social impairment when symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were controlled (Table 3). DSM-IV inattention symptoms were also independently associated with many of these impairment measures, but only SCT symptoms were independently associated with social isolation and low achievement in written language. Importantly, nearly all of these results remained significant when full scale IQ and symptoms of ODD, CD, GAD, and MDD were added to the model (Supplement Table 3).

Table 3 Multiple regression analyses of the relations between symptoms of ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo and measures of functional impairment

Neuropsychological Functioning

Zero-order correlations were significant between SCT and all six neuropsychological composites (Table 4), and both SCT and DSM-IV inattention were significantly associated with processing and naming speed when SCT, inattention, and hyperactivity-impulsivity were included in multiple regression models. In contrast, the multiple regression models indicated that only inattention was independently associated with response inhibition, working memory, and increased response variability, and only SCT was independently associated with a weakness in sustained attention after full scale IQ was controlled (Supplement Table 4).

Table 4 Multiple regression analyses of the relations between symptoms of ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo and measures of neuropsychological functioning

To supplement the primary neuropsychological analyses, additional multiple regression analyses were conducted to test whether SCT, inattention, or hyperactivity-impulsivity were each associated with multiple independent neuropsychological weaknesses. In the first step of each model, scores on one of the symptom dimensions were regressed simultaneously onto the six neuropsychological composites. Scores on the other two symptom dimensions were then added as independent variables in a second step to test whether any significant neuropsychological weaknesses in the first step were uniquely associated with the symptom dimension when scores on the other two dimensions were controlled.

Inattention symptoms were associated with independent weaknesses in response inhibition (B = 0.22, 95 % CI [0.12, 0.32], p < 0.0001), response variability (B = 0.10, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.18], p < 0.01), naming speed (B = 0.14, 95 % CI [0.06, 0.22], p < 0.01), and processing speed (B = 0.33, 95 % CI [0.23, 0.43], p < 0.0001), and all of these effects remained significant when hyperactivity-impulsivity and SCT were controlled in the second step of the regression model (all p < 0.01). SCT scores were also independently associated with weaknesses in naming speed (B = 0.17, 95 % CI [0.07, 0.27], p < 0.0001), and processing speed (B = 0.20, 95 % CI [0.09, 0.31], p < 0.0001), along with a significant weakness in sustained attention (B = 0.10, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.18], p < 0.01). However, only the weakness in sustained attention was significantly associated with SCT when symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were added in the second step of the model. Finally, hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms were predicted independently by weaknesses in response inhibition (B = 0.26, 95 % CI [0.16, 0.36], p < 0.0001) and processing speed (B = 0.25, 95 % CI [0.14, 0.36], p < 0.0001), but only the association with inhibition remained significant after controlling for symptoms of inattention and SCT.

Analyses of the Relation Between SCT and DSM-IV ADHD Subtypes

The final series of analyses examined the relation between SCT scores and groups that met research criteria for the DSM-IV ADHD subtypes (descriptive characteristics of the subtype groups are summarized in Table 5). Mean SCT scores were significantly higher in groups with ADHD-I and ADHD-C than the group with ADHD-H and the comparison group without ADHD. The group with ADHD-I also exhibited significantly more symptoms of SCT than the group with ADHD-C based on teacher ratings and combined parent and teacher ratings. However, the effect sizes for these comparisons were small (Hedges’ g = 0.2–0.3), and groups with ADHD-C and ADHD-I did not differ on parent ratings of SCT.

Table 5 Characteristics of groups with and without DSM-IV ADHD subtypes based on research criteria

To test whether elevations of SCT symptoms identified a subgroup of ADHD-I that was more clearly distinct from ADHD-C, the group with ADHD-I was divided into groups with low versus high levels of SCT (Fig. 1; results were similar for all neuropsychological measures, so were combined in the Figure). Results indicated that the group with ADHD-C and both groups with ADHD-I differed from the comparison group and the group with ADHD-H on all measures of functional impairment and neuropsychological functioning (p < 0.001). In contrast, the only significant difference between the group with ADHD-C and the group with ADHD-I with high SCT was the significantly higher number of externalizing symptoms in the group with ADHD-C.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Point estimates indicate effect sizes (Hedges’ g) for comparisons of each DSM-IV ADHD subtype and the control group without ADHD. The group with ADHD-I was subdivided into groups with (N = 89) and without (N = 144) high SCT based on a cutoff score one standard deviation above the estimated population mean on the SCT composite measure. Error bars indicate the standard error of each effect size

Discussion

This study systematically evaluated the internal and external validity of SCT and its relation with DSM-IV ADHD and other psychopathology. In this section we first discuss the implications of the current results for the internal and external validity of SCT and the DSM-IV model of ADHD. We then examine the broader clinical and theoretical implications of these findings, and conclude by highlighting several key directions for future research to better understand the nature of SCT and its relation with ADHD and other disorders.

Internal Validity of SCT

Exploratory factor analyses identified six SCT items that loaded on a factor separate from symptoms of DSM-IV inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, replicating most previous factor analytic studies of SCT and ADHD symptoms (Barkley 2013; Garner et al. 2010; McBurnett et al. 2001; Penny et al. 2009; Todd et al. 2004). Furthermore, the same 6-item SCT factor emerged in a second EFA that also included symptoms of DSM-IV MDD, GAD, ODD, and CD. These results are consistent with one previous study that identified a SCT factor among seven factors that emerged in EFA of teacher ratings on a psychopathology screening measure (Neeper and Lahey 1986). However, a second EFA of a larger pool of items derived from symptoms of DSM-IV disorders did not identify a separate SCT factor, and instead found that several potential SCT symptoms loaded on a factor with items similar to DSM-IV inattention symptoms (Lahey et al. 2004).

One possible explanation for the apparent discrepancy between the current results and the study by Lahey and colleagues (2004) is the specific SCT items that were included in each study. The SCT items with the highest factor loadings in the current study emphasize low physical energy, sleepiness, and sluggishness (e.g., underactive, slow moving, sluggish, drowsy/sleepy), whereas the items in the earlier study primarily assess slow responses to questions and difficulty completing tasks efficiently (dawdled and worked slowly, difficulty thinking or deciding, took longer to answer than others). Two other independent EFA of large pools of potential SCT items also found that items that assessed slow initiation and completion of tasks loaded with DSM-IV inattention symptoms, whereas items similar to the SCT items included in the current study loaded on a Sleepy/Daydreamer factor that was distinct from DSM-IV inattention (Barkley 2013; Penny et al. 2009).

Overall, results of the current EFA provide strong support for the internal validity of SCT. Although SCT symptoms are significantly correlated with most other dimensions of psychopathology, these results provide initial support for the hypothesis that SCT is a distinct symptom dimension, especially when measured by items that assess low energy and sluggish or sleepy behavior.

External Validity of SCT and Discriminant Validity of SCT and DSM-IV Inattention

Zero-order correlations indicated that SCT symptoms are associated with nearly all measures of functional impairment, replicating and extending previous studies (e.g., Garner et al. 2010; Neeper and Lahey 1986). Further, most of these associations remained significant when symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, internalizing and externalizing psychopathology, and Full Scale IQ were controlled. Symptoms of SCT and DSM-IV inattention were both independently associated with overall social difficulties, higher rates of being ignored by peers, and lower achievement in reading and math. In contrast, only SCT was significantly associated with social isolation and weaknesses in written production, and only DSM-IV inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were independently associated with greater likelihood of being disliked by peers.

Analyses of neuropsychological measures yielded a similar pattern of shared and distinct weaknesses associated with SCT and inattention. SCT and inattention were both significantly correlated with all six neuropsychological composites, and multiple regression analyses indicated that SCT and inattention were independently associated with slow naming and processing speed. In contrast, only SCT symptoms were also associated with deficits in sustained attention, and this weakness remained significant when the other five neuropsychological composites were included in a single multiple regression model. On the other hand, only DSM-IV inattention symptoms were associated with independent weaknesses on measures of response inhibition and response variability. The patterns of results for SCT and DSM-IV inattention are both consistent with the findings reported by the only other dimensional study of the neuropsychological correlates of SCT and inattention (Wahlstedt and Bohlin 2010) and a recent study of the relations among parent ratings of SCT, ADHD, and executive functions (Barkley 2013).

In summary, the external validity of SCT was strongly supported by significant associations between SCT and multiple dimensions of functional impairment and neuropsychological functioning, and most of these effects remained significant when general cognitive ability and symptoms of ADHD and other psychopathology were controlled. Although many external correlates were associated with both SCT and ADHD, the discriminant validity of SCT and DSM-IV inattention was supported by differential associations with social isolation, academic achievement, and several dimensions of neuropsychological functioning. These results suggest that SCT may be best conceptualized as a clinically valid construct that is at least partially distinct from ADHD and other DSM-IV disorders.

Impact of SCT on ADHD Subtype Classification

The group with ADHD-I had significantly higher SCT scores than any other group, but the effect size of the difference between groups with ADHD-C and ADHD-I was small in magnitude (g = 0.2–0.3). In contrast, both ADHD-I and ADHD-C exhibited significantly more SCT symptoms than the comparison group without ADHD, and effect sizes were large for both groups (ADHD-I vs. control g = 1.3–1.5; ADHD-C vs. control g = 1.1–1.3).

To test directly whether elevations of SCT symptoms identified a subgroup of ADHD-I that was more clearly distinct from ADHD-C, the group with ADHD-I was subdivided into groups with low versus high levels of SCT. The only significant difference between the group with ADHD-C and the subgroup with ADHD-I with high SCT was the significantly higher number of externalizing symptoms in the group with ADHD-C. Contrary to our initial hypothesis, elevations of SCT symptoms do not appear to be a marker for a subset of cases with ADHD-I that are distinct from ADHD-C. Instead, the current findings and the results of a recent meta-analysis (Willcutt et al. 2012) suggest that SCT symptoms are strongly associated with both ADHD-C and ADHD-I.

Clinical Implications of SCT

From a clinical perspective, arguably the most important result from the current study is the finding that ADHD and SCT symptoms are independently associated with multiple aspects of functional impairment even after symptoms of ODD, CD, GAD, and MDD are controlled. These findings indicate that ADHD and SCT symptoms provide unique information that has important clinical implications, and suggest that symptoms of SCT should be assessed routinely as part of a comprehensive clinical evaluation.

Multiple regression models of ADHD and SCT composite scores indicated that only SCT symptoms were independently associated with increased social isolation and withdrawal and specific difficulties in written language, but the causes of these associations are unknown. Although admittedly speculative, one possible explanation is that individuals with SCT may become overwhelmed by the rapid flow of complex information that must be processed continuously to successfully navigate social interactions, which may then lead to avoidance of social situations and subsequent isolation. Similarly, difficulty sustaining attention and working quickly could have an especially pronounced impact on writing tasks that require sustained effort over an extended period of time. Alternatively, poor performance on tests of achievement in written language could also reflect weaknesses in more basic processes such as the fine-motor movements that were not assessed in the current study. Future research is needed to test these tentative hypotheses and other competing explanations for the relations between SCT and specific aspects of functional impairment.

Finally, the significant impairment associated with SCT suggests that future research is needed to develop and test interventions that target SCT symptoms. To the best of our knowledge no previous or ongoing intervention studies have focused specifically on SCT, suggesting that it may be some time before these data are available. Until then, preliminary analyses of the impact of SCT on treatment response could potentially be conducted as an extension of ongoing treatment studies of ADHD or other disorders that included measures of SCT.

Implications and Future Directions for Theoretical Models of SCT

Despite a growing literature that suggests that SCT is a reliable and valid construct that is clinically important, theoretical models of SCT remain largely descriptive (e.g., Penny et al. 2009). To facilitate the development of a more comprehensive model of SCT, this section highlights several findings from the current study that may provide useful guidance or important constraints for future theoretical models.

Refinement of the Construct of SCT

Although EFA are descriptive and largely atheoretical, these results provide useful data to refine theoretical models of SCT. Along with other factor analyses (e.g., Barkley 2013; Penny et al. 2009), the current results suggest that SCT items that are characterized by low energy and sluggish and sleepy behavior are more clearly separable from symptoms of ADHD than items that assess confusion, slow responding, and difficulty completing tasks efficiently. These converging results suggest that future theoretical models of SCT may be improved by focusing on this sluggish/sleepy dimension of behavior.

Neuropsychological Models of ADHD and SCT

The current study is one of the first studies to examine the neuropsychological correlates of SCT, extending our previous work on the neuropsychology of ADHD (e.g., McGrath et al. 2011; Willcutt et al. 2010b). Similar to other complex disorders (e.g., Pennington 2006), SCT and ADHD appear to arise from the combined effects of multiple neuropsychological weaknesses rather than a single primary deficit. As described earlier, slow naming and processing speed may represent a shared weakness that accounts for covariance between SCT and inattention, whereas SCT and inattention are distinguished by differential associations with measures of sustained attention, response inhibition, and response variability.

Several future lines of research may facilitate the continued development and refinement of a comprehensive cognitive model of SCT. Future research should include measures of a broader range of cognitive constructs that may be relevant to SCT. These may potentially include measures of the speed, consistency, and variability of simple reaction time and more complex speeded responses, along with tasks designed to assess sustained attention and other aspects of attentional processing. In addition, future studies of behavioral measures of vigilance, processing speed, and response variability may be further strengthened by incorporating electrophysiological or neuroimaging techniques to more directly measure the neurophysiological correlates of SCT. As a straightforward first step to facilitate this line of research, it may be useful for ongoing or newly initiated neuroimaging studies of related constructs such as ADHD and internalizing disorders to include a brief measure of SCT.

Developmental and Etiological Models

Despite the fact that over 25 years of research has been completed since SCT was first described (Neeper and Lahey 1986), virtually nothing is known about the etiology or developmental course of SCT. Etiologically-informative designs such as family and twin studies and longitudinal studies that track the development of SCT and other symptom dimensions from an early age would provide important new information for theoretical models of SCT.

Other Limitations and Future Directions

Sample

An important limitation of the current study is the fact that the sample was initially selected for ADHD. Results of the factor analyses and the analyses of external validity were similar when analyses were restricted to the comparison group without ADHD, and several key findings are consistent with results reported in an unselected sample of children (Wahlstedt and Bohlin 2010) and in groups of children that were selected independently for elevations of ADHD or SCT (Barkley 2013). Nonetheless, future studies of unselected samples or groups selected directly for SCT would provide a useful extension of the current research.

SCT Item Pool

The initial pool of nine potential SCT items is among the largest that has been analyzed to date, and represents a significant improvement over previous studies for which only two or three SCT symptoms were available (e.g., Carlson and Mann 2002; Frick et al. 1994; McBurnett et al. 2001; see Willcutt et al. 2012 for a detailed review). On the other hand, the six items that loaded on the final SCT scale for the current analyses did not include several additional items that loaded on SCT factors in the most extensive factor analytic studies of SCT symptoms (tired, lethargic, yawning/stretching/sleepy-eyed appearance, low energy, in a world of his/her own; Barkley 2013; Penny et al. 2009). Future studies should also incorporate these symptoms and continue to develop and test new SCT items to continue to improve the measurement model of SCT.

Conclusion

The current results provide strong support for the internal and external validity of SCT. SCT symptoms are elevated in both ADHD-C and ADHD-I, and high levels of SCT do not appear to be a marker for a subgroup of ADHD-I that is distinct from the remainder of cases with DSM-IV ADHD. Instead, the current results suggest that DSM-IV inattention and SCT are correlated but distinct symptom dimension that are each independently associated with important aspects of functional impairment and neuropsychological functioning.