Introduction

Childhood brain tumors (CBT) are the second most common type of childhood cancer after acute leukemia and are associated with a high rate of morbidity and mortality. The known causes of CBT are limited to exposure to ionising radiation in childhood and a few specific genetic syndromes which account for less than 5 % of cases [1]. Many cases of CBT occur in early childhood, indicating that some predisposing or initiating events may occur before conception, during fetal life or in infancy.

The International Agency for Research on Cancer has stated that there is sufficient evidence for the carcinogenicity of X-radiation in humans, including ‘substantial evidence that suggests a causal association between exposure to diagnostic radiation in utero and childhood cancers’ (p168) [2]. Ionising radiation has been shown to increase the risk of brain tumors among children treated for tinea capitis or hemangioma [3]. Children and fetuses may be more susceptible to the DNA-damaging effects of radiation than adults, because of the high rate of cellular proliferation [4].

Two recent cohort studies of exposure to computerised tomography (CT) in childhood and/or early adulthood reported positive associations for brain tumor risk, after follow-up of up to 23 years [5, 6]. In both studies, the increased risks were highest after CTs of the brain and, in the Australian study, for CTs done at younger ages and closer to the time of diagnosis (after excluding a ‘lag period’) [6]. Another smaller cohort study with maximum follow-up to age 15 reported an OR of 0.52 (95 % CI 0.25, 0.95) for any radiological procedure in childhood; only 3.7 % of procedures were CT scans [7].

Among case–control studies of childhood radiological exposure and CBT, three reported little evidence of associations [810], while four others reported at least weak evidence of positive associations, with ORs of 1.5 (95 % CI 0.8, 3.0) [11], 1.5 (95 % CI 0.7, 3.3) [12], 2.2 (95 % CI 0.6, 8.8) [13], and 2.3 (95 % CI 0.91, 5.7) for primitive neuroectodermal tumors (PNETs) after cranial X-rays for reasons other than head injury [14].

As described in two recent reviews [15, 16], most studies of maternal radiological procedures during pregnancy and risk of CBT have reported null results, consistent with studies published since these reviews [7, 10]. Only one study of gestational procedures reported an increased risk, and this was observed for PNET only [17]. The only previous study we identified of parental preconception radiological exposures reported no increased risk for procedures carried out on either parent [11].

The Australian Study of Childhood Brain Tumors (Aus-CBT) was a nationwide case–control study designed to investigate environmental and genetic risk factors for CBT. The aim of the current analysis was to determine whether diagnostic radiological procedures in the parents or the child were associated with an increased risk. We specifically aimed to investigate whether any such association varied by period of exposure and procedure type (and by inference, dose).

To our knowledge, this is the first epidemiological study to investigate the risk of CBT in relation to parental preconception, fetal, and postnatal exposure to specific types of radiological procedures.

Methods

Details of the design and recruitment methods for Aus-CBT have been described previously [18]. Briefly, incident cases of CBT diagnosed between 2005 and 2010 were identified through all 10 pediatric oncology centers in Australia. Where deemed appropriate, the treating clinician invited the parents to participate in the study as soon as possible after diagnosis. Controls were recruited by national random-digit dialing (RDD) and frequency-matched to cases by age (within 1 year), sex, and state of residence in a ratio of 3:1. Controls recruited in 2007–2010 were frequency-matched to incident CBT cases, while those recruited in 2005 and 2006 had been frequency-matched to cases in our concurrent study of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia (Aus-ALL; 2003–2007); the studies used identical RDD recruitment methods [19].

Cases and controls were eligible for inclusion if they resided in Australia and had a biological parent who could complete questionnaires in English. Both studies were approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees at all participating hospitals.

As soon as consent was received, parents were mailed questionnaires requesting detailed information about demographics and relevant exposures, including radiological procedures. Mothers were asked whether the child had undergone any ‘X-rays’ before their diagnosis (for cases) and since birth (for controls). Requested details included the body part/s examined: ‘Head (including dental),’ ‘Chest,’ ‘Abdomen (including stomach/hips),’ ‘Arm(s),’ ‘Legs(s),’ ‘Whole body’; and procedure type: ‘Plain X-ray,’ ‘CAT scan (CT scan),’ ‘IVP (kidney)’ (intravenous pyelogram), ‘Barium study,’ ‘other.’ The child’s age at the time of the procedure was requested in years and months. The reason for the procedure was not ascertained.

Both parents were asked whether they had undergone any radiological procedures before the child was born, and the body parts involved: ‘Pelvis,’ ‘Hips,’ ‘Lower back,’ ‘Abdomen (including stomach),’ ‘Kidneys’; the scan type (options as above), and the age in years when the procedure took place. The body areas chosen were designed to obtain information about procedures that could potentially affect the reproductive organs in the parents, or the fetus in utero. The information provided also enabled exclusion of non-radiological procedures (e.g., MRI, ultrasound) from the analyses.

As previously described, we also had an area-of-residence-based measure of socioeconomic status (SES) for each family who agreed to take part in the study, the Index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage [18].

Exposure metrics

Radiological procedures undertaken on the mother or father at any time before the index pregnancy were identified from the questionnaire data. Maternal procedures during the pregnancy and paternal procedures during the 2 years leading up to the conception of the index child were also identified; paternal procedures after the conception date were excluded. Child exposures were limited to procedures that occurred more than 6 months before the censoring date (the age at diagnosis for cases and the age at questionnaire return for controls), in an attempt to exclude procedures that may have been related to early symptoms of CBT. Sensitivity analyses using 12-month and 5-year lag periods were also performed.

Statistical procedures

Odds ratios and 95 % CIs were estimated using unconditional logistic regression in PASW Statistics for Windows version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, 2009). The matching variables (child’s age, sex, and state of residence) were included in all analytical models. The following variables were investigated as potential confounders as they have been previously reported, by us or others, to be associated with CBT risk, participation, or the exposures under investigation: child’s birth year, diagnosis/recruitment year, child’s ethnicity, parental age, parental alcohol use, parental smoking, maternal pre-pregnancy folate supplementation, parental education, household income, and area-based SES. Variables assessed by visual comparison of distributions as associated both with case–control status and with exposure among controls were included in the models (erring on the side of inclusion). Different covariates were included in the models of the child’s and parents’ radiological exposures, as different variables met the criteria for confounding in each. The covariates included in final statistical models are listed in the footnotes of Tables 2 and 3. Linear trends were tested for with a Wald test for ordinal categorical variables entered into models with 1 degree of freedom.

Results

Full recruitment details and participation rates have been published previously [18]. Briefly, 730 eligible CBT cases diagnosed between 2005 and 2010 were identified, 568 of whom were invited to participate by their treating physician; the physician opted not to invite the remaining 162 cases for medical or psychosocial reasons. Parents of 374 cases consented to participate (65.8 % of invited, 51.2 % of eligible). The age and sex distributions among invited and uninvited cases were similar: mean age 6.9 versus 6.1 (respectively), and 58.3 versus 55.6 % were boys (respectively). Similarly, the age and sex distributions among cases who consented and those who did not were: mean age 6.8 versus 7.2 (respectively), and 58.6 versus 57.7 % were boys (respectively).

In the same time period, 3,624 eligible control children were identified by RDD, of whom 2,255 (62.2 %) agreed to participate; in accordance with our frequency-matching quotas, 1,467 children were recruited. For ethical reasons, no information was available about potential controls that declined to participate.

Information on radiological procedures was provided via questionnaire by 302 case mothers (81 % of those recruited), 247 case fathers (66 % of recruited), 941 control mothers (61 % of recruited), and 801 control fathers (55 % of recruited). The distributions of child’s age and sex were similar among questionnaire completers and non-completers. Among both cases and controls, however, completers had higher area-based SES scores than non-completers (p < 0.01) and, in cases but not controls, completers were born 2 years earlier, on average, than non-completers (p < 0.01).

The demographic characteristics of families who had returned any written questionnaire are shown in Table 1. Because controls for cases diagnosed in 2005 and 2006 had been matched to cases from our study of ALL, there were proportionally more female controls and fewer controls aged over 10 years, compared with case children. Controls were more likely than cases to be recruited in 2005–2006, have older and more highly educated parents, and be of European ethnicity.

Table 1 Distribution of demographic and birth characteristics

The OR for having any diagnostic radiological procedures during childhood was 0.66 (95 % CI 0.48, 0.90), with an apparent inverse trend (p for trend = 0.009) (Table 2). The ORs for X-ray examinations, including those to the head or the chest, were below the null. The ORs for CT scans were also below the null, although they tended to be higher than those for X-rays and were less precise as a result of relatively small numbers. When the lag period between the last exposure and the censoring date was increased from 6 to 12 months and 5 years, ORs for any radiological procedure during childhood changed little: 0.59 (95 % CI 0.43, 0.83) and 0.61 (95 % CI 0.39, 0.97), respectively. Our findings did not vary by age at exposure (data not shown). There was little evidence of any interaction between parental or filial radiological procedures and age at diagnosis/recruitment (data not shown). The results for childhood exposures were fairly consistent across CBT subtypes, although the OR for any childhood radiological exposure and high-grade glioma was 1.71 (95 % CI 0.68, 4.30; Table 4). No case children and only 14 control children had a barium study, and only 3 cases and 14 controls had an IVP (data not shown).

Table 2 Childhood exposure to diagnostic radiological procedures and the risk of CBT

There was essentially no evidence of any increased risk of CBT associated with maternal or paternal exposure to radiological procedures at any time before the index pregnancy; in fact, the ORs for maternal pre-pregnancy exposures were all below the null, with an apparent inverse trend (Table 3). The ORs for paternal exposure were all close to the null: 0.89 (95 % CI 0.64, 1.24) for any time prior to conception (Table 3) and 1.15 (95 % CI 0.60, 2.21) for procedures in the 2 years before the child’s conception (results not tabulated).

Table 3 Parental exposure to diagnostic radiological procedures at any time before the index pregnancy and the risk of CBT

The effect estimates for childhood and parental radiological procedures in the CBT subtype analyses generally lacked precision (Table 4).

Table 4 Childhood and parental diagnostic radiological procedure by CBT subtype

The overall OR for fetal in utero exposure (based on eight exposed cases and 14 exposed controls) was 1.71 (95 % CI 0.69, 4.23) (results not tabulated). There were too few procedures carried out during pregnancy to investigate fetal exposure further.

Discussion

We found no evidence of positive associations between risk of CBT overall and diagnostic radiological procedures during childhood or in either parent in the period leading up to the child’s conception. Weak evidence of an increased risk was seen only for maternal procedures during pregnancy, and for childhood procedures and risk of high-grade gliomas, but these results were based on small numbers and may be due to chance.

Unexpectedly, most of our results for childhood and maternal pre-pregnancy radiological procedures were suggestive of inverse associations with risk, and decreasing trends were observed for all procedures combined. An inverse association was also seen for childhood exposure in a small German cohort study that included 10 CBT cases [7]. However, two large cohort studies have found childhood CT scans are associated with increased brain tumor risk in childhood and early adulthood [5, 6], and previous case–control studies of CBT have generally reported null or positive results [814]; therefore, we believe our results are unlikely to reflect true protective effects.

That we did not see increased risks associated with childhood CTs like those reported in two recent cohort studies [5, 6] might be explained by the different calendar periods during which the majority of scans were done. As in the UK, CT scan doses reduced considerably in Australia from 2001 because of the more frequent use of age-specific dose settings [6]. For example, the current UK dose estimate for a CT brain scan on a 5-year-old child is 28 mGy—approximately one-half to one-third of the dose used before 2001 [5]. In our study, more than two-thirds of the CTs for children were done from 2001 onwards, while approximately two-thirds of CTs in the Australian cohort study [6], and almost all CTs from the UK study [5] were done before 2001. Thus, lower post-2001 dose levels may explain, at least in part, the lack of evidence of an increased risk in our study. In addition, the two cohort studies included tumors that developed in early adulthood.

The fact that most previous studies of radiological procedures during childhood have reported null or positive associations with CBT suggests the inverse associations we observed between use of radiological procedures and CBT may be due to bias or uncontrolled confounding. However, we have not identified any obvious candidate biases or confounders. Both cases and controls in our study had higher area-based SES scores than the average for Australia [18], but scores among participating controls were similar to those for cases. Further, although SES was related to study participation, it was not associated with radiological procedures for the child or the mother, and only marginally associated with area-based SES for the father. In addition, all analyses were adjusted for factors meeting the criteria for confounding. It is therefore unlikely that selection bias or confounding had a major impact on our results. If case parents had recalled exposures more fully than control parents, the ORs would tend to be inflated rather than reduced; therefore, it is unlikely that our findings are attributable to recall bias. Similarly, the use of a 6-month lag period would not necessarily exclude confounding by indication. However, such an influence would lead to inflated rather than reduced ORs, and extending the lag period to 12 months and then 5 years did not change the observed associations; thus, confounding by indication is unlikely to explain our findings. Apart from chance, notwithstanding the low p value, alternative explanations for these findings are difficult to identify.

Few mothers (8 cases and 14 controls) in our study underwent radiological procedures during pregnancy, so the effect estimate was imprecise: OR 1.71 (95 % CI 0.69, 4.23). A Swedish study [17] reported an increased OR only for PNET: 1.88 (95 % CI 0.92–3.83). The great majority of recent studies have reported null results [15]. Thus, this increased OR may be due to recall bias if case mothers had better recall than control mothers when considering the causes of their child’s disease. Recall bias is unlikely in the Swedish study; however, as data were extracted from medical records, but the estimates were based on only 16 exposed cases. In older studies where positive associations were seen for gestational procedures [20, 21], more than 10 % of mothers were exposed, and radiological doses were higher than those currently used.

Consistent with our results for radiological procedures in the parents prior to the child’s conception, Shu et al. [11] found no association for paternal procedures, but some evidence of an inverse association for maternal procedures: OR 0.6 (95 % CI 0.3, 1.2) among mothers who had two or more X-rays in the 2 years before conception. Bunin et al. [8] reported reduced ORs for paternal CT and risk of astrocytoma and PNET. These findings are imprecise and may be due to chance; alternative explanations are not clear.

Our study is only the second to investigate a variety of radiological procedures in fathers as well as mothers and children. We were also able to conduct preliminary analyses by procedure type, body area scanned, and CBT subtype, although the power for these sub-analyses was limited, particularly for in utero exposure because of the relative infrequency with which radiological procedures are carried out during pregnancy [22].

Like all case–control studies involving self-report, our findings are subject to recall error. However, our questionnaires were designed to aid accurate recall, with explicit prompts regarding the procedure type, body part, and timing; thus, we were able to reduce misclassification by excluding non-radiological procedures (e.g., ultrasounds/MRIs), those potentially related to the child’s CBT diagnosis, and paternal procedures occurring after the child’s conception.

In conclusion, we found no evidence that diagnostic radiological procedures during childhood—including CTs—increase the risk of CBT, with the possible exception of high-grade gliomas. This may be due to the relatively lower exposures from CTs done since 2001 or to currently unexplained bias in our results. Similarly, although there is little evidence of an increased risk of CBT with fetal in utero exposure to radiological procedures at current doses, more research is needed to establish the safety of more intense procedures like CT scans.