Introduction

Late-onset glycogenosis type II (GSD II), also named late-onset Pompe disease or glycogen storage disease type II (GSD II; OMIM #232300), is a rare, autosomal-recessive metabolic disorder caused by an accumulation of glycogen in the lysosome due to deficiency of the l-acid alpha-glucosidase enzyme (van der Ploeg et al. 2010). A number of mutations in both copies of the acid alpha-glucosidase (GAA) gene have been reported, with varying degrees of GAA deficiency. In late-onset GSD II, some residual enzymatic activity (from 1 % to 40 %) is usually present compared with infantile onset in which it is totally absent (van der Ploeg et al. 2010). Compared with the infantile form, late-onset GSD II is more slowly progressive and is clinically characterized by muscle weakness, predominantly in proximal lower limbs, variable respiratory involvement, and generally with spared heart function (American Association of Neuromuscular and Electrodiagnostic Medicine 2009; Kishnani et al. 2006; Wokke et al. 2008). Even though skeletal and smooth muscles are mainly involved, other tissues may be affected by the disease process (Braunsdorf 1987; Laforet et al. 2008). Glycogen storage is also autoptically present in the brain, brainstem, and anterior horns (Di Rocco et al. 2007; Raben et al. 2002). Accordingly, a few reports in cases of infantile-onset disease have highlighted the presence of delayed cerebral myelinization (Chien et al. 2006; Ebbink et al. 2012) and a variable degree of cognitive development ranging from normal to mild retardation (Ebbink et al. 2012; Spiridigliozzi et al. 2012). However, no systematic research on this issue has been carried out, and no data in late-onset GSD II are yet available. Therefore, it remains unknown whether—and if so, which—cognitive and brain changes may occur in these patients.

Imaging genetics is a rapidly emerging field that is opening up a new landscape of discovery in neuroscience (Thompson et al. 2010). In this context, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has become an increasingly popular tool for human brain investigation in vivo. MRI has the unique ability to provide quantitative information on both brain-tissue structure and functioning. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) is currently regarded as a robust MR technique suitable for assessing structural gray matter (GM) modifications in an unbiased fashion (Borroni et al. 2008). On the other hand, resting-state functional MRI (RS-fMRI) has shown the ability to provide measures of functional brain connectivity (De Luca et al. 2006; Fox and Raichle 2007). Functional connectivity is a concept based on evidence that different brain regions present with synchronous patterns of activity at rest. Those regions are likely to be part of common networks subserving complex brain functions. From RS-fMRI data (i.e., fMRI time series collected while patients lie vigilant but at rest in the scanner), several networks can be extracted at the same time in a data-driven fashion using independent component analysis (Greicius et al. 2003). The default-mode network (DMN) is by far the most extensively studied network. This is believed to be relevant for specific higher-level functions, such as the working memory, mind wandering, and goal-directed behaviors (Fox and Raichle 2007). The so-called salience network (SN) is another extensively studied RS-fMRI component, which is believed to be particularly informative when investigating executive functions (Zhou et al. 2010).

SN is built around paralimbic structures, most prominently, the dorsal anterior cingulate and anterior insula, which underlie interoceptive–autonomic processing to guide behavior (Mesulam 1998; Seeley et al. 2007). These regions coactivate in response to emotion (Craig 2009, 2002), pain (Peyron et al. 2000) and are active in cognitive and homeostatic control (Menon and Uddin 2010). So far, SN is considered to be related to socially and emotionally relevant information processing (Seeley et al. 2007).

In this work, we aimed at evaluating whether—and if so, which—brain changes occur in patients with late-onset GSD II using VBM and RS-fMRI. We hypothesized that central nervous system (CNS) involvement is a common feature of the disease. We further corroborated neuroimaging data with a careful assessment of neuropsychological performances.

Methods

Study participants

Participants entering this study were recruited at the Neurology Unit, University of Brescia, Italy, from September 2008 to September 2012. Patients were selected by three inclusion criteria: (a) confirmed diagnosis of GSD II, either by reduced GAA activity <35 % of controls in skeletal muscle, leukocytes, or fibroblasts; or by confirmed pathogenetic mutations in the GAA gene; (b) adult age at disease onset; (c) presence of signs or symptoms of the disease (e.g., at least grade 1 on the Walton scale) (American Association of Neuromuscular and Electrodiagnostic Medicine 2009; Bembi et al. 2008). All patients underwent a standardized neuropsychological evaluation and neurological assessment, genetic analysis, and alpha 1,4 glucosidase activity measurement. To characterize pathogenetic mutations within the GAA gene, DNA was isolated from whole blood samples and direct sequencing of the entire coding regions (20 exons) as well as intronic flaking sequences performed with standard procedures. Nucleotides are numbered according to the GeneBank Accession N° NM_000152. GAA dosage in lymphocytes was performed as previously reported (Jack et al. 2006). A group of 18 age- and gender-matched healthy controls (patients:controls 1:2) were included for MRI analyses (mean age 48.0 ± 9.8; 40 % male). Each control was screened for global cognitive functions, which were within normal range; MRI scan was unremarkable. Written informed consent was obtained for each procedure before study initiation, including MRI scanning. The work conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Neuropsychological assessment

A neuropsychological battery of tests was administered to each patient by trained neuropsychologists 48 h before acquisition of the MRI. It included a general cognitive evaluation using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975) and tests specific for each cognitive domain: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Berg 1948) as a measure of reasoning ability and shifting cognitive strategies; Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop 1935) as a general measure of cognitive flexibility and control; Controlled Oral Word Association Test and Category Fluency Test (Isaacs and Kennie 1973) as a measures of verbal fluency; Trail Making Test Parts A and B (Reitan 1955) as measures of executive functions; Clock Drawing Test as a measure of several skills, including visuospatial construction ability and executive functioning (Sunderland et al. 1989). For each administered test, appropriate adjustments for age and education were applied according to Italian normative data. Further, scores were expressed as equivalent scores (ES) (Capitani and Laiacona 1997); ES is a 5-point scale (from 0–4, worst to best). We considered ES ≤1 as poor performances and ES ≥2 as good performances.

MRI acquisition and analysis

All imaging was obtained using a 1.5-T MR scanner (Siemens Symphony, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a circularly polarized transmit–receive coil. In a single session, the following scans were collected from each studied individual: (1) dual-echo turbo spin-echo (TSE) [repetition time (TR) = 2,500 ms, echo time (TE) = 50 ms] to exclude the presence of macroscopic brain abnormalities, according to exclusion criteria; (2) 3D magnetization-prepared rapid-gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted scan [TR = 2,010 ms, TE = 3.93 ms, matrix = 1 × 1 × 1, in-plane field of view (FOV) = 250 × 250 mm2, slice thickness = 1 mm, flip angle = 15°]; and (3) T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (EPI) sensitized to blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast [TR = 2,500 ms, TE = 50 ms, 29 axial slices parallel to anterior commissure–posterior commissure line (AC-PC) line, matrix = 64 × 64, field of view = 224 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm] for RS-fMRI. BOLD EPI images were collected during rest for an 8-min period, resulting in a total of 195 volumes. During this acquisition, participants were instructed to keep their eyes closed, not to think of anything in particular, and not to fall asleep. Dual-echo TSE images were carefully reviewed to exclude the presence of signs suggestive of cerebrovascular disease, and T1-weighted images from all participants were visually inspected for qualitative assessment to check data quality before carrying out a quantitative volumetric analysis. Intracranial mass was considered an exclusion criterion for further analysis of MRI scans.

Voxel-based morphometry

MP-RAGE data were processed using the VBM protocol in Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). For each participant, an iterative combination of segmentations and normalizations (implemented within the Segment SPM8 module) produced a GM probability map (Ashburner and Friston 2005) in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates. To compensate for compression or expansion during warping of images to match the template, GM maps were modulated by multiplying the intensity of each voxel by the local value derived from the deformation field (Jacobian determinants) (Ashburner and Friston 2001). All data were then smoothed using a 12-mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Modulated and smoothed GM were analyzed in SPM8 using a full factorial design. Age and gender were added as covariates of no interest. GM map analysis was also adjusted for total intracranial volume (ICV = GM volume + white-matter volume + cerebrospinal fluid volume). Contrasts were designed to assess the effect of glycogenosis type II in regional GM volumes by comparing patients with controls. P values were accepted as statistically significant for p values family-wise error (FWE) cluster level corrected inferior to 0.05.

fMRI data analysis

RS-fMRI data were analyzed using SPM8 for image preprocessing and statistical comparison, and the Group Independent Component Analysis (ICA) for fMRI Toolbox (GIFT, icatb.sourceforge.net/) for network identification. For each participant, the first four volumes of the fMRI series were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. The preprocessing steps included correction for head motion, compensation for slice-dependent time shifts, normalization to the EPI template in Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates provided with SPM8, and smoothing with a 3D Gaussian kernel with 8-mm3 FWHM. Then, all images were filtered by a phase-insensitive bandpass filter (pass band 0.01–0.08 Hz) to reduce the effect of low-frequency drift and high-frequency physiological noise. ICA analysis was employed to identify 20 independent components. Briefly, group ICA for fMRI Toolbox first concatenates the individual data across time and then produces a computation of participant-specific components and time courses. For all participants grouped together, Toolbox performed the analysis in three steps: (1) data reduction, (2) application of the FastICA algorithm, and (3) back-reconstruction for each individual (De Luca et al. 2006). Results were converted to Z scores. The 20 components were reviewed and compared by computing the spatial correlation coefficient to customized templates of the SN and DMN. These templates were obtained from an independent sample of 28 healthy controls [13 women, mean age 38.6 years; standard deviation (SD) 7.5 years]. This procedure was performed using the tool for spatial sorting of the components available with GIFT. Every individual’s Z-score maps corresponding to these two resting-state networks were used for cross-subject analyses. Differences between patients and controls were considered in each network. Age and gender were entered as covariates of no interest. P values were accepted as statistically significant for p values FWE cluster level corrected inferior to 0.05.

Results

Patients

Demographic characteristics of GSD II patients are reported in Table 1. Patients had a mean age of 46.6 (SD = 8), 55.5 % were men, and mean age at disease onset was 31.6 (SD = 12.6). The most common mutation, c-32 13 T/G (IVS1), was found in all patients In regard to the other allele, four patients were 2237G/A mutation carriers, three had 525delT genetic variation, two were delexon18 carriers, and one had 1927G/A mutation. Endogenous GAA activity levels from lymphocytes ranged from 2 % to 25 %. All patients except one were in treatment with enzymatic replacement therapy (therapy initiation range 1–5 years).

Table 1 Demographic and genetic features of patients with glycogenosis type II

Neuropsychological assessment

A number of patients with late-onset GSD II reported pathological performances in the WCST, a test assessing strategic planning, organized searching, utilizing environmental feedback to shift cognitive sets, and modulating impulsive responding (see Table 2). As shown in Fig. 1, when ES were considered, 66.7 % of patients showed poor performances (≤1) at WCST global score and at nonperseverative errors of WCST, whereas 55.5 % of patients showed impaired perseverative errors at WCST. No significant deficits (>50 % of patients) in the other tests were reported.

Table 2 Neuropsychological assessment in patients with late-onset glycogenosis type II
Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentage of patients with poor cognitive performances: equivalent scores (ES) ≤1, indicative of poor performances (see text for details). MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, Phon. phonemic, Sem semantic, TM trail making, IT interference time, IE interference errors, WCST Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, tot global score, per perseverative errors, nper nonperseverative errors

Visual inspection of T1-weighted and T2-FLAIR images

T1-weighted images did not reveal any macroscopic abnormality and according to experienced radiologist were indistinguishable from those of healthy controls of comparable age. In this series of patients, ectasia of the basilar artery in two cases was reported, as already documented in the literature (Laforet et al. 2008). In eight of nine patients with GSD II, no alterations were found on fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images were found; in one patient, a chronic ischemia event with no clinical relevance was detected.

VBM

There was no significant difference in regional GM volumes between patients and age-matched controls at the pre-established threshold.

RS-fMRI

Among the 20 components modelled in the ICA analysis, several well-known resting-state networks were identified, including the SN and the DMN.

Analysis of the SN revealed a significantly reduced functional connectivity in patients compared with controls. As shown in Fig. 2 and Table 3, these clusters of reduced functional connectivity were localized in the middle frontal lobe, bilaterally, and in the cingulate gyrus.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Reduced salience network (SN) connectivity in late-onset glycogenosis type II patients compared with healthy controls. Results are superimposed on a 3D brain template. L left. P < 0.001 uncorrected, only cluster survived at family-wise error (FWE) < 0.05 cluster level were reported. Threshold = 100 voxels

Table 3 Reduced brain connectivity in the salience (SN) and default-mode (DMN) networks in late-onset glycogenosis type II patients compared with healthy controls

The inverse correlation (i.e. controls < patients) showed no cluster above the pre-established threshold in the SN. Analysis of the DMN showed a reduced connectivity in patients with GSD II compared with controls in the left caudate. Again, the inverse correlation (i.e., controls < patients) showed no cluster above the pre-established threshold in the DMN. No significant correlation was found between functional connectivity within the SN and WCST, residual enzymatic activity, and disease duration.

Discussion

In the last 30 years, a growing number of patients with late-onset GSD II has been described (Winkel et al. 2005) and the clinical picture carefully detailed. Late-onset GSD II is a multisystem disorder primarily characterized by muscle weakness but with other emerging signs and symptoms that need further attention to counteract disease progression properly. In this study, we aimed at evaluating whether GSD II may also be considered a brain disease, as there is evidence at autopsy of glycogen accumulation in the brain (Di Rocco et al. 2007; Raben et al. 2002). We therefore performed a comprehensive study of brain structure and function using VBM and RS-fMRI. Our neuroimaging findings along with neuropsychological assessment confirmed the main hypotheses of our work.

When looking at differences in regional brain atrophy, VBM analysis revealed no significant cluster above the pre-established threshold, in line with autopsy studies, which reported no evidence of cortical atrophy (van der Walt et al. 1987). Conversely, significant changes in connectivity of the explored functional brain networks were reported. Patients with GSD II showed SN disruption. This network is implicated in executive functioning, planning, and abstract reasoning, and is consistent with findings of the neuropsychological profile. Indeed, disconnection of cortical regions is a potential mechanism for cognitive dysfunction in various neurological disorders (Dineen et al. 2009) and may also be responsible for the reported impairment at WCST, as this test maps frontal-lobe functions. Despite the absence of direct associations between cognitive data and changes in functional connectivity within the SN, which is likely due to the relative low number of patients, we can speculate that there is a direct correlation between cognitive impairment and fMRI findings. Indeed, we applied two tests that are able to detect dysfunctions of the frontal lobe—namely, the WCST and Stroop tests—and found impaired performances in the former. Indeed, this mismatch may be explained by differences in the explored functions. The WCST is more complex and evaluates set shifting abilities, cognitive flexibility, and problem solving, whereas the second measures selective attention and the ability to inhibit automatic responses when necessary.

The relationship between our current neuroimaging and cognitive findings and the molecular mechanisms underlying neuronal firing patterns are complex and still not elucidated. Few studies on glycogen accumulation in the human brain are available as yet (Di Rocco et al. 2007; Taksir et al. 2007), and results of our study suggest that future work is warranted to increase our understanding of this issue. For the first time, our work reported here extends the clinical spectrum of this multisystem disorder and might provide a model by which to characterize in vivo the brain changes occurring in patients with GSD II. Indeed, recent studies suggest that nonspecific demyelinating white-matter changes in infantile-onset disease were observed (Chien et al. 2006). Accordingly, experimental mouse models of Pompe disease have widely confirmed the presence of glycogen accumulation in the CNS (Taksir et al. 2007).

The association between glycogen accumulation and the selective disruption of the SN found in our study needs to be further investigated. Furthermore, even without a clear stringent correlation with clinical symptomatology, DMN alterations also should be considered in future studies. Indeed, we found RS-fMRI brain abnormalities without grey-matter atrophy, suggesting functional disruption of neuronal networks without structural changes. These findings might have potential consequences on the current treatment approach (Angelini et al. 2012). Enzyme replacement therapy has a beneficial impact upon muscle manifestations of late-onset disease, but the enzyme does not cross the blood–brain barrier and cannot overcome CNS changes (Ebbink et al. 2012; Begley et al. 2008). Nevertheless, a previous study indicated that long-term enzyme replacement therapy ameliorated white-matter changes in infantile-onset disease (Chien et al. 2006). Therefore, the effect of current pharmacological treatment in covering CNS manifestations needs to be further investigated.

This study has some limitations: First, as this is a rare disorder, the sample size is relatively small. We overcame this limitation by applying stringent statistical analysis corrected for multiple comparisons. In the same way, we were unable to detect associations between measures of global cognition and the strength of connectivity in the evaluated functional networks. Second, a more detailed cognitive assessment might be of help to evaluate in depth the subtle cognitive deficits and to further explore executive functions. Third, a comprehensive, in-depth work evaluating white-matter abnormalities is desirable. Future studies on larger samples evaluating the effect of different mutations and different rates of GAA deficiency are needed to clarify whether and how the human brain may cope with glycogen accumulation.