Introduction

Most sexually active people can remember at least one incident of engaging in a sexual activity they later regretted (e.g., Dickson, Paul, Herbison, & Silva, 1998; Galperin et al., 2013; Oswalt, Cameron, & Koob, 2005) such as, for example, infidelity, unprotected and/or unsafe intercourse, or an episode in which one has gone “too far” too early. In the heat of the moment, we obviously make decisions that are not always in line with our moral ideals and behavioral standards valued in less “hot” states. Empirical findings indicate that increased sexual desire leads to an underestimation of the dangers of sexual risk behavior (Blanton & Gerrard, 1997; Ditto, Pizarro, Epstein, Jacobson, & MacDonald, 2006). Also, acute sexual arousal boosts other sexually disinhibited behaviors in men, including a broadened range of sexual interest and a greater willingness to engage in sexually coercive behavior (Ariely & Loewenstein, 2006). In the present study, we sought to replicate and expand Ariely and Loewenstein’s findings by (1) reproducing sexual arousal effects under more controlled laboratory settings, (2) testing whether the effects applied to women, and (3) exploring whether the effects of sexual arousal were specific to sexual disinhibition or would generalize to non-sexual disinhibited behavior.

Surprisingly, little scholarly research has experimentally manipulated sexual arousal to test its effect on outcome variables such as disinhibition, evaluation, and decision making. This stands in stark contrast to a large amount of research elucidating either the very phenomenon of sexual arousal and its time course (e.g., Janssen, Everaerd, Spiering, & Janssen, 2000; O’Donohue & Geer, 1985; Rosen & Beck, 1988; Singer, 1984) or physiological and subjective sexual arousal as an outcome, that is, a dependent variable (Adams, Wright, & Lohr, 1996; Barbaree, Blanchard, & Langton, 2003; Chivers & Bailey, 2005; Geer & Fuhr, 1976).

Regarding the effects of sexual arousal, many studies have measured subjective or genital sexual arousal and found that individual differences in sexual arousal in response to arousing stimuli were associated with individual differences in the self-reported readiness to engage in unprotected sex (Abbey, Saenz, & Buck, 2005; Norris et al., 2009) or differences in self-estimated likelihood to sexually aggress in men (Davis, Norris, George, Martell, & Heiman, 2006a). Measured sexual arousal has also been found to mediate the effects of alcohol on the likelihood of agreeing to unprotected sex in men and women (George et al., 2009; Prause, Staley, & Finn, 2011), women’s rape myth acceptance (Davis, Norris, George, Martell, & Heiman, 2006b), and men’s self-rated likelihood to sexually aggress (Davis et al., 2006a). Although these studies provide important insight regarding the dynamics of sexual decision making, it is unclear whether acute sexual arousal or individual differences in arousability (or any other third variable) are the causal factors. To further exemplify this limitation, consider the finding that men’s sexual arousal in response to a rape scenario predicted their self-rated likelihood to behave like the aggressor (Davis et al., 2006a). While it may very well be that this proneness to become sexually aggressive is directly caused by the arousal, it is equally plausible that some men are less inhibited in their sexual responding by depictions of non-consensual sex and that these a priori differences determined the stronger arousal as well as the self-reported likelihood to commit sexually aggressive acts.

It is, therefore, important to add to this literature by experimentally inducing sexual arousal in order to elucidate its causal impact on decision making. Unfortunately, previous studies that have experimentally manipulated sexual arousal of men and women have sometimes failed to find clear main effects of arousal (Macapagal, Janssen, Fridberg, Finn, & Heiman, 2011; Ruiz-Díaz, Hernández-González, Guevara, Amezcua, & Ågmo, 2012). Experimentally induced sexual arousal, however, has been shown to increase general approach behavior (Both, Spiering, Everaerd, & Laan, 2004) and to affect the self-estimated likelihood of engaging in various sexual activities (Ariely & Loewenstein, 2006), a study particularly relevant for the present research. Ariely and Loewenstein instructed men either to masturbate (to sexually explicit imagery presented on screen) or not to masturbate (no images on the screen) while—at the same time—responding to questions from three broad sexually relevant categories: the attractiveness of different sexual activities, the lengths the participant would go to in order to obtain sexual gratification, and the participant’s attitude toward sexual risks. Parsimoniously, these three domains might be described as examples of sexual disinhibition,Footnote 1 defined as (hypothetical) behavior that is more instrumental to gratify personal desires than to conform with perceived social rules (i.e., social desirability). In line with these assumptions, sexual arousal indeed strengthened the sexual appetitive system: Across all three domains, sexually aroused participants estimated their own likelihood to engage in the respective activities as greater than non-aroused participants did. Despite the fact that Ariely and Loewenstein’s study clearly constituted an important pioneering work, it had methodological limitations that we sought to address.

Although it was never explicitly mentioned in the article, it seems that participants in the original study received laptops with the computer experiment to take home before they “returned the laptop to the experimenter on the following day” (Ariely & Loewenstein, 2006, p. 91). Although this form of data collection might be seen as particularly ecologically valid, it is also at the same time a liability that the experimenters had no control over the situation in which the participants completed the study. A second methodological problem is that the task was much more complex in the sexual arousal condition than in the non-aroused condition. Non-aroused participants merely responded to questions by moving a response slider using two keys on a keypad. In contrast, participants in the sexual arousal condition continuously had to navigate between three different screen panels to change sexual stimuli, rate their arousal, and respond to the questions while at the same time continuously masturbating to keep their sexual arousal at a certain predefined level. The fact that the task in the aroused condition—in all likelihood—was more taxing than the task in the non-aroused condition not only decreases comparability between the experimental conditions, but also leaves room for alternative explanations of the results, as we will outline below.

Potential Alternative Explanations

Ariely and Loewenstein (2006) based their theoretical rationale that sexual arousal strengthens the sexual appetitive system on the more general idea that opportunities for need satisfaction (as implied by delicious food or sexually arousing stimuli) strengthen the motivation to satisfy those respective needs (Rolls, 1999). Although their results were fully in line with this idea, we argue that a similar pattern could be expected based on two alternative explanations of the sexual arousal effect: general disinhibition and/or mere indifferent responding (i.e., giving responses more or less randomly).

A general disinhibition account refers to the possibility that sexual arousal might put participants in a state of reduced inhibition, independent of the inhibition domain (e.g., inhibition of sexual impulses, but also other activities that require either resistance to short-term temptation, like impulsive eating or smoking, or a mobilization of effort to reach long-term goals, like working out or studying). We have argued above that the items used by Ariely and Loewenstein were likely to yield stronger agreement in a state of disinhibition, that is, in “a state in which people do not or only weakly care about what others think of their actions” (van den Bos et al., 2011, p. 794). It is hence conceivable that the observed effects were not due to a strengthening of the appetitive system specifically in terms of sexual approach motivations as argued by Ariely and Loewenstein, but to a weakened relevance of social desirability norms. In a state of sexual approach motivation toward active sexual behavior it may indeed be dysfunctional to elaborate too much on how one’s actions would be perceived and evaluated by others.

Related to this notion, it could be further argued that answering in accordance with social desirability norms requires self-regulatory resources. The more taxing navigation in the sexual arousal condition in all likelihood also consumed cognitive resources and, thus, might have weakened participants’ ability to act in such a self-regulatory manner. Notably, this account received support from the fact that, even in the original study, sexual arousal also increased non-sexual risk-taking (see Ariely & Loewenstein, 2006, footnote 2, p. 90), an outcome commonly seen as an indicator of self-regulatory depletion (Freeman & Muraven, 2010; Imhoff, Schmidt, & Gerstenberg, 2013b, Study 2).

A second, more critical alternative—not necessarily mutually exclusive—explanation could be based on mere indifferent responding. It is conceivable that individuals did not pay close attention to the questions and clicked their response in a more or less random way. On average, non-aroused participants gave responses that were below the midpoint of the 100-point scale (across all items for which an increase under sexual arousal was expected: M = 36.34). In contrast, sexually aroused participants gave responses around the midpoint on the same items (M = 52.55). If non-aroused participants truthfully responded to the questions and (at least some) aroused participants showed more indifferent responding, the same pattern might appear. Sexually aroused participants might have had a decreased motivation to elaborate on the questions due to either sexual disinhibition or the more taxing navigation task and thus might have been less inclined to fine-tune their answers (i.e., clicked more indiscriminately).

Gender Specificity

Previous work has primarily focused on men, as does a large portion of sex research, potentially because men have a higher sex drive (Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001) and are also more specific in their sexual responding than (at least heterosexual) women (Chivers, Rieger, Latty, & Bailey, 2004). Given that there are fundamental differences in sexual behavior and functioning (e.g., Galperin et al., 2013) that has led some to propose fundamentally different sexual cognitions for men and women (Ogas & Gaddam, 2012), we aimed to address these potentially different effects of sexual arousal empirically. Although it may be true that the pathways to the experience of sexual arousal are fundamentally different for men and women (Ogas & Gaddam, 2012), the consequences might still be the same. Replicating the pattern of sexual disinhibition under sexual arousal not only for men but also in women would constitute an important example of a universal principle of human sexual arousal.

The Present Research

We thus sought to replicate the findings of Ariely and Loewenstein (2006) in a more controlled laboratory setting, inducing sexual arousal using auditory stimuli that were pretested to be normatively arousing to the majority of participants. As a second alteration, we included men and women to test whether women show a similar effect of sexual arousal on sexual disinhibition. More importantly, we sought to critically address alternative explanations: By including measures of non-sexual disinhibition, we tested the specificity of the sexual arousal effects. If the results reported by Ariely and Loewenstein were due to increased general disinhibition or a weakening of self-regulatory capacities, we would expect an effect of sexual arousal on non-sexual disinhibition. Likewise, if sexual arousal merely led to more indiscriminate responding, we would expect an effect on any item that has a mean score below the mid-point of the scale for non-aroused participants, such as, for example, items assessing non-sexual disinhibited behavior.

Pilot Study

We translated eight items from Ariely and Loewenstein (2006) to tap into sexual disinhibition and purpose-designed eight items to tap into non-sexual disinhibition. To provide empirical support for our notion that the type of items employed in Ariely and Loewenstein and our newly generated non-sexual disinhibition control measures were indeed reflective of disinhibition, we conducted a pilot study. Closely aligned with our above-mentioned definition of disinhibition, participants in the pilot study were asked to respond to these items as they would normally respond (control condition) and with the instruction to respond as if they did not care or only weakly cared about what others thought of them (disinhibition condition). Higher scores in the disinhibition condition could be interpreted as the degree to which the items tapped into disinhibition.

Method

Participants

We recruited 101 individuals (51 men, 50 women) for a study on hypothetical behavior via the crowdsourcing marketplace Amazon Mechanical Turk. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 61 years (M = 30.3, SD = 9.4) and received 25 cents for their participation. The majority of participants self-identified as White (80.2 %) and had a university degree (52.5 %).

Procedure

All participants first provided demographic information and indicated whether they were primarily sexually oriented toward men or women. They were asked to complete 16 hypothetical questions (the sex of sexual partner in sexual disinhibition items was chosen in accordance with participants’ self-identified sexual orientation) with two different instructions that were worded to be consistent with previous experimental manipulations of disinhibition (van den Bos, Müller, & van Bussel, 2009). One of the instructions read, “We are interested in how you would answer the following questions if you were to answer them like you would normally respond when you care about what others think of you” (control condition) whereas the other read “We are interested in how you would answer the following questions if you were to answer them with no inhibitions, that is, as if you do not or only weakly care about what others think of you” (disinhibition condition). The 16 questions within each condition were presented in fully randomized order. All participants completed both conditions but the order was counter-balanced across participants. To make sure that participants paid attention to the different instructions, they received a full-page warning between the two item lists (“Attention! On the following page you will be asked to respond to the same questions again but in a different manner. Please read the instruction carefully”).

Measures

Dependent Variables

We translated eight items from the original items employed by Ariely and Loewenstein (2006). Specifically, we selected items on which the sexual arousal manipulation in the original article (masturbation) had produced the largest mean differences (greater than 20 scale points difference between aroused and non-aroused participants) and that were equally appropriate for men and women (i.e., no explicit date rape items). We purpose-designed analogous items to tap into non-sexual disinhibition, that is, the self-estimated likelihood of engaging in or endorsing behavior that is usually inhibited due to social desirability concerns. All items are shown in Table 1 and were completed on a continuous slider scale ranging from “No” to “Yes.” The positions on the scale represented numerical equivalents not visible to participants ranging from 0 (“No”) to 100 (“Yes”).

Table 1 Items to measure sexual and non-sexual disinhibition

The design was a 2 (Order: Control condition first vs. Disinhibition condition first) by 2 (Gender) by 2 (Condition: Control vs. Disinhibition) by 2 (Disinhibition Type: Sexual vs. Non-sexual) mixed-model design with the first two factors between-subjects and the last two factors being within-subjects.

Results

We separately aggregated the eight items representing sexual disinhibition (Cronbach’s α = .63 in the control condition, α = .69 in the disinhibition condition) and non-sexual disinhibition (α = .83 in the control condition, α = .86 in the disinhibition condition) for each condition by calculating the mean score for each category. These aggregate scores were then subjected to a 2 (Order) by 2 (Gender) by 2 (Condition) by 2 (Disinhibition Type) mixed-model ANOVA.

The notion that higher scores on the selected items could indeed be regarded as indicators of disinhibition was supported by the expected main effect of Condition, F(1, 94) = 30.76, p < .001, η 2 p  = .25. If participants were instructed to respond to the items in a disinhibited manner, they produced greater mean scores on the sexual disinhibition scale (M = 40.74, SD = 20.29) than in the control condition (M = 34.65, SD = 17.96), t(97) = 4.93, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.32, as well as greater mean scores on the non-sexual disinhibition scale (M = 26.68, SD = 20.84) than under control conditions (M = 19.51, SD = 15.62), t(97) = 4.94, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.39. Importantly, both scales were comparably susceptible to the disinhibition instruction, as indicated by a non-significant Condition × Disinhibition Type interaction, F < 1. No other significant interactions with the Condition factor emerged.

Discussion

The finding that both scales showed higher scores when participants were instructed to respond as if they had no inhibitions indicated that increased scores on these measures can indeed be interpreted as assessments of disinhibition. Crucially, this effect was not stronger for items tapping into sexual disinhibition than for items tapping into non-sexual disinhibition. These two scales were, thus, adequate for putting the specificity of disinhibition under sexual arousal to a fair test.

Experiment 1

We tested the effect of experimentally induced sexual arousal on the pilot-tested measures of sexual and non-sexual disinhibition in a sample of male and female students. An effect of sexual arousal on sexual but not non-sexual disinhibition would speak to the specificity of the effects of arousal whereas an effect on both sexual and non-sexual disinhibition would corroborate non-specific processes of either general disinhibition and/or indifferent responding.

Method

Participants

University students (45 men, 39 women) were recruited on campus for a computer-guided experiment on the effect of acoustic stimuli on cognitive processing. One man self-identified as homosexual (his exclusion did not alter results) and about 56 % were currently in a relationship. Due to a technical error, age was not recorded for a subset of participants. The average age of those for whom age was recorded (n = 29) was 23.3 years (SD = 2.2), range 19–27. No other demographic variables were recorded but the general local student population can be described as predominantly White German citizens. All participants received 4€ (approx. US$3) as a compensation for their participation.

Procedure

Participants provided informed consent to participate in a study that might include erotic stimuli as well as questions about their sexual behavior. Men and women were separately randomized to the sexual arousal versus control group. They were then asked to put on cushioned headphones, close their eyes, and imagine as vividly as possible taking part in the situations that were subsequently presented. After each episode, six questions were shown on a screen. The first one was always the manipulation check whereas the following five items tapped into sexual and non-sexual disinhibition. As in the pilot study, questions including a sexual partner were presented in a formulation appropriate for participants’ self-reported sexual orientation (i.e., all gay and female participants received items referring to male partners). To maximize the chances of replicating the effect on sexual disinhibition (and prevent a drop of arousal before the relevant replication measures were screened), the sexual disinhibition items were always presented before the non-sexual disinhibition items. After answering all items, participants pressed a “Next” button and were led to the next episode and the following item block. After completing three episodes, the experiment was over.

Sexual Arousal Manipulation

To manipulate their sexual arousal, participants were played three episodes (lasting approximately 2 min each) of an explicitly erotic story (a commercial pornographic episode told from a female perspective in which a woman seduces a male sexual partner) versus a clearly non-erotic story (the description of someone’s experiences with cats). Translations of the episodes are provided in Appendix 1.

Both stories were told by a female narrator, as informal pretesting showed that women felt less inclined to identify with stories told from a male perspective and men felt more easily disturbed by a male voice than a female voice. This is corroborated by findings of females reporting higher levels of sexual arousal in reaction to audiotapes of female-initiated sexual interactions as compared to audiotapes of male-initiated activity (Heiman, 1977). Also, men’s sexual arousal can be predicted from both watching described actions as an observer as well as imagining oneself as a participant whereas women’s sexual arousal depended more critically on imagining oneself as a participant (Janssen, Carpenter, & Graham, 2003). Thus, a constant female voice was chosen to ensure that participants of both sexes would have comparable chances of getting sexually aroused by the stimuli.

Manipulation Check

The first question was always a manipulation check asking participants to indicate their current level of sexual arousal (“How sexually aroused are you in this moment?”) on a continuous slider scale from 0 (not at all) to 100 (very much).

Measures

Dependent Variables

Below the manipulation check item, five more questions appeared and were completed on an analogous slider scale ranging from 0 (No) to 100 (Yes). The five questions tapped into either sexual disinhibition or non-sexual disinhibition, but items addressing the former always appeared first. We selected seven items tapping into Sexual DisinhibitionFootnote 2 (α = .65) and eight items tapping into Non-sexual Disinhibition (α = .57). Responses to items were averaged to calculate a composite score of sexual disinhibition and non-sexual disinhibition, respectively.

Results

As a first step, we tested whether our sexual arousal manipulation had the desired effect on the manipulation check of self-estimated arousal in both men and women. To this end, we subjected the three ratings of sexual arousal to a 2 (Condition: Sexual arousal vs. Control) × 2 (Gender) × 3 (Episodes) mixed-model ANOVA. Results revealed that the manipulation of sexual arousal was successful, as participants in the sexual arousal condition reported greater sexual arousal (M = 31.68, SD = 25.13) than those in the control condition (M = 4.21, SD = 10.28), F(1, 75) = 39.31, p < .001, η 2 p  = .34, Cohen’s d = 1.43 (Fig. 1). Men reported greater arousal than women, F(1, 75) = 5.92, p = .02, η 2 p  = .07. The only other significant effect was a Condition × Episode interaction, F(2, 74) = 4.16, p = .02, η 2 p  = .10, which indicated increased arousal over time, but only in the sexual arousal condition. The three-way interaction did not reach conventional significance, F(2, 74) = 2.71, p = .07, η 2 p  = .07.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean scores (+SE) on manipulation check of self-reported sexual arousal as a function of experimentally induced sexual arousal, episode, and participant gender in Study 1

As we were only interested in the effect of sexual arousal, we used the aggregated self-estimated arousal across all three episodes to exclude participants who were in the sexual arousal condition but showed no arousal (M < 10; n = 10) as well as participants who reported sexual arousal although they were in the control condition (M > 10; n = 6).Footnote 3 All subsequent analyses were conducted without these 16 participants (8 men, 8 women). Control analyses that included these individuals generally yielded similar results but with somewhat weaker effects.

To test whether the manipulation of sexual arousal also caused sexual disinhibition, non-sexual disinhibition or both, we conducted a 2 (Condition) × 2 (Gender) × 2 (Disinhibition Type: Sexual vs. Non-sexual) mixed-model ANOVA. Results showed that the hypothesized Condition × Disinhibition Type interaction was significant, F(1, 66) = 5.92, p = .02, η 2 p  = .08. Specifically, a separate 2 (Condition) × 2 (Gender) between-subjects ANOVA on the sexual disinhibition scale showed that sexually aroused participants reported more sexual disinhibition than non-aroused participants, F(1, 66) = 9.16, p = .004, η 2 p  = .12Footnote 4 (Fig. 2). A non-significant interaction with gender, F < 1, indicated that the effect of sexual arousal on sexual disinhibition was equally large and present for men (Cohen’s d = 0.94) and women (Cohen’s d = 0.51). In contrast, the level of non-sexual disinhibition was not contingent on participant gender, sexual arousal, or their interaction, all ps > .18.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean scores (+SE) on measures of sexual and nonsexual disinhibition as a function of experimentally induced sexual arousal and participant gender in Study 1

Discussion

The results of Study 1 replicated the effect of sexual arousal on sexual disinhibition but also expanded them in several regards. First of all, the effect of greater sexual disinhibition was replicated under more controlled settings in the laboratory. Second, we also expanded previous results that showed sexual arousal effects on men to women. Finally, we obtained preliminary support that the effect was indeed specific to sexual disinhibition and could not be attributed to either general disinhibition or indifferent responding.

Despite these important results, there were some methodological limitations to this first study. The experimental induction of sexual arousal was not standardized and, despite the successful manipulation check, the two conditions clearly differed in more features than their potential to induce sexual arousal, as they were chosen as non-representative exemplars of clearly erotic versus non-erotic stories (e.g., they were different in the degree to which they were captivating and emotion inducing). Although we deem it implausible as an alternative explanation, it is theoretically conceivable that the absence of human characters in the non-erotic story prevented identification with the story and thus reduced an otherwise observable effect of listening to any narrative. More importantly, the order of dependent variables after arousal induction was always fixed to present the manipulation check first, then the sexual disinhibition items, followed by the non-sexual disinhibition items. This fixed order was chosen to maximize the chances of a replication, as otherwise it could have been argued that measures of non-sexual disinhibition critically attenuated arousal effects and, thus, a failure to replicate would have been attributable to these interfering items. Although we replicated the effect of greater sexual disinhibition under sexual arousal conditions, this fixed order provided a comparably conservative test for the non-sexual disinhibition account. It is conceivable that time alone makes experimentally induced sexual arousal fade away during the course of the responding blocks. The later position of non-sexual disinhibition items could, thus, be confounded with decreased sexual arousal. Experiment 2 sought to replicate the findings of Experiment 1 while addressing these limitations.

Experiment 2

In the second study, we addressed potential problems of Experiment 1 in a systematic manner. First, we created more standardized experimental stimuli (based on stories previously used as auditory stimuli in phallometric testing) to realize two more comparable experimental arousal conditions. The potential for an order effect was eliminated by presenting items of non-sexual and sexual disinhibition in randomized order after the sexual arousal induction. To confirm that sexual arousal had not fully faded away before participants completed these items, we put the manipulation check of sexual arousal at the end of the experiment.

Method

Participants

University students (41 men, 45 women; M age = 24.3 years, SD = 5.6) were recruited on campus. One man and one woman self-identified as homosexual (exclusion did not alter results) and about 63 % were currently in a relationship.

Procedure

Participants were given information that the study dealt with how humans process auditory presented narratives, that the experiment might include sexually explicit language, and that they would be asked about their sexual behaviors. Again, participants of both genders were separately randomized to the sexual arousal versus control groups. The protocol closely followed that of Experiment 1 with the important differences that there were four episodes in each condition (as these were independent stories, their order was randomized for each participant), that each episode was followed by four questions in randomized order (two tapping into sexual disinhibition, two tapping into non-sexual disinhibition), and that the manipulation check was included after all other questions had been answered.

Sexual Arousal Manipulation

Instead of commercially available material, we used standardized erotic stories from the Quinsey auditory stimuli set for phallometric testing (Quinsey, Chaplin, & Upfold, 1984). These stimuli were designed for the induction of sexual arousal and had comparable lengths and structures. The consensual sexual stories have been shown to produce a significant increase (>+1.5 SD in penile response) in heterosexual men. We translated four neutral and four consensual heterosexual stories into German. As a further refinement (i.e., standardization) compared to Study 1, all stories were read by the same female actress from her perspective. All stories described social interactions involving individuals. Participants listened to either four sexually arousing or four neutral stories in randomized order (see Appendix 2 for the English language wording of the adapted narratives).

Measures

Dependent Variables

Non-sexual disinhibition and sexual disinhibition were assessed with eight items each (see Table 1). After each story, two non-sexual and two sexual disinhibition items appeared. As an important alteration to Study 1, the sequence of these items was fully randomized, thus de-confounding order of items and item content.

Manipulation Check

After listening to all four stories, participants completed a one-item manipulation check (“How sexually aroused are you by the presented stories?”) on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much) as the last item of the experiment.

Results

Again, the sexual arousal condition had an effect on the manipulation check of self-estimated arousal, as participants in the sexual arousal condition described themselves as more aroused, M = 2.72, SD = 1.03, than participants in the neutral condition, M = 1.35, SD = 0.57, F(1, 83) = 59.50, p < .001, η 2 p  = .42 (Fig. 3). There was no main effect of Gender, F(1, 83) = 1.40, p = .24, η 2 p  = .02, nor was there Gender × Condition interaction, F(1, 83) = 1.83, η 2 p  = .02, suggesting that the manipulation was equally successful for male and female participants. As in Experiment 1, we excluded participants who were in the sexual arousal condition but showed no arousal (M < 2; n = 6) as well as participants who reported sexual arousal although they were in the control condition (M > 1; n = 13).Footnote 5 All consecutive analyses were conducted without these 19 participants (8 men, 11 women). Retaining them in the sample made the effects weaker but did not change the direction of results.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean scores (+SE) on manipulation check of self-reported sexual arousal as a function of experimentally induced sexual arousal and participant gender in Study 2

Conducting the same ANOVA as in Experiment 1 (Condition × Gender × Disinhibition Type) revealed that the hypothesized Condition × Disinhibition Type interaction was again significant, F(1, 63) = 4.24, p = .04, η 2 p  = .06. Similar to Experiment 1, separate ANOVAs indicated that sexual arousal increased sexual disinhibition, F(1, 63) = 5.55, p = .02, η 2 p  = .08,Footnote 6 but not non-sexual disinhibition, F < 1. Men showed higher scores on sexual, F(1, 63) = 11. 28, p = .001, η 2 p  = .15, and non-sexual disinhibition, F(1, 63) = 4.30, p = .04, η 2 p  = .06, but neither of these participant gender effects were moderated by Condition, Fs < 1. Thus, sexual arousal led to sexual but not to non-sexual disinhibition in both men (Cohen’s d = 0.48) and women (Cohen’s d = 0.67) (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean scores (+SE) on measures of sexual and nonsexual disinhibition as a function of experimentally induced sexual arousal and participant gender in Study 2

Discussion

Experiment 2 replicated the findings of Experiment 1 in a more controlled fashion by using standardized arousal inducing stories and fully randomized item orders. Nevertheless, the results remained virtually unaltered. In line with Experiment 1, men showed higher levels of non-sexual disinhibition than women but this was unaffected by sexual arousal. It thus seems that sexual arousal indeed specifically reduces sexual inhibition and not just any conformity to social norms and rules (or leads to merely indifferent responding).

General Discussion

The two studies provided concordant support that sexual arousal evokes sexual disinhibition as operationalized by an increased self-reported likelihood to engage in uncommon, risky, and coercive sexual activities. This effect was independent of participant gender and specific to sexual (but not non-sexual) disinhibition. Hence, situational sexual arousal may function as a previously largely ignored risk factor for (self-reported interest in) socially inadequate, unhealthy, and manipulative sexual behavior.

As an important addition to previous work, we established this effect under much more controlled laboratory settings and, more importantly, for women as well as for men. Previous research has largely neglected the issue of whether sexual arousal has an identical effect on women’s sexual behavior as it has on men’s, presumably because many social problems associated with sexual disinhibition, such as, for example, coercive sexual behavior, are far more frequently observed in men than in women (e.g., Imhoff, Bergmann, Banse, & Schmidt, 2013a). Nevertheless, the present studies obtained an effect of sexual arousal not only on the arguably predominantly male forms of problematic sexual behavior but also on indicators of sexual risk behavior regarding the protection against sexually transmitted diseases and unwanted pregnancy (both among the top five sexual regrets reported by women) (Galperin et al., 2013). As these are real-world problems for women and men (at least sexually transmitted diseases), the negligence of women in research addressing this sexual risk behavior is ill-advised.

The consequences of sexual arousal in women are relevant for real-world problems as argued above; however, they also invite speculation about gender differences in sexual responding. It has often been argued that the different required minimal investment to reproduce between men and women has led to the evolution of fundamentally different sexual systems guiding sexual desire and behavior (Buss, 2003; Symons, 1979; Townsend, Kline, & Wasserman, 1995). From this perspective, it could be speculated that sexually disinhibited behavior has markedly greater costs for women and it would thus be adaptive for women if sexual arousal did not lead to disinhibition, but rather to greater caution, selectivity, and processing of contextual information (Baumeister, 2000; Rupp et al., 2009). Our studies, in contrast, showed a remarkable uniformity of the effect of sexual arousal on men’s and women’s (self-reported) sexual behavior. It may thus be that the described gender differences are predominantly situated in the pathway to sexual arousal whereas the consequences of (or pathway from) sexual arousal are much more similar for men and women.

Two studies have provided consistent support for the notion that sexual arousal leads to sexual disinhibition (as we label it) but not to non-sexual disinhibition. As an important caveat, our results were tightly connected to the way we have construed disinhibition. We relied on a phenomenological definition based on a well-established definition in social psychology: Disinhibition is reflected in reduced consideration of what others might think of the respective behavior. This has to be differentiated from definitions that understand (dis-)inhibition as an internal mental process. Although it is conceivable that sexual arousal also impaired such internal processes of inhibiting sexual impulses, it cannot be determined from our data whether this was the case or whether sexual arousal merely increased the strength of the sexual impulse (while the inhibition remains unaltered). Future research dedicated to the questions of whether sexual arousal boosts sexual excitation and approach behavior or whether it dampens sexual inhibition and avoidance behavior seems warranted.

Given the available evidence, what is the most plausible model of causal factors creating the observed disinhibitory effects? Our studies ruled out the alternative explanation that sexual arousal merely reduces cognitive capacity to act and respond in a socially desirable manner. Thus, if the effect was due to reduced inhibition, we would need to presume a highly specific system for sexual inhibition which would contradict the idea that goal-directed inhibition is a domain-general process as evidenced in the fact that overly risky behavior seems to generalize across various activities, including unsafe sex (Zuckerman & Kuhlman, 2000). This leaves the idea that sexual arousal strengthens the appetitive sexual system—rendering sexual temptations more salient, appealing, or rewarding—as the most plausible process assumption. A similar argument was brought forward by Ariely and Loewenstein (2006), who proposed that evolved appetitive systems (i.e., hunger, thirst, mating-relevant behavior) in the brain were designed to increase motivation once they get primed with suitable opportunities (also labelled motivational myopia) (Ditto et al., 2006). This would corroborate that motivational salience effects as a consequence of increased sexual arousal drive the domain-specific disinhibition observed in the present studies.

Another open issue is the specificity of the effect of sexual arousal. Although the current data did not speak to any effect on non-sexual forms of disinhibition, Ariely and Loewenstein (2006) did report that sexual arousal also led to more risky decisions. How are these results reconcilable? It is conceivable that sexual arousal also triggers approach motivation to tempting stimuli other than sexual stimuli (Gold, 1993). A second possibility is that the limited comparability of the arousing and non-arousing conditions in the Ariely and Loewenstein study led to other effects that are not due to sexual arousal. We have argued above that the more taxing nature of the arousing condition might also have led to greater ego depletion and resultantly riskier choices (Imhoff et al., 2013b), an effect that was controlled for in this research.

Limitations and Future Directions

A clear limitation of the present studies is the reliance on self-reported likelihood of showing sexually risky, manipulative, or uncommon behavior. Whether this reduced weight of how one’s actions will be evaluated by others will translate to actual corresponding behavior is of course dependent on many factors other than just the level of sexual arousal (which is true for virtually every behavioral phenomenon). To elucidate the causal contribution of sexual arousal, we deemed it necessary to manipulate arousal under controlled laboratory settings, which unfortunately are an obstacle to the observation of spontaneous and actual real-life behavior.

Independent of a more fine-grained understanding of the underlying processes involved in the disinhibitory effects of sexual arousal on decision making and behavior, these effects might also provide an explanatory base for other examples of problematic sexual behavior. Neglecting social desirability concerns for the sake of sexual gratification could also be brought forward as an explanation of sexual undesirable behavior like lowered age thresholds for acceptable sexual partners: In a preliminary study, we have found that experimentally induced sexual arousal led to lowered age thresholds for sexual partners accepted as adequate (Imhoff, 2012). This corroborates the importance of situational factors (above and beyond well-documented trait influences) such as sexual arousal in gaining a more complete understanding of sexually undesirable behaviors. Actively searching for sexually pleasing content in a state of acute sexual arousal might increase the risk to access sexually more extreme material because social desirability concerns will be less attended to. In combination with sexual desensitization or satiation, this might lower the threshold for a progression toward increasingly extreme non-deviant and deviant pornography (Seigfried-Spellar & Rogers, 2013). Maybe even more relevant, in a study on online sexual solicitation, adult men who self-reported having engaged in sexual conversations with children and adolescents reported to have experienced sexual arousal not only during but also before the communication (Bergen et al., in press). This possible etiological pathway remains speculative at present but, nevertheless, constitutes an important research agenda to gain a better understanding of situational factors in sexual offending.

In conclusion, the present two studies constituted a controlled replication and expansion of the domain-specific disinhibitory effects of sexual arousal. We were able to rule out the potential alternative explanation that sexual arousal merely leads to unspecific social undesirable or even merely more indifferent responding. Perhaps most importantly, we showed that these effects were not limited to male sexual arousal but that women showed the same pattern of sexual disinhibition under sexual arousal.