Introduction

Female mate choice is one of the main processes of sexual selection (Darwin 1859). Despite the fact that for humans (Buss 1994) and a wide variety of animals (Andersson 1994) the importance of female mate preferences and mate choice has been widely accepted, for non-human primates, evidence for female mate preferences remains largely anecdotal (Paul 2002). Gowaty (1997) postulated that this is a consequence of highly variable and often antagonistic sexual tactics, and therefore, in non-human primates, female mate choice can be constrained by female–female competition, male–male contests, reproductive suppression, male choice and male coercion. Thus, female preferences will remain speculative, but behavioural studies can produce data on female choice (Manson 1992).

There is evidence for female mate choice in non-human primates (Keddy-Hector 1992; Paul 2002; Pusey and Wolf 1996; Small 1989; Smuts 1987). However, in species where females stay in multi-male groups and females can mate with several males (as in grey-cheeked mangabeys), female choice can be especially important, but also cryptic (Smuts and Smuts 1993), due to male behaviour or female–female competition (Bercovitch 1991; Manson 1992; Soltis et al. 2001). In Papionines, there are several examples of active involvement of females in the mating process such as following, presentation and grooming between non-consort females and males, participation in ‘sneak’ matings with subordinate males (Nunn 1999; Smuts 1983) and female competition for mating partners (Bercovitch 1995).

Although mate preference criteria of females are usually unknown, skewed female mate choice has been demonstrated in some non-human primate species living in multi-male groups. Females may choose to mate with high-ranking males (Dixon et al. 1993); Macaca mulatta: (Bercovitch and Nurnberg 1997); Papio cynocephalus: (Altmann et al. 1996); Cercocebus torquatus atys: (Gust et al. 1998) or mid-ranking males [Macaca mulatta: (Manson 1992, 1994); Macaca fuscata: (Huffman 1991; Soltis et al. 2001)]. In baboons, females mate with multiple males but may try to develop a special relationship with at least one male (Smuts 1985), who also forms an affiliative bond with her offspring (Bercovitch 1991; Palombit et al. 1997). However, that relationship rarely has a positive influence on male mating success (Bercovitch 1991; Huffman 1991; Manson 1994). In contrast to these affiliative bonds, females may show preferences towards newcomer males (Bercovitch 1997; Small 1989). Several paternity studies provided evidence that such female behaviour towards newcomers can have a positive impact on the reproductive success of migrating males (Berard et al. 1994; Fietz et al. 2000; Gagneux et al. 1997; Launhardt et al. 2001).

One means whereby females can manipulate male sexual behaviour is signalling fertility. Sexual swellings are common signals among female primates that live in multi-male groups (Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1976). The function of exaggerated swelling is controversial and may vary among species; it may be a reliable indicator of timing of ovulation (Nunn 1999), or female quality (Pagel 1994), or it may be a within-female indicator of conception across cycles (Emery and Whitten 2003; Zinner et al. 2002). The timing of exaggerated sexual swellings in females is not always synchronised with ovulation (Dixon 1998; Nunn 1999), and therefore, females may have a large time window to exhibit sexual behaviour that does not lead to fertilisation, increasing the opportunity for female manipulation of male behaviours (Pagel 1994; van Schaik et al. 2000). The presence of sexual swellings can promote male competition which often concentrates paternity in one, dominant male (the best male hypothesis; Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1976). On the other hand, sexual swellings can confuse paternity among males to reduce aggression towards infants, when females mate with many males during the periods with sexual swellings (the many males hypothesis; Hrdy and Whitten 1987). During their ovarian cycle, females can also employ both strategies, depending on fertility (the graded signal hypothesis; Nunn 1999).

Post-copulation calls can be indicators of female choice although their ultimate function remains often a matter of speculation (O’Connell and Cowlishaw 1994). At least 15 different hypotheses have been proposed to explain the function of female copulation calls in primates (Maestripieri and Roney 2005). For example, (a) female copulation calls may be associated with orgasm [Japanese macaques: (Troisi and Carosi 1998)], (b) females call to synchronise orgasm with the male (Hamilton and Arrowood 1978) and therefore promote the sperm transfer to aid conception (Fox and Fox 1971), (c) female copulation calls can incite male sperm competition to ensure that sons will inherit the best sperm [‘sexy sons’ hypothesis; (O’Connell and Cowlishaw 1994] and (d) female copulation calls may also have a function of announcing paternity certainty to promote paternal investment (Henzi 1996).

Grey-cheeked mangabeys live in multi-male groups, and males often migrate between groups, while females tend to stay in their natal groups. Females exhibit sexual swellings that cover a relatively small area ventral to the base of the tail (Chalmers and Rowell 1971; Danjou 1972; Rowell and Chalmers 1970; Wallis 1983). Females with sexual swellings are present throughout the year, and matings are recorded only during the oestrus period (Wallis 1983). Females usually copulate with more than one male (Chalmers 1968; Wallis 1979). High-ranking males were about five times more aggressive towards females than migrating males, and low-ranking males behave almost three times more aggressively towards females than migrating males. However, high-ranking males are seven times less aggressive towards juveniles than migrating males, and in comparison to migrating males, low-ranking males are 14 times less aggressive towards juveniles (Arlet et al., in preparation). Dr. William Olupot (personal communication) observed two females carrying dead infants shortly after new males entered the group, which suggests that migrating male aggression towards infants can be lethal. Males can also care for offspring (Arlet et al., in preparation; Struhsaker and Leyland 1979; Wallis 1979).

We investigated female sexual behaviour within five groups of grey-cheeked mangabeys in Kibale National Park, Uganda, and tested the hypothesis that females exhibit mate choice using as indications the prevalence of (1) females soliciting matings by presenting to males and (2) females refusing to mate with approaching males. In addition, we describe how these behaviours as well as grooming and copulation calls are distributed over high-ranking, low-ranking and migrating males and discuss these patterns with regard to trade-offs that can be expected to play a roll in female mate choice in multi-female–multi-male groups. While much is known about female sexual behaviour in the closely related baboons, arboreal mangabeys can provide an interesting comparison because habitat can be an important factor in mating systems.

Materials and methods

Study site and subjects

After a pilot study in 1999, data were collected for 6 months in 2001 (January–June) and supplemented with observations in 2002 (April–June) in Kibale National Park (0°13′–0°41′N and 30°19′–30°32E). Kibale (795 km2) is a moist, evergreen medium altitude forest with a mosaic of swamp, grassland, thicket and colonizing forest (Chapman and Lambert 2000). Mangabeys in Kibale live in multi-male groups of on average 14 individuals (Olupot 1999; Wallis 1979; Waser 1977). In total, we observed 26 sub-adult and adult males and 35 females.

Fourteen males were recognised by the unique colour combination of their collars or attached radios placed on them in an earlier study (Olupot 1999). Males without a collar were classified as adult male (AM, N = 7) or sub-adult male (SAM, N = 5) and were recognisable on the basis of their relative sizes and other distinguishing features. In each of the groups, there were at least two individual males present in the same group between January–June 2001. These males were regarded as resident males (N = 16). Males that emigrated at least once from a group or that immigrated into a group and then dispersed during these 6 months were called migrating males (N = 10). This excluded visiting males that were sighted only briefly (<2 days) in groups.

Females with sexual swellings were present throughout the year. The swelling increases in size and colour gradually, deepening to pink (oestrous adult female inflating: OAFi) until the maximum stage of swelling is reached (oestrous adult female peak: OAFp). As oestrus passes, the swelling becomes less turgid and the colour changes to dark purple (oestrous adult female deflating: OAFd; Danjou 1972; Deputte 1991; Wallis 1983). The duration of sexual swellings average 17–31 days, with the phase from quiescence to peak swelling being 4–14 days long, the peak size of sexual swelling lasting 2–4 days and the deflating stage lasting 7–14 days (Danjou 1972; Deputte 1991; Rowell and Chalmers 1970; Wallis 1983).

Observation methods

From January 2001 to June 2001, two observers collected behavioural observations for 8–9 h/day, for 6 consecutive days per week, for a total of 2,036 h. To measure the rate of movement of males between groups, we censused the focal group every day during each observation period. Dominance interactions among males were recorded on the basis of approach–retreat interactions following de Waal (1987), which were scored for all nearby males during focal follows of oestrus females (Olupot and Waser 2001). Dominance relationships are ordinal within a group, and therefore, we simplified this classification to compare males across groups; the highest-ranking male was classified as high ranking and others as low ranking. In two groups, the ranks of the two highest-ranking males could not be distinguished and both males were classified as high ranking. Mating was defined as a mount involving intromission (Wallis 1983), and mating success of a male was measured as the number of observed matings.

We predominantly followed females with sexual swellings. Focal females were classified as ‘adult female’—without sexual swelling (AF), or female with sexual swelling—‘inflating female’ (OAFi), ‘peak female’ (OAFp) and ‘deflating female’ (OAFd). The behaviours of focal females were recorded all day, divided into 30-min sampling blocks with no more then 5 min between blocks. During these all-day follows we recorded female behaviour. During focal sampling of females with sexual swellings, we recorded all occurrences of interactions with males and other group members within 10 m of the focal female. We focussed on the following behaviours which have been used to study female mate choice in the socially similar baboons (Bercovitch 1995): (1) the direction and frequency of grooming during the oestrous period, (2) the direction of sexual presentation by females, (3) sexual refusals, (4) subsequent successful matings and (5) the presence of female copulation calls.

Data analysis

To compare the behaviour of different classes of females with sexual swellings and males, the observations were divided by the number of days the interaction could have occurred during the 6-month period (when a particular male and female were observed together in a group). For the grooming data, we excluded males that were guarding females. In our analysis, each day represented one data point for females and for males. These standardised values were then used for statistical comparisons and graphical representations. To simultaneously explore multiple factors and their interactions we used an Analysis of Multi-Factor Regression (GLM) approach on the number of events (such as sexual presentations, matings, copulation calls, mating refusals and grooming) per the number of days an interaction could have occurred, thus using a single data point for each male. The analyses were performed using Statgraphics 5.0. (two-tailed probability with alpha at 0.05).

Results

Sexual presentations

Grey-cheeked mangabey females mated only when they had a sexual swelling. Forty-eight percent of sexual presentations by females led to matings (N = 63 sexual presentations of 26 females). Most of the sexual presentations that resulted in mating were performed by females with peak swellings (66%), then inflating females (18%) and deflating (16%; GLM analysis: p = 0.01, df = 2, F = 4.84, R 2 = 32.67%).

There was no difference between male categories in receiving sexual presentations, but peak females presented mainly to high-ranking males, and deflating females to migrating males (GLM analysis: R 2 = 32.67%, male category p = 0.1 df = 2 F = 2.29, male category × female category p = 0.002, df = 4, F = 4.85; Fig. 1)

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean number of sexual presentations of oestrous females towards high-ranking, low-ranking and migrating males. The error bars show the interval mean ± 2 SE

Mating

Fifty-three percent of matings were initiated by females (N = 56 matings). Females mated with between one and three different partners (N = 28 oestrus females) during one cycle. Usually, one of the partners was a high-ranking male who also followed the females during this period, and in addition, she mated with one of the low-ranking or migrating males. During days with maximum swelling, females mated with two to three males, but during the deflating stage, females mated with one to two males only. Some matings with migrating males occurred outside the group (sneaky copulations). Most matings were recorded with high-ranking males (GLM analysis: p = 0.0002, df = 8, F = 9.67, R 2 = 60.5%). Peak females mated mainly with high-ranking males but also migrating males and low-ranking males (GLM analysis: p = 0.00001, df = 4, F = 7.86, R 2 = 60.5%; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean number of matings with inflating females, peak females and deflating females. The error bars show the interval mean ± 2 SE

Copulation calls

We recorded 25 matings that were accompanied by a female call (grunt), which represents 46% of all recorded matings. The presence of female copulation calls depended on male rank (GLM: F = 8.72, df = 25, R 2 = 0.66, p = 0.0003); most female copulation calls occurred during matings with high-ranking males, except for matings with one migrating male, who elicited many copulation calls.

Mating refusals

One third of all mating approaches by males were refused (N = 87 approaches), and 59% was refused by peak females. High-ranking males approached more and received more refusals than other males (Poisson Regression, p = 0.0005), hence there was no difference between the male categories in proportion of approaches refused.

Grooming

For this analysis we used data of males that were not guarding females. Receptive females groomed males more than non-oestrus females (GLM: F = 8.09, df = 1, R 2 = 57%, p = 0.007). Most of the grooming by oestrus females was observed with high-ranking males (GLM: F = 8.19, df = 1, R 2 = 57%, p = 0.0001).

Discussion

This study revealed that females actively participate in mate choice; females are not just accepting mating with eager males but refuse as well as actively approach and solicit copulations by males. The observed behaviour can be seen as the range of solutions for the female’s dilemma—on one hand, she could choose to mate the best male, capable to protect her and her offspring, but she may also mate with many males to avoid inbreeding or reduce aggression or risk of infanticide (van Schaik et al. 2000). The evolution of prolonged follicular phases and unpredictable ovulations (van Schaik et al. 2000) as well as sexual swelling that advertises female reproductive state (Domb and Pagel 2001; Nunn 1999) and also copulation calls (Semple and McComb 2000) may give females opportunity to express their preferences within the social context.

In grey-cheeked mangabeys, 48% of the observed sexual presentations initiated by females resulted in matings. On average, resident and migrating males received similar presentation rates from females but females at the peak of sexual swelling presented mainly to high-ranking males and deflating females to migrating males. Oestrus females were also more active in grooming males than non-oestrus females, with preferences toward high-ranking males and then towards migrating males. Females tended to mate with more males during the peak of sexual swelling than during the deflating stage.

Mangabey females at the peak of sexual swelling refuse almost 60% of male mating approaches. High-ranking males are the ones who approach the most and receive more refusals than other males. That the proportion of mating refusals received by the male categories did not differ significantly conceals that particular females did refuse to mate more or less with particular males or male classes. The high incidence of refusals and variation among females indicates that they are not mating with any approaching male but are choosy. Moreover, mangabey females initiated more than half of the observed copulations and mated with more than one partner. This result corroborates the findings of Wallis (1983), where 42% of copulations were initiated by females. In many social species, females show physiological and behavioural adaptations that lead to copulation with more males and over a longer period than is necessary to ensure fertilisation (Smuts 1985). It seems that, similar to rhesus macaques (Manson 1992, 1994), mangabey females can mate with males of various ranks, but in some groups, they can be monopolised by high-ranking males which are able to prevent them from mating with other males as high-ranking males often attacked females that mated with other males.

That females mainly gave copulation calls when mating high-ranking males can be explained in two different ways. Firstly, it is possible that females of mangabeys give copulation calls to promote paternity of these mails by advertising their interest in mating with high-ranking males or to synchronise orgasm with the preferred male (Hamilton and Arrowood 1978) and, therefore, promote the sperm transfer up the reproductive track which aids conception (Fox and Fox 1971). Secondly, when a high-ranking male was in the proximity of a female mating with a migrating male or low-ranking male, he would attack them, and this risk may, therefore, explain why females only rarely gave copulation calls when mating these males.

Resident males are more aggressive to females than migrating males who, at the other hand, attack juveniles more often, which could probably lead to infanticide (Dr. W. Olupot, personal communication). Therefore, sexual interaction with more than one male may reduce aggression from migrating males to her juveniles, as a result of confused paternity (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2000), but may increase aggression from high-ranking males.

It is also interesting that mainly high-ranking males provided infant care, and those males were most successful at obtaining matings. Similarly, female baboons may preferentially mate with males who invest most in parental care (Bercovitch 1991). It is possible that caretakers are more likely to be fathers based on their general mating success (as high-ranking males) and, although females mate with many partners, males may even recognise their own infants (Buchan et al. 2003), and thus this care could be regarded as paternal investment in offspring rather than a tactic to obtain more matings.

In mangabeys in Kibale, mate choice was mainly indicated by more than half of the matings being initiated by females and almost 60% of male approaches being refused by peak females. Female sexual behaviour depended on oestrus phase (number of partners, proportion of male categories presented to and mated with) and male tactic in the group.

Females mated mainly with high-ranking males, and that can be explained by monopolisation by these males (mate guarding), their aggression towards females, female preference based on ‘the best male’ or infant care. However, females also mated with migrating males and lower ranking males despite efforts of the high-ranking male to monopolise matings. This can be explained by attractiveness towards novel males (possibly to avoid inbreeding) or as a tactic to avoid their aggression towards juveniles. Female sexual behaviour in grey-cheeked mangabeys changes with oestrus phase and may therefore serve different functions at different times of the female reproductive cycle. These results indicate that female choice has an important role in shaping mating systems in multi-male and multi-female groups.