Introduction

Aging is associated with changes in functional brain networks, such as the default mode network (DMN) (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2007; Bai et al. 2008; Damoiseaux et al. 2008; Mevel et al. 2011). The DMN is a bilaterally distributed functional brain network (Schilbach et al. 2012) consisting of an anterior [medial prefrontal (mPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)], as well as a medial [posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus] and lateral posterior part (inferior parietal lobule and parts of the temporal lobe). It is characterized by its activation during phases of rest, while it is deactivated (or suppressed) during externally focused states, and is associated with a variety of cognitive functions, such as attention, self-referential cognition, episodic and working memory (Buckner et al. 2008; Raichle et al. 2001; Amft et al. 2015).

In the course of aging, several studies revealed changes in functional connectivity within the DMN (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2007; Batouli et al. 2009; Damoiseaux et al. 2008; Mowinckel et al. 2012; Wu et al. 2011). For example, Andrews-Hanna et al. (2007) reported a decrease of the functional coupling during task performance between the mPFC and PCC/precuneus in older compared to younger adults, making the two parts more independent from each other. Accordingly, age-related increases in resting-state functional connectivity were reported for the anterior DMN but not the posterior DMN in older subjects (Jones et al. 2011). Similar effects were found during task-based functional imaging. Older subjects were found to show less deactivation within the PCC/precuneus as compared to the mPFC (Davis et al. 2008; Grady et al. 2006; Lustig et al. 2003). Thus, a shift in (de-) activation from posterior to anterior brain regions in aging (PASA) might be present in older adults that serves as a compensatory mechanism to maintain cognitive performance as stable as possible (Davis et al. 2008; Grady et al. 1994).

While there is evidence for a functional reorganization of the DMN in terms of PASA, differences in hemispheric asymmetries regarding age-related decline have not been explored yet. Investigating young adults, first studies showed that there might be hemispheric differences in functional connectivity of the DMN (Saenger et al. 2012). Regarding cognitive decline in the course of aging, it is established that not all cognitive functions decrease at the same rate from early to late adulthood (Hedden and Gabrieli 2004). Dolcos (2002) for example discussed whether there are differences in the aging processes of the left as compared to the right hemisphere. The right hemi-aging theory states that the right hemisphere is more prone to age-related decline (Grady et al. 1994). This, in turn, would result in an earlier decrease of cognitive functions that are predominantly processed within the right hemisphere, such as visual–spatial functions. In terms of age-related structural changes of the DMN, a more pronounced structural decrease of right hemispheric parts would be reasonable. Thus, based on functional imaging and behavioral research evidence exists that age-related reorganization of the DMN might happen along a posterior–anterior (PASA) as well as right-left axis (right hemi-aging).

While there is evidence for a functional reorganization of the DMN, the question of the possible underlying structural correlate is still unresolved. To assess the structural correlates of the DMN, gyrification, i.e. degree of cortical folding is a valuable measure for studying changes of brain structure. Driven mainly by cell proliferation and migration as well as fiber connectivity, it is supposed to reflect the underlying structural connectivity and complexity (for a recent review: Zilles et al. 2013, 2015), particularly as a measure of local atrophy in later decades of life (Kochunov et al. 2005; Magnotta et al. 1999; Van Essen 1997; Welker 1990; Zilles et al. 1988). The gyrification index (GI) as a measure of the degree of cortical folding was introduced by Zilles et al. (1988) as a contour-based two-dimensional (2D) approach to measure gyrification in post-mortem brains. The ratio between the total outer cortical contour length and that of the superficially exposed part of the cortex, which does not comprise the segments buried in the sulci, defines the GI: the higher the GI, the higher the degree of cortical folding. A three-dimensional (3D) extension of the original GI was developed by Schaer et al. (2008) using the reconstructed surface from in vivo T1-weigthed brain images. The so-called local gyrification index is calculated over thousands of vertices. Several studies used the method of Schaer et al. (2008) to study changes in local gyrification indices with respect to development and aging, cognitive abilities as well as diseases such as schizophrenia, psychosis and Parkinson’s disease (Palaniyappan and Liddle 2012; Docherty et al. 2015; Palaniyappan et al. 2013; White et al. 2010; Zhang et al. 2014). With respect to healthy aging, Hogstrom et al. (2013) were able to show age-related regional decreases in local gyrification within, e.g. the postcentral gyrus and inferior parietal lobules.

Taken together, based on functional imaging the DMN has been shown to be susceptible to age-related functional reorganization, such as the PASA from early to late adulthood. By contrast, potential structural correlates of this age-related functional reorganization of the DMN have not been identified yet. In the present study, we implemented measures of the local gyrification index as a marker of age-related structural atrophy within the DMN. We investigated the relation between local gyrification indices within different parts of the DMN and age as well as cognitive performance in 749 older adults. It was hypothesized that, in line with the PASA theory, age-related structural decline would be more pronounced in posterior parts of the DMN as compared to the anterior parts of the DMN. In addition, according to the right hemi-aging theory, we hypothesized that the right hemispheric parts of the DMN would be more prone to age-related structural decline as compared to the left hemispheric parts of the DMN.

Methods

Subjects

Data of all subjects included in the present study (age range: 55–85 years) were derived from 1000BRAINS (Caspers et al. 2014), an epidemiologic population-based study assessing the variability of brain structure and function in the course of normal aging, based on the epidemiologic population-based Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study with focus on risk factors for atherosclerosis, cardiovascular disease, cardiac infarction and death (Schmermund et al. 2002). From the initial sample of 989 subjects, 843 subjects were aged between 55 and 85 years. Complete T1-weighted MRI data sets were available for 792. Two subjects had to be excluded from the analysis due to considerable structural brain defects post stroke. 25 subjects had to be excluded due to postprocessing failure within FreeSurfer (see below) and another 16 subjects had more than three tests that were not executed during the neuropsychological assessment and therefore were excluded as well. Finally, 749 subjects (mean age: 67.24, SD: 6.78, 413 males, 336 females) were eligible for the structural analysis of the gyrification index. All subjects gave written informed consent at the beginning of the study. The study protocol was approved by the local Ethics Committee of the University of Essen.

Image acquisition

Brain imaging data were acquired on a 3T Siemens Tim-TRIO MR scanner (Erlangen, Germany), using a 32-channel head coil. For the surface reconstruction, a 3D high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) anatomical scan was acquired in each subject (176 slices, slice thickness 1 mm, repetition time (TR) = 2250 ms, echo time (TE) 3.03 ms, field of view (FoV) = 256 × 256 mm2, flip angle = 9°, voxel resolution 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). For the resting-state analyses, as used here for the extraction of the DMN, 300 functional images were acquired at rest using a gradient-echo echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (36 slices, slice thickness 3.1 mm, TR = 2200 ms, TE = 30 ms, FoV = 200 × 200 mm2, voxel resolution 3.1 × 3.1 × 3.1 mm3). During the resting state condition, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed, relax, let their minds wander, but not to fall asleep. This was affirmed by post-scan debriefing.

Definition of the DMN seed regions on resting-state fMRI data

Image analysis was performed using FSL [FMRIB Software Library: http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; (Jenkinson et al. 2012)]. For every subject, preprocessing included motion correction [MCFLIRT; (Jenkinson et al. 2002)], brain extraction [BET; (Smith 2002)], and high pass temporal filtering (100 ms). FMRIB’s Linear and Nonlinear Image Registration tool [FLIRT and FNIRT; (Jenkinson et al. 2002; Jenkinson and Smith 2001)] were used to register the participant’s resting state-fMRI volumes to the individual structural scan. The latter was registered to the standard space template (MNI 152). Then, all images were smoothed by a 5-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. For every subject, EPI images were denoised using FMRIB’s ICA-based Xnoiseifier and corrected for motion artefacts [FIX; (Griffanti et al. 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2014)]. Finally, EPI images were resampled at 3 mm isotropic voxel size.

After preprocessing, MELODIC multi-session temporal concatenation (Beckmann et al. 2005) was used to identify common spatial patterns across subjects within the resting state data using probabilistic independent component analysis decomposition of the resting state signals (Beckmann and Smith 2004). Subjects for this study were selected based on availability of structural MRI data to increase sample size. Since not all subjects also completed the resting state scan, a bootstrapping approach was used to reliably define the DMN across the whole sample [similar to other approaches, such as ICASSO, which uses a clustering to reliably extract resting state networks (Himberg and Hyvarinen 2003)]: Resting state datasets were available for 691 subjects. From this subsample 200 subjects were randomly selected using a random number generator. Resting state networks were extracted from the group of 200 randomly selected subjects using MELODIC. DMN was selected via visual inspection to identify the best spatial match to the DMN as published by Beckmann and Smith (2004) and Smith et al. (2009). This procedure was repeated 100 times (Fig. 1a), giving 100 DMNs, each based on a random sample of 200 subjects. Afterwards, the extracted 100 DMNs were superimposed onto each other in standard MNI space. This resulted in a DMN probability map, expressing the likelihood of a brain region to be involved in the DMN (ranging from 0 to 100 %) (Fig. 1b). The probability map was then thresholded at 95 % (using fslmaths, FSL), meaning that only if 95 out of the 100 DMNs included a given brain area, it was included into the final definition of the DMN (Fig. 1c). Each ROI was finally represented by its center of gravity (Fig. 1d). These final ROIs as defined in MNI-152 space were transformed into each subject’s individual space (using FNIRT/FLIRT as implemented in FSL) to allow for subsequent surface-based extraction of the local GI by means of FreeSurfer (Fig. 1i).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Left panel resting state (RS) workflow. Extraction of DMN, superimposition of all extracted DMNs, thresholding and projection on pial surface. Right panel local gyrification index (LGI) workflow. Freesurfer pipeline and calculation of local gyrification index (LGI; of every vertex on the pial surface and ROI)

ROIs of the DMN

The resulting mean DMN, averaged over all permutations and thresholded at 95 %, consisted of six clusters: Two anterior and four posterior clusters: (1) left mPFC (2) right mPFC (3) left PCC/precuneus, (4) right PCC/precuneus, (5) left AG (left angular gyrus), and (6) right AG . Figure 2 provides an overview of these six DMN-ROIs including macroanatomical and cytoarchitectonical identification of brain areas according the JuBrain Cytoarchitectonic Atlas (http://jubrain.fz-juelich.de) (Zilles and Amunts 2010), as implemented in the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al. 2005). Cytoarchitectonic areas were denoted in accordance with the respective publications (Amunts et al. 2000; Bludau et al. 2014; Caspers et al. 2008; Caspers et al. 2006; Kujovic et al. 2013; Malikovic et al. 2007; Palomero-Gallagher et al. 2015; Scheperjans et al. 2008a, b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Description of brain areas involved within the DMN, including macroanatomical and cytoarchitectonical identification of brain areas according the JuBrain Cytoarchitectonic Atlas

Surface reconstruction and local gyrification index

Anatomical images were first preprocessed using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Modeling: www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). All anatomical images were segmented into grey matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. Grey and white matter images were combined to generate a reliable brain mask for subsequent processing within the FreeSurfer pipeline. Afterwards, all skull stripped images were processed using the cortical reconstruction pipeline implemented in FreeSurfer 5.1.0 [http://freesurfer.net/, (Dale et al. 1999; Fischl et al. 1999)]. This included transformation to Talairach space, and segmentation of grey and white matter tissue, tessellation of grey/white matter boundary and correction of topological defects. The grey/white matter interface was expanded to create the pial surface, followed by spherical morphing and registration (Fig. 1e).  Local gyrification indices in native space were calculated according to Schaer et al. (2008, 2012) as implemented in FreeSurfer. While the pial surface was already generated in the standard processing pipeline of FreeSurfer, the outer hull had to be created for calculation of the local gyrification index (also implemented in Freesurfer; Schaer et al. 2008, 2012). This was achieved by tightly warping the pial surface by means of a morphological closing operation that uses a 15 mm sphere to close every sulcus (Fig. 1f). For each vertex of the pial surface, a local gyrification index value was assigned, calculated as the ratio of the pial surface area (defined as a sphere with the vertex as center and a 25 mm radius, according to Schaer et al. 2008) to the outer hull (Fig. 1g, h). Finally, local gyrification index was extracted for the vertices at the position of the center of gravity to best represent the gyrification over the whole extent of the ROI (Fig. 1i).

Surface reconstructions were randomly controlled for the participants. We did not find any systematic bias in the current data that would, in turn, bias the results on the local gyrification index measure. Further, all transformations of the DMN onto the individual subjects were controlled. Subjects for whom the transformations were not satisfactory were excluded from further analysis. In total, 25 subjects had to be excluded due to failure within FreeSurfer preprocessing and transformations from MNI to single subject space, which was mainly due to motion artefacts.

Cognitive performance

All subjects underwent extensive neuropsychological assessment [for an overview: see Caspers et al. (2014)]. Detailed description of all cognitive functions measured, tests included and variables chosen are provided in Table 1. For subjects having not more than 3 missing values in the neuropsychological assessment (>3 missing values led to exclusion, see above), the missing values were replaced by the median (matched for gender and age decades: 55–64, 65–74, 75–85 years). Performance tests included in the present study covered measures of 14 distinct cognitive functions: selective attention, processing speed, susceptibility to interference, figural fluency, problem solving, concept shifting, vocabulary, verbal fluency (semantic and phonemic versions), figural memory and verbal learning as well as visual, visual–spatial and verbal working memory.

Table 1 Overview of cognitive functions, tests and variables used, together with mean test scores and standard deviations (SD)

Statistical analysis

The overall relation between age and local gyrification indices in parts of the DMN was analyzed using multivariate ANCOVA (Analysis of CoVariance) with gender as covariate. Afterwards, post hoc partial correlation analyses were carried out to assess the influence of age on every single part of the DMN after correcting for gender. Reported results were significant at p < .05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate [FDR; (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995)].

For the relation between cognitive performance and local gyrification indices in parts of the DMN, the following steps were executed: First, all neuropsychological variables were rank-transformed as they were not normally distributed. Afterwards, the ranks were transformed into standard scores (using mean centering and scaling). We then calculated partial correlations between cognitive function scores and local gyrification indices while correcting for gender and age. All reported correlation results were significant at p < .05, FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons across the number of neuropsychological variables and brain regions. Finally, differences of correlation strength were tested for statistical significance using an adaption of Steiger’s Z (Steiger 1980; Hoerger 2013).

Subsequently, we re-runned all analysis by including the total score of the dementia screening test [DemTect, Kalbe et al. (2004)] as a covariate. Further, for the subsequent analysis of cognitive performance, we additionally excluded all subjects with missing values.

Results

Effects of age

To test for age-related changes of gyrification within the DMN, local gyrification indices for each part of the DMN were extracted. A three-way ANCOVA was applied to test for the effects of age and gender on local gyrification indices within the parts of the DMN and the interaction between ROI and age and gender. Afterwards a post hoc partial correlation analysis was conducted to investigate the effects of age on local gyrification indices for each part of the DMN while correcting for the effects of gender.

Three-way ANCOVA using ROI, age and gender as factors revealed significant main effects for ROI (F = 106.28, p < .001, df = 5), and age (F = 34.26, p < .001, df = 1), but not for gender (F = .98, p = .3228, df = 1). Further, the interaction between age and ROI was significant (F = 3.68, p = .0025, df = 5). No significant interaction between ROI and gender (F = .73, p = .5988, df = 5), age and gender (F = .09, p = .7607, df = 1) or ROI, age and gender was found (F = .39, p = .8593, df = 5). Thus, the influence of age on ROI was further analyzed using partial correlation analyses (gender as covariate).

Post hoc partial correlation analysis (FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons) revealed heterogeneous relations between local gyrification indices and age for the different parts of the DMN. Local gyrification index of the right mPFC showed decreases with age (r = −.128; p = .0004; Fig. 3d), while there was no significant effect of age on local gyrification index in the left mPFC (r = .015; p = .6853; Fig. 3a). For the posterior parts of the DMN, we found significant age-related decreases in local gyrification indices within the left (r = −.130; p = .0004; Fig. 3c) and right PCC/precuneus (r = −.132; p = .0003; Fig. 3f). In contrast, left (r = −.053; p = .1512; Fig. 3b) and right AG (r = .059; p = .1052; Fig. 3e) did not show significant decreases in local gyrification indices with increasing age. Figure 3 shows the relation between residual local gyrification indices (with gender regressed out of the raw local gyrification indices) and age. Thus, the posterior parts of the DMN showed a more pronounced age-related decrease in local gyrification indices than the anterior parts of DMN. Both right hemispheric medial DMN-ROIs showed age-related local gyrification index decline, while only the local gyrification index of the left PCC/precuneus was affected by age.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Left scatterplots of relation between local gyrification index (LGI; residuals, corrected for gender) and age, with regression lines and correlation coefficients. Right group comparison between youngest and oldest 25 % of the participants, with raw LGI values and statistical outcomes. Parts of the DMN that were affected by age, using overall and specific analyses are marked with an asterisk

To test whether the decrease in local gyrification indices in the posterior parts of the DMN were more pronounced than in the anterior parts of DMN and whether right hemispheric parts of the DMN showed more pronounced decreases in local gyrification indices than left hemispheric parts, differences of correlation strength were assessed (using average local gyrification indices across the respective parts of the DMN). We found that the posterior parts of the DMN showed a more pronounced decrease in local gyrification index as compared to the anterior parts of DMN (Z = 1.86, p = .032, one-tailed). Furthermore, the right hemispheric parts of the DMN showed a more pronounced decrease in local gyrification index decrease compared to the left hemispheric parts of the DMN (Z = 1.81; p = .035, one-tailed).

To illustrate the size of these age effects within the raw local gyrification index data, we calculated the mean local gyrification index for every ROI for the 25 % youngest (n = 186; male: 95; mean age: 58.9 years ± 1.37) as well as 25 % oldest subjects (n = 190; male: 120; mean age: 76.2 years ± 2.93). The age effects within the raw local gyrification index data confirm the results shown in the correlation analysis with a more pronounced local gyrification index decrease within the posterior parts of the DMN compared to the anterior parts of DMN and a more pronounced local gyrification index decrease within the right hemispheric parts of the DMN. Overview of raw scores and group comparisons (matched for group size, gender and age decades: 55–64, 65–74, 75–85 years) are presented in Fig. 3g–l.

In addition, we checked for influences of possible cognitive impairment on the effects of age on local gyrification index by adding the total score on a dementia screening test [DemTect, Kalbe et al. (2004)] as a covariate. From all correlations between local gyrification indices and age, and after correcting for multiple comparisons using FDR-correction (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995), the relations between age and local gyrification index still remained significant for the mPFC (r = −.125, p = .001); left PCC/precuneus (r = −.125, p = .001) and right PCC/precuneus (r = −.123, p = .001). Thus, even after correcting for global cognitive functioning, age effects on local gyrification index showed a more pronounced local gyrification index decrease within the posterior parts of the DMN and a more pronounced local gyrification index decrease within the right hemispheric parts of the DMN. The comparison between affected parts of the DMN by the overall analysis (without any covariates) and the specific analysis in which we corrected for the DemTect score is displayed in Fig. 5a.

Effects of cognitive performance

Partial correlations were calculated to assess the relation between cognitive performance and local gyrification indices within the ROIs of the DMN, while correcting for the effects of age and gender. Results were corrected for the number of correlations using FDR correction (6 ROIs × 14 neuropsychological tests).

Significant correlations were found between the local gyrification indices in the right mPFC, left and right PCC/precuneus as well as right AG and selective attention, visual and visual–spatial working memory and semantic verbal fluency. Figure 4 displays all significant correlations between cognitive performance and local gyrification indices. Selective attention test scores were negatively correlated with local gyrification index in the left PCC/precuneus (r = −.134; p = .0003). Visual working memory performance was positively correlated with both the left PCC/precuneus (r = .114; p = .0018) and the right AG (r = .113; p = .0019). Visual–spatial working memory performance was positively correlated with local gyrification index in the right PCC/precuneus (r = .108; p = .0032). Further, semantic verbal fluency was positively correlated with local gyrification indices of the right AG (r = .133; p = .0003) and right mPFC (r = .109; p = .0028). All other correlations did not reveal significant results.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Left scatterplots of the significant relations between local gyrification index (LGI) values (residuals, corrected for age and gender) and cognitive performance (residuals, corrected for age and gender), with regression lines and correlation coefficients. A higher cognitive performance relates to higher LGI values. Note selective attention was measured as time in seconds. Therefore, less time needed (higher performance) was associated with higher LGI values. Parts of the DMN that were affected by cognitive performance using overall and specific analyses are marked with an asterisk. Right group comparison between highest and lowest 25 % of the performers, with raw LGI values and statistical outcomes

In summary, the current results showed significant relations between cognitive performance and all right hemispheric DMN-ROIs. Only the local gyrification index of left PCC/precuneus showed significant correlations to cognitive performance. To illustrate the size of the effects of cognitive performance on local gyrification index, we calculated the mean local gyrification indices for every ROI for the 25 % best performing subjects as well as for the 25 % lowest performing subjects for every significant correlation described above (matched for group size, gender and age decades: 55–64, 65–74, 75–85 years). As a result, the effects of cognitive performance on the raw local gyrification index values confirmed the results derived from the correlation analysis since the local gyrification index decrease was present within all right hemispheric parts of the DMN and the left PCC/precuneus. Overview of raw scores of cognitive performance and group descriptives are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Mean test scores (raw scores) and group descriptives for the 25 % highest performers and 25 % lowest performers and group comparisons

To further check whether the main results were biased by those subjects, for whom missing values in the neuropsychological assessment had to be imputed from their age- and gender-specific peer groups (n = 67, subjects with more than three missing values were excluded), analyses were repeated without subjects having missing values, including the DemTect score as covariate. Of the six correlations between local gyrification indices and cognitive performance, three were still significant after exclusion of the subjects with missing values (corrected for multiple comparisons using FDR correction), i.e. between selective attention and local gyrification index in the left PCC/precuneus (r = −.138, p = .0003), visual–spatial working memory and local gyrification index in the right PCC/precuneus (r = .125, p = .0011), and semantic verbal fluency and local gyrification index in the right mPFC (r = .138, p = .0003). The comparison between affected parts of the DMN by the overall analysis (including all subjects) and the specific analysis in which we excluded subjects with missing values and corrected for the DemTect score is displayed in Fig. 5b.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Parts of the DMN that were affected by age and cognitive performance, using overall and specific analyses. Left overall analysis of age (including all subjects, covariate: gender) and specific analysis of age (including all subjects, covariates: gender and DemTect). Right overall analysis of cognitive performance (including all subjects, covariate: age and gender) and specific analysis (excluding subjects with missing values in the neuropsychological assessment, covariates: age, gender and DemTect)

Discussion

The current study assessed the relation between local gyrification indices and age as well as cognitive performance within the DMN-ROIs (left and right mPFC, PCC/precuneus and AG) in a population-based cohort of older adults. Remarkably, age and cognitive performance showed similar relations to local gyrification indices across the different parts of the DMN: (1) local gyrification indices within posterior parts of DMN showed a stronger relation to age and cognitive performance compared to anterior parts of DMN; and (2) local gyrification indices of right hemispheric DMN-ROIs was stronger correlated with age and cognitive performance than that of the left hemispheric DMN-ROIs.

Effects of age

In the current study, age-related decreases in local cortical folding were found for right mPFC and bilateral PCC/precuneus. According to previous studies, increasing age leads to increasing brain atrophy, with a similar progression in the left and right hemisphere (Hogstrom et al. 2013; Kochunov et al. 2005, 2008; Liu et al. 2010; Magnotta et al. 1999; Rettmann et al. 2006; Zilles et al. 1988). This is in line with the gyrification decreases found in the current study, but focused on age-related gyrification decline across different multimodal brain areas encompassed in the DMN. On the one hand, posterior parts of DMN showed a larger age-related local gyrification index decrease than the anterior parts of the DMN. On the other hand, local gyrification index decrease of right hemispheric parts of the DMN was more pronounced than that of the left hemispheric DMN. This hints at local, rather than global age-related decreases of cortical folding within the DMN.

Previous studies discovered changes of the functional organization from early to late adulthood within the DMN (Damoiseaux et al. 2008; Gould et al. 2006; Mowinckel et al. 2012; Sambataro et al. 2010). Interestingly, most of these studies showed increases in resting-state functional connectivity within the anterior part of the DMN. Additionally, age-related decreases of PCC/precuneus activity have been shown (Schlee et al. 2012). As proposed by Davis et al. (2008), the increase in anterior DMN deactivation together with less deactivations of the PCC/precuneus is part of the PASA theory. So, in the brain of older adults the medial PCC/precuneus shows lower (de-) activations as compared to younger subjects. In line with this, PCC/precuneus was found to be vulnerable to age-related gray matter loss (especially within Brodmann area 31) (Mann et al. 2011). With respect to the anterior parts of the DMN, Davis et al. (2008) reported increases in anterior DMN deactivation during task performance in older adults. Thus, while the posterior parts of the DMN seem to show structural decline in older adults, increase in anterior parts of the DMN activation was interpreted as a compensatory mechanism to counteract the structural decline of the posterior parts of the DMN. Following, increases in anterior DMN (de-) activation might be a compensatory attempt to maintain cognitive performance as stable as possible (Ansado et al. 2012; Davis et al. 2008). The current results presented a similar picture, with more pronounced atrophy in PCC/precuneus as compared to mPFC (as measured via decreases in local gyrification index). Thus, the results of the present study provide evidence for a structural correlate of the functionally motivated PASA theory of effects of aging on the brain.

As a second prominent finding, the current study revealed a more pronounced local gyrification index decrease of the right hemispheric parts of the DMN (right: mPFC and PCC/precuneus; left: only PCC/precuneus). Previous research indicated that the right hemisphere might be more prone to age-related decline compared to the left hemisphere, the so-called right-hemi-aging hypothesis (Grady, et al. 1994). Hemispheric asymmetries have been previously reported with regard to structural and functional changes during brain aging. For example, Kovalev et al. (2003) reported accelerated brain aging for right ACC, using MRI texture analysis. Further, Lu et al. (2010) showed that age-related decreases in resting state activity from 20 to 89 years of age were most notably present within right PFC. Interestingly, when assessing the PASA theory, Davis et al. (2008) found more pronounced deactivation in left mPFC (especially left ACC), which provided evidence for better preserved functioning of left as compared to right hemispheric areas. In line with this, our results of the present study revealed age-related decreases in local gyrification index for right, but not left mPFC. Thus, left mPFC might be an important driving factor for any compensatory process, as it is less affected by age-related atrophy, supporting the right hemi-aging theory. Nevertheless, it has to be noted, that the right-hemi-aging hypothesis is mainly based on behavioral observations showing that right-hemispheric cognitive processes, such as spatial processing are markedly affected during aging. Thus, further research is necessary to explore the relationship between cognitive decline according to the right hemi-aging theory, functional reorganization in the aging brain, and age-related structural atrophy, as reflected by the results of the present study.

Taken together, the current results provided evidence for more pronounced decrease in local gyrification index in the posterior as compared to the anterior parts of the DMN—supporting the functionally based PASA theory—as well as in right as compared to left hemispheric parts of the DMN—supporting the right-hemi-aging model.

Effects of cognitive performance on local gyrification indices

In the current study, significant correlations between local gyrification indices and cognitive performance were found in largely the same parts of the DMN as already found for the correlation of local gyrification index with age. Performance in the domain of attention was positively correlated with local gyrification index of left PCC/precuneus. Better working memory performance correlated with a higher local gyrification index of PCC/precuneus and right AG, and better performance in language performance positively correlated with local gyrification index in right mPFC and right AG.

Like for the age-related decreases in local gyrification indices, cognitive performance was more pronouncedly related to local gyrification indices in posterior as compared to anterior parts of the DMN. Local gyrification index decreases of posterior parts of the DMN (bilateral PCC/precuneus and right AG) were related to deficits in selective attention, as well as visual and visual–spatial working memory performance. Functionally, the DMN as such and the PCC/precuneus in particular, seem to be strongly involved in attentional processes (Buckner et al. 2008, 2009). Moreover, the involvement of the AG in visual processing has also been explored. For example, the transmitter receptor distribution of AG is similar to that of the extrastriate visual brain areas, indicating the AG to be involved in visual processing (Caspers et al. 2013). During aging, deficits in early visual processing stages seem to impair visual working memory performance (Gazzaley et al. 2008; Gazzaley and Nobre 2012). Further, it was shown that a lack of desynchronization of brain oscillatory activity within PCC/precuneus and right AG disrupts the efficient processing within the fronto-parietal working memory network, leading to a decline in visual working memory performance (Pinal et al. 2015).

Thus, the current study revealed cognitive performance-related local gyrification index decreases in the posterior parts of the DMN (bilateral PCC/precuneus and right AG). The bilateral PCC/precuneus and right AG were functionally shown to be involved in the processing of visual working memory and visual attention. Additionally, subjects with a lower local gyrification in these brain areas in our sample also showed a lower performance in visual working memory and attention. Relating the current study to the aforementioned functional imaging study, it might be assumed that low performers also show functional impairments in brain networks involved in visual working memory and visual attention. This observation would be also in line with the PASA hypothesis, since PASA suggests a more pronounced atrophy within posterior brain regions to be associated with cognitive performance decline. However, further research is necessary to test the exact function-structure relationship with respect to PASA.

Further, the correlations between local gyrification decreases and cognitive performance complement those found for age: gyrification in posterior as compared to anterior parts of the DMN were not only more prone to age-related alterations, but were also found to relate to specific cognitive performance decline. Thus, the present results hint at a structural correlate of the functionally established PASA theory in a large cohort of older adults in two aspects: (1) with regard to an integrated view on structure–function reorganization in the older adults’ brain; and (2) in relation to its behavioral performance outcome.

Similar to the age effects, the relation between cognitive performance and local gyrification revealed a second prominent finding: local gyrification indices of all right hemispheric DMN-ROIs were correlated to cognitive performance decline, as compared to only a relation between local gyrification index of the PCC/precuneus in the left hemisphere. This, again, is in line with the right hemi-aging theory. The local gyrification indices of the right PCC/precuneus and AG were associated with visual (-spatial) working memory. The right hemi-aging theory, which is mainly built on behavioral observations, states a prominent and earlier decline of visual–spatial cognitive processes, compared to language processes (Dolcos et al. 2002). The authors explained these effects by an earlier and more pronounced decline of right hemispheric functions, which should be associated with earlier and more prominent changes of the structural or functional architecture of the right as compared to the left hemisphere. The current results fit to this hypothesis, because the right PCC/precuneus and AG were positively correlated to visual–spatial working memory performance. Although the right hemi-aging theory is widely debated, especially in functional imaging [e.g. Dolcos et al. (2002)] the current results are further supported by Li et al. (2009). In this study, structural and functional connectivity was investigated in older adults. A right hemispheric fronto-parietal structural connectivity reduction was found in older compared to younger adults. Our results might add to this finding in that we found predominant grey matter atrophy in right as compared to left fronto-parietal regions. In addition, more pronounced atrophy of white matter in the right as compared to the left hemisphere, especially in the right frontal lobe, was associated with verbal and nonverbal cognitive performance decline (Brickman et al. 2006). The results of the present study further amend these previous findings by showing that also cortical structure of left and right hemisphere might be differentially affected during aging. As this was shown here for the default mode network, future studies might yield further insight if right hemi-aging is a global or rather local process during aging, at this differentially affecting cognitive performance in older adults. Additionally, intra-subject trajectories of structural changes of right versus left hemispheric regions could be assessed in a longitudinal design to further elucidate if aging processes indeed happen earlier in the right than in the left hemisphere.

Interestingly, local gyrification indices of the right mPFC and AG were positively correlated to performance in semantic verbal fluency. Functionally, the AG has strongly been associated with semantic verbal fluency (Binder et al. 2009; Shalom and Poeppel 2008; for a recent review see Seghier 2013). Nevertheless, age-related decline of the anterior part of the DMN (i.e. ACC) has also been shown to be correlated to verbal fluency scores (Pardo et al. 2007). However, contrary to the current results, previous studies showed a more prominent involvement of left mPFC and AG in semantic verbal fluency tasks (Binder et al. 2009; Wagner et al. 2014). Even though the relation between local gyrification indices of right mPFC and AG and cognitive performance seems to fit with the right hemi-aging hypothesis, it remains questionable why only the right hemispheric DMN-ROIs related to cognitive performance in a task which is known to involve mainly left hemispheric brain areas. Most of the aforementioned studies focused on subjects younger than those included in the current study. It is known in older adults, though, that a more bilateral recruitment of brain regions is necessary to maintain performance in cognitively demanding tasks as high as in younger subjects (HAROLD-Model; Hemispheric-asymmetry-reduction-in-older-adults) (Cabeza 2002). Relating this to the current results, it might be assumed that right AG and mPFC gain importance in semantic verbal fluency performance in older adults. Consequently, a more pronounced atrophy of these brain regions (according to the right-hemi-aging theory) would lead to a loss of relevant additional functional recruitment (according to HAROLD), and would impair performance in the older adults. In summary, similar to the age-related local gyrification index decreases within the DMN, local gyrification index of posterior as compared to anterior parts of the DMN showed stronger correlations to cognitive performance decline, supporting PASA. Furthermore, local gyrification index of all right hemispheric parts of the DMN correlated to cognitive performance decline, strengthening the right-hemi-aging model.

Finally, it has to be mentioned that the current study was designed as a population-based study. Thus, we did not exclude subjects based on any criteria except from claustrophobia and contraindications to the MR. Since some of the participants had missing values on the neuropsychological assessment (n = 67) we re-ran the correlation analyses of cognitive performance and local gyrification indices, while excluding these participants from the analysis. In addition, it might be possible that global cognitive functioning had an influence. Therefore, we additionally included the DemTect as a covariate in the analysis of age as well as cognitive performance (see above on the discussion of “Effects of age”). This additional analysis revealed an interesting finding: the same DMN-ROIs that were affected by the factor “age” were affected by cognitive performance (cf. Fig. 5). Thus, the more conservative approach including only those subjects who had all neuropsychological test data available, re-focused the findings to a core of brain regions in which local gyrification was robustly associated with cognitive performance. This stepwise inclusion of potential influencing factors, which might be relevant when studying relation between cortical folding and age or cognitive performance, thus further strengthens the idea that there might be a structural correlate for the PASA as well as for the right hemi-aging theory. It remains for future studies to further establish if and in which groups of subjects cognitive performance might be additionally associated with structural brain changes, as could be assumed based on our results when including all subjects in the analyses, for whom stratified neuropsychological test scores were imputed.

Methodological issues

The current study has several advantages and limitations which should be addressed. First, the effects of age as well as cognitive performance only explained small parts of the total variance in this sample. Although results are statistically significant, even after correction for multiple comparisons, the factors “age” and “cognitive performance” could not explain the same amount of variance in local gyrification as it was shown for the whole lifespan from early to late adulthood (Hogstrom et al. 2013). However, it has to be noted that in comparison to other studies, investigating young versus older adults, the current sample investigated age effects in a sample consisting of older adults only. Previous studies already showed that the effects of age on brain structure do not necessarily follow a linear relationship from early to late life, with smaller rates of brain atrophy in older adults [e.g. Sowell et al. (2003)]. The reliable detection of subtle, but relevant, effects in local gyrification requires large sample sizes (Button et al. 2013). Given the large number of older adults investigated (i.e. 55 years and above), the present study did indeed have sufficient power to demonstrate small effects of age and cognitive performance on local gyrification in a population-based sample of older adults. These effects might nevertheless provide relevant impact on brain structure in older adults as part of the inter-individual variability, together with other factors that might influence brain atrophy in older subjects, such as genetic variations, environment or lifestyle. With each of them having only a small effect, they all could add up to finally give an overall picture of the inter-individual variability of brain phenotypes in late decades of life. Further studies are warranted to assess the particular relevance of these significant but small effects for structural or functional reorganization in the aging brain.

A second important point that has to be discussed is the population-based design of the present study. The examined cohort represents a high inter-individual variability across normal older adults. In contrast to other study designs, e.g. case–control designs, the advantage of population-based cohort studies is to assess influencing factors for brain aging in the normal population, with a high generalizability. While exploring such influences in the population, a basis of the normal aging process of the brain can be established, which is necessary to better characterize the normal range of aging within the assumed continuous spectrum from health to disease (Tucker and Stern 2011). However, taking such a global approach across the general population could bear the chance of showing only global effects, while more specific findings might be disregarded, e.g. an effect such as global cognitive impairment Therefore, using a stepwise inclusion/exclusion approach of factors that might bias the results is of particular interest in a population-based design such as the present study. We used this stepwise approach by including additional covariates, such as the DemTect, and by excluding participants who had missing values in the neuropsychological data since it helped to further differentiate between overall and more specific effects (cf. Fig. 5).

Moreover, the cross-sectional design of the present study did not allow for investigating the intra-individual course of cognitive decline and changes in local gyrification index during aging. Future research on longitudinal data could thus further elucidate the individual trajectories of local gyrification index in older adults in relation to age and cognitive performance. Furthermore, the current study draws inferences on the local gyrification index of DMN regions. It remains for future studies to investigate regional susceptibility of local gyrification index to age and cognitive performance in other cognitive brain networks, e.g. frontoparietal resting state networks.

Conclusions

The current study assessed gyrification of the DMN in relation to age and cognitive performance in an older adults’ population-based sample. Age-related structural decline was more pronounced within posterior as compared to anterior parts of the DMN, providing a structural correlate for the functionally established PASA theory. Further, the prominent structural decline in right hemispheric ROIs of the DMN is in accordance with the right hemi-aging model. Beyond age, cognitive performance-related changes in local gyrification index were present in largely the same parts of the DMN that were already more vulnerable to age-related local gyrification decreases. First, local gyrification index decreases in posterior parts of the DMN were related to performance decline in visual (-spatial) working memory and selective attention, supporting the PASA theory. Second, local gyrification index decreases of right hemispheric DMN-ROIs were correlated to performance decline in semantic verbal fluency, supporting the right hemi-aging-theory. Thus, age and cognitive performance-related local gyrification index decreases provide structural correlates for PASA and the right-hemi aging theory. The present study therefore suggests common underlying processes of reorganization in the older adults brain, encompassing brain structure and function as well as related performance outcome.