Introduction

The potential role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia has been highlighted by the “cognitive dysmetria” hypothesis (Andreasen et al. 1996, 1999; Schmahmann 1991) which suggests an impaired coordination of sensorimotor and mental processes. In particular, cognitive dysmetria has been related to thought disorders. Thought disorders, rather than the more obvious signs, such as delusions and hallucinations, have been described as a primary feature of schizophrenia by Bleuler (1911). Previous studies reported volumetric abnormalities of the cerebellum in patients suffering from schizophrenia compared to healthy controls. Most of the studies showed reductions in cerebellar volume in patients (Barak et al. 2002; Heath et al. 1979; Nasrallah et al. 1982; Weinberger et al. 1982) but, in contrast, some others reported cerebellar volume increases (Keller et al. 2003; Levitt et al. 1999). The majority of the early volumetric studies used region-of-interest (ROI) analyses in which total cerebellar volume or substructures of the cerebellum such as the vermis were delineated manually or automatically. More recent studies relied on whole-brain analyses using voxel-based morphometry (VBM), providing a more exhaustive and unbiased assessment of structural differences. Some of these VBM studies have reported cerebellar abnormalities in schizophrenia patients (Chua et al. 2007; Venkatasubramanian 2010). Several quantitative meta-analyses of VBM studies exploring structural changes in brains of schizophrenia patients aimed at identifying regions that were consistently found as being altered. However, these analyses reported no convergence of evidence within the cerebellum, neither in grey (Cheung et al. 2010; Fornito et al. 2009; Glahn et al. 2008) nor in white matter (Di et al. 2009). This overall lack of convergence in quantitative meta-analyses stands in sharp contrast to a more consistent pattern in ROI-based analyses, which could be explained by methodological problems. For VBM methods, the small size of the subdivisions in the cerebellum poses a particular challenge to inter-subject normalization (Diedrichsen et al. 2010). The results rely on accurate normalization of cerebellum which has been shown to be suboptimal in custom analyses (Diedrichsen 2006; Diedrichsen et al. 2009). Recently, a spatially unbiased template has been introduced that addresses these problems. It improves the overlap of the major fissures (e.g., the primary fissure) in the cerebellum between subjects by reducing the spatial variance to one-third (Diedrichsen 2006). Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to employ the Spatially Unbiased Infratentorial (SUIT) toolbox (Diedrichsen 2006) in conjunction with VBM in order to provide an optimized assessment of structural alterations in the cerebellum of schizophrenia patients and their association with the psychopathology of thought disorders and cognitive performance.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-nine schizophrenia patients were compared to 45 healthy control subjects. Demographic and additional data describing patients and control subjects are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of schizophrenic patients and healthy control subjects studied

Patients were recruited among inpatients (n = 13) and outpatients (n = 16) of the Charité Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Mitte. To confirm the initial previous diagnosis by an independent psychiatrist, a Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders (First et al. 1996) was performed by an experienced senior consultant psychiatrist. 27 patients had been treated with risperidone at the time of the scan (17 had been treated <1 year, 7 longer than 1 year and 4 longer than 5 years; mean dose 4.1 ± 1.56 mg). Three of the patients received benzodiazepines additionally. The duration of illness was assessed as the time in years between the first episode and the current investigation. Patients with other psychiatric Axis I disorders were excluded.

Control subjects were recruited by means of newspaper advertisements. According to personal interviews (Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview, Sheehan et al. 1998) carried out by a psychiatrist, healthy subjects were free of medical, neurological and psychiatric disorders. Control subjects with a family history (first degree) of Axis I disorder were excluded. In addition, exclusion criteria for all subjects were abnormalities in the MRI, general medical disorders, or other clinically relevant abnormalities. The study was approved by the ethics committee of Charité University Clinic Germany. After complete description of the study, the subjects gave a written informed consent.

Symptom measurement and cognitive tests

To assess the severity of schizophrenia symptoms, we used the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS, Overall and Gorham 1962). We administered several cognitive tests including the Trail-making test (TMT A and B, Reitan and Wolfson 1996) providing information on visual search, scanning, speed of processing, mental flexibility and executive control; the Stroop task (Stroop 1935) a test of attentional and executive control; two word fluency test (TWFT): naming animals (Goodglass and Kaplan 1972) and naming words with the letter “S” (Thurstone 1938); the continuous performance test (CPT, Mackworth and Taylor 1963) assessing sustained and selective attention; and a picture memory test (participants saw line drawings of 20 known objects for 30 s and had to name those that they remembered immediately after the 30 s).

Scanning procedure

MR measurements were carried out on a 3-Tesla scanner (MEDSPEC 30/100, Bruker Biospin, Ettlingen, Germany). T1-weighted images were acquired using MDEFT (modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform, with T E = 3.8 ms, T R = 20.53 ms; T I = 550 ms, nominal flip angle 30°; 128 contiguous slices, 1.5 mm thick; 1-mm inplane (xy) resolution).

Data analysis

First, study-specific templates were built to compensate scanner specific contrast differences and non-uniformities as well as demographic differences between our current sample and the sample from which the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) templates were created. By means of the isolate function within the SUIT toolbox (http://www.icn.ucl.ac.uk/motorcontrol/imaging/suit.htm, Diedrichsen 2006), we isolated infra-tentorial structures, namely cerebellum and brainstem, from the surrounding tissue. Then a new prior probability distribution defined in the SUIT space was used. Because of the improved anatomical alignment, this prior probability distribution is much more clearly defined than the one used in the first iteration, therefore improving the classification results. The segmented grey matter (GM) images were then normalized to the SUIT template. A modulation of the segmented GM probability map was undertaken to compensate for volume changes during the spatial normalization by multiplying the intensity value in each voxel with the Jacobian determinants. Finally, the resulting GM probability images were smoothed with a 4 mm full-width half maximum (FWHM) kernel in SPM5 to satisfy the Gaussian distribution assumption for statistical analysis of regional differences. The use of the small 4 mm smoothing kernel is in line with previous publications that focussed on cerebellum VBM by means of the SUIT toolbox (D’Agata et al. 2011; Fan et al. 2010). All images were visually inspected to ensure that the pre-processing steps were successful and that the quality of each image was acceptable for subsequent analysis. Because we found significant differences in age, cigarettes per day and the distribution of sex between patients and healthy controls (Table 1), we controlled for brain volume (cerebellum and brainstem) age, sex and cigarettes per day in our analysis. The resulting maps were thresholded with p < 0.001 and cluster size corrected by means of Monte Carlo simulation. Accordingly, significant effects were reported when the volume of the cluster was greater than the minimum cluster size determined by Monte Carlo simulation on the cerebellum GM volume (>120 voxels), above which the probability of type I error was below 0.01 (AlphaSim, Ward 2000). Anatomical localizations (i.e., cerebellar lobules) were determined by the probabilistic MRI atlas of the human cerebellum developed by Diedrichsen et al. (2009).

In order to compare our cerebellum results to those of common whole-brain VBM analyses we computed the same analysis (controlling for age, sex, cigarettes per day and brain volume) on GM maps based on the optimized VBM procedure. This procedure spatially normalizes GM-segmented images to a standard space by matching images to their GM template (Good et al. 2001). We used the same smoothing kernel of 4 mm FWHM as in the previous analysis but a lenient threshold of p < 0.001 (uncorrected), in order to obtain a high sensitivity in detecting GM effects in the cerebellum.

We extracted the GM volume of each participant within the regions of significant difference between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls (in left Crus I/II) by means of MarsBaR (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net). The correlation analysis for cerebellar volume deficit regions and clinical or cognitive parameters was performed using Pearson’s correlation test.

Results

To explore structural differences between schizophrenia patients and healthy control subjects in the cerebellum we contrasted the GM volume maps against one another. We controlled for age, sex, brain volume and cigarette consumption. We found that schizophrenia patients had lower GM volume than healthy controls in left cerebellum Crus I/II (−36, −64, −48, cluster size 505 voxels, cluster corrected at p < 0.01, Fig. 1). There were no regions where GM volume was significantly increased in schizophrenia patients compared to healthy controls.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Significant cluster of cerebellar grey matter volume reduction in left Crus I and II (blue; −36, −64, −48) in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy controls (p values corrected for multiple comparisons, p < 0.01). Top coronal slice, bottom left sagittal slice, bottom right 3D rendering of the cerebellum

In order to relate the cerebellar abnormality to clinical symptoms, we correlated GM volume in Crus I/II with the factor thought disorder of the BPRS. We found a negative correlation between thought disorder and left Crus I/II [r (26) = −0.383, p < 0.05, one-sided, Fig. 2]. There were no significant correlations between left Crus I/II and other factor scores of the BPRS.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scatter plot of grey matter volumes extracted from left Crus I/II and BPRS thought disorder subscale [r (26) = −0.383, p < 0.05, one-sided] in schizophrenic patients

Since cerebellar volume has been associated with cognition, we aimed at exploring whether reduced GM volume in Crus I/II is correlated with cognitive performance measures in schizophrenia patients. We found a significant positive correlation between the percent rank in TMT B and GM volume in left Crus I/II [r (26) = 0.55, p < 0.01, Fig. 3], indicating that slower performance was associated with less GM volume but not for TMT A [r (26) = −0.03, p = 0.90]. Both correlation coefficients are significantly different from one another (z = 2.2, p < 0.05; Hinkle et al. 1988).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatter plot of grey matter volumes extracted from left Crus I/II and Trail-making test B [r (26) = 0.55, p < 0.01] in schizophrenic patients

To compare the results of our cerebellum-optimized VBM procedure to regular whole-brain VBM results in the cerebellum, we analyzed our data with an optimized VBM protocol (Good et al. 2001) but with the same smoothing kernel as in the cerebellum-optimized analysis. In line with several meta-analyses on VBM GM changes in schizophrenia (Cheung et al. 2010; Fornito et al. 2009; Glahn et al. 2008), we found no increases or reductions in GM volume in the cerebellum in this analysis (p < 0.001, uncorrected). No cluster corrected results were found in the whole-brain VBM analysis outside the cerebellum.

Discussion

The present study focussed on structural alteration in cerebellum of schizophrenia patients as compared to healthy controls using a cerebellum-optimized VBM protocol that allows a highly sensitive investigation of cerebellar GM volume. The results show a volume deficit in a particular region of the left Crus I/II in schizophrenia patients predicting the amount of thought disorder in the current psychopathology and performance in the TMT B.

Previous studies that have segmented the entire cerebellum or large substructures of the cerebellum, manually or automatically, frequently reported decreases in cerebellar volume in schizophrenia patients (for an overview: Andreasen and Pierson 2008; Konarski et al. 2005; Picard et al. 2008). A disadvantage of these ROI-based approaches is that the localization within the cerebellum is not very precise; the results are reported mostly for the entire cerebellar hemisphere or vermis.

Recent advances in the analysis of structural brain images introduced whole-brain VBM methods that test for local differences in GM volume on the whole brain level. Although some studies reported VBM findings in the cerebellum of schizophrenia patients (Chua et al. 2007; Venkatasubramanian 2010) the localization of these abnormalities seems to be inconsistent. Quantitative meta-analyses testing for consistent findings across whole-brain VBM studies comparing grey and white matter in schizophrenia patients against healthy controls failed to find overlap in the cerebellum (Cheung et al. 2010; Fornito et al. 2009; Glahn et al. 2008; Di et al. 2009). The VBM results highly depend on accurate normalization. This is problematic for results in the cerebellum, because especially the normalization of the cerebellum and brainstem has been shown to be difficult in custom neuroimaging analysis (Diedrichsen 2006; Diedrichsen et al. 2009).

Our results are in line with these methodological considerations: we found no differences in the cerebellum when processing the structural images according to a common optimized VBM procedures (Good et al. 2001), but we found clear differences when using a cerebellum-optimized VBM procedure (based on the SUIT toolbox, Diedrichsen 2006). The observed reduction of GM volume in left Crus I/II of the cerebellum in schizophrenia patients is in accordance with previous ROI-based studies that reported reductions in the entire cerebellum (Barak et al. 2002; Nasrallah et al. 1982; Weinberger et al. 1982; Keller et al. 2003). A recent study that explored regional cerebellar differences between first-episode schizophrenic patients and controls using cortical pattern matching (Rasser et al. 2010) reports more widespread group differences within the cerebellum including the region of difference, which we observed within Crus I/II.

The absence of whole-brain VBM differences in the cerebrum could be due to our moderate sample size and is at least partly in line with the fact that some studies report GM volume decreases in schizophrenic patients, but others report increases (Glahn et al. 2008; Honea et al. 2005) and the regions in which alterations have been observed are diverse (Honea et al. 2005).

Although the role of the cerebellum has traditionally been seen in the coordination of motor functions, recently non-motor functions such as cognition and emotion have been associated with the cerebellum (Strick et al. 2009). In particular, Crus I and II have been related to cognitive functions. A meta-analysis on functional neuroimaging studies with healthy participants has associated Crus I with verbal working memory and executive functions (Stoodley and Schmahmann 2010). Studies comparing cerebellar activity of schizophrenia patients and healthy controls demonstrated reduced activity during recall of facial pictures (Andreasen and Pierson 2008), working memory (Walter et al. 2007) and recall of practiced wordlists (Crespo-Facorro et al. 1999) in close proximity to the region in which we found GM volume reductions. The “cognitive dysmetria” hypothesis (Andreasen et al. 1996, 1998, 1999) assumes that the cerebellum of schizophrenia patients is less efficient in error detection and in relaying updated information to the cerebral cortex, thus causing symptoms like disordered thought, hallucinations and deficiencies in cognitive task performance.

Based on these functional associations between Crus I/II and cognition and based on the “cognitive dysmetria” hypothesis, we explored correlations between the GM volume within left Crus I/II and symptoms of the BPRS as well as several cognitive tasks within the sample of schizophrenia patients. We found a negative correlation with the symptom-scale thought disorder. The term thought disorders is used to describe patterns of disordered language use that are presumed to reflect disordered thinking. The language is characterized by abnormalities, such as tangentiality, neologisms, flight of ideas, paraphrasias and private use of words (Andreasen 1997). In schizophrenia formal thought disorder has been related to impairments in semantic and executive components as well as self monitoring during language processing (Kerns and Berenbaum 2002). Previous studies have reported associations between thought disorder and cerebellum in schizophrenia patients. Levitt et al. (1999) found a positive correlation between vermis white matter volume and symptoms of thought disorder. Kircher et al. (2001) reported a positive correlation between neural activity of patients while talking about Rorschach inkblots in cerebellar vermis and formal thought disorder. The association between cerebellar abnormalities and formal thought disorder is in accordance with assumptions of the “cognitive dysmetria” hypothesis suggesting that online monitoring of thoughts is impaired or mistimed, leading to a disruption of fluid and coordinated sequences of thought (Andreasen 1999). But the correlation observed does not survive Bonferroni correction when accounting for multiple testing. Future research is needed to explore the relationship between cerebellar volume and thought disorder a priori.

Furthermore, we found that better performance in the Trail-making B test was associated with higher GM volume in left Crus I/II. Being one of the common neuropsychological tests to assess the executive function of mental set shifting, the TMT B is required to search for and connect targets containing digits and letters with a pencil in ascending order, alternating between digits and letters. In order to exclude that the observed results are due to neurological soft signs that have been reported in schizophrenia (Chan and Gottesman 2008; Dazzan et al. 2008; Gupta et al. 1995), we compared the results to the association between Crus I/II and TMT A. TMT A involves the connection of targets but consisting of digits only, so that the task switching component is absent but the motoric component is present as well. Interestingly, we only found a significant association between GM in Crus I/II and the TMT B, not TMT A. Hence, we conclude that the observed association is based on the task switching and therefore cognitive components not on the motoric component of the task. Impaired performance in schizophrenia has been reported at different stages of illness (Bilder et al. 2000; Liddle and Morris 1991; Riley et al. 2000) as well as for healthy relatives of schizophrenia patients (Franke et al. 1993), suggesting the test’s potential as a vulnerability marker. The association of this behavioral vulnerability marker with the GM volume reduction in left Crus I/II indicates its possible role in schizophrenia.

To conclude, the uncovering of effects with the cerebellum-optimized procedure, in contrast to the failure of a common whole-brain VBM analysis to detect such effects, suggests that previous studies exploring structural abnormalities might have underestimated the importance of alterations in the cerebellum of schizophrenia patients. Further research on larger samples of schizophrenia patients is needed to establish connections between thought disorder, cognitive task performance and localized cerebellar GM volume reductions.