Introduction

Novel landscapes can expose individuals to challenges that may substantially alter their behavior (Sol et al. 2013; Alberti et al. 2017; Lapiedra et al. 2017). This action is typified by the behavioral shifts of animals living within urban environments (Shochat et al. 2006; Garroway and Sheldon 2013; Lowry et al. 2013; Sol et al. 2013), and has been documented in birds (Atwell et al. 2012), insects (Schuett et al. 2018), mammals (Lyons et al. 2017), reptiles (Peterman and Ryan 2009), and spiders (Kralj-Fišer et al. 2017). These urban-derived divergent behaviors can include altered anti-predator responses (McCleery 2009; Blumstein 2014), foraging behavior (Geggie and Fenton 1985; Shochat et al. 2004; Short and Petren 2008), increased problem-solving ability (Sol et al. 2011), behavioral thermoregulation (Peterman and Ryan 2009), and mate communication (Parris et al. 2009; Barnett 2015). Furthermore, changes in behavioral traits (e.g., boldness, neophilia, and exploration) may also provide advantages to navigating and exploiting urban environments (Kralj-Fišer et al. 2017). Boldness reflects an individual’s propensity to take risks; bolder individuals may be more active in novel, urban landscapes and situations, which could increase their time spent foraging, mate searching, or defending a territory (Réale et al. 2007; Sol et al. 2013; Sprau and Dingemanse 2017). Similarly, neophilia—an individual’s willingness to engage with novel stimuli or objects—could provide substantial advantages within an urban environment by increasing their ability to exploit novel resources (e.g., food sources or shelter; Bókony et al. 2012; Miranda et al. 2013). Finally, an individual’s propensity to explore could influence their success in urban environments by increasing their ability to disperse across novel landscapes (Damas-Moreira et al. 2019) and to gather important environmental information (e.g., identifying refuge, basking, and perching locations in novel environments; Lapiedra et al. 2017).

Although altered behavior and behavioral traits have been documented in numerous urban-living species, the specific mechanisms driving the formation of urban-derived divergent behavioral traits are largely unclear (but see Miranda et al. 2013). Behavioral plasticity has been suggested to aid urban-dwelling individuals increase their exploitation of urban resources and decrease the costs associated with urban habitats (Ditchkoff et al. 2006; Partecke et al. 2006; French et al. 2008; Atwell et al. 2012; Lucas and French 2012; Lampe et al. 2014; Kralj-Fišer et al. 2017). Alternatively, if these behavioral traits are heritable, and provide an advantage in urban environments, then selection may favor them in urban populations. Recent research has suggested that urban evolution is driving the persistence of species in heavily human-modified habitats (Johnson and Munshi-South 2017). Yet, even though divergent behavior in urban-living populations is likely adaptive, it is unclear if these behaviors are a result of selection or plasticity (Diamond 1986; Møller 2008; Lowry et al. 2013; Sol et al. 2013; Alberti et al. 2017). Recently, heritability of behavioral traits (e.g., boldness and aggression) has been documented for several urban-dwelling bird species (Evans et al. 2010; Müller et al. 2013; Holtmann et al. 2017; Sprau and Dingemanse 2017). Flight-capable birds, however, should experience weaker selection within urban environments compared with terrestrial species, because they are able to rapidly vacate urban habitats leading to increased gene flow. In contrast, less vagile terrestrial species are physically tied to specific locations within urban environments (Brown 1978; Wiens and Donoghue 2004; Lyons et al. 2017), and thus may experience stronger selection. Research into heritable behavioral traits in terrestrial urban species remains rare (but see Kralj-Fišer and Schneider 2012), but they are a study system that could greatly enhance our understanding of the full extent to which urban environments are shaping animal behavior.

The Australian water dragon (Intellagama lesueurii) is an agamid lizard species found throughout eastern Australia (Cogger 2014). Water dragons are common in urban areas and appear to have successfully exploited human-altered landscapes, where some populations have experienced rapid morphological evolution (Littleford-Colquhoun et al. 2017). This species is therefore a good model for testing whether behavior may play a role in their success in urban environments. Specifically, we used a common garden experiment, which removed the confounding effects of rearing environment and prior experience, to test whether urban environments are favoring particular heritable behavioral traits or whether behavior is best explained by experience. We raised hatchling dragons from eggs collected from mothers living in urban, semi-natural, and natural populations, and repeatedly quantified their behavioral traits (boldness, neophilia, and exploration) over the first year of life. We predicted higher levels of boldness, neophilia, and exploration in individuals from urban and semi–natural origin populations compared with their natural-living counterparts. If these predictions are upheld, this would constitute evidence for heritable behavioral divergence in urbanized populations. We also examined if behavioral traits were repeatable throughout development, and compared their consistency among origin population categories (urban, semi-natural, and natural). Repeatability of behavioral traits across time suggests strong, constant selection for a particular behavioral type within an environment (Dingemanse and Réale 2005; Bell 2012) and may give an indication of broad heritability of a trait (Dohm 2002). In contrast, lack of repeatability of behavioral traits may indicate plasticity; which may be beneficial in order to cope with changing, novel environments (Lampe et al. 2014; Griffin et al. 2016).

Methods

Study species

Australian water dragons are a large (maximum snout-vent length: 304 mm; Thompson 1993) agamid lizard. They are relatively long-lived species (28–40 years; Harlow and Harlow 1997; Griffiths 2006) with a generation time of 5 years (Littleford-Colquhoun et al. 2017). They are naturally found in forested areas associated with creeks, rivers, and other freshwater bodies (Cogger 2014); however, they are also common in urban parklands and other green spaces (Littleford-Colquhoun et al. 2017). These lizards are a dietary generalist (Baxter-Gilbert 2014), and are known to exploit anthropogenic food sources in urban areas (Baxter-Gilbert 2018).

Field collection and husbandry

In the spring (October and November) of 2015, we collected gravid female water dragons from 12 sites (four urban, four semi-natural, and four natural) within a 50-km radius within the greater Sydney area in New South Wales, Australia (see Supplementary Materials for exact location details, sample sizes, and SM Fig. 1). Urban sites had a dense local human population and a landscape that was widely human-modified (e.g., concrete, buildings, gardens, roads). Semi-natural sites were protected green spaces (national and regional parkland) that contained waterways adjacent to urban areas, and they had a moderate human presence (park visitors). Natural sites, although not completely free from human disturbance, were generally associated with native bushland, waterways with treed shorelines, and a relatively low human presence.

Upon capture, we transported females to Macquarie University (Sydney, NSW), or if captured at Taronga Zoo (Sydney, NSW) they were held there, and then oviposition was induced (for egg collection details see Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2018). Clutches of eggs were identically incubated throughout development (at a constant temperature of 26.5 °C allowing for an equal sex-ratio; Harlow 2001). Upon emerging, hatchlings were marked using a passive integrated transponder (PIT) tag and randomly allocated to one of 15 common garden enclosures (approximately 6–7 lizards per enclosure; initial experimental group N = 97 but group size varied over time due to seasonal differences in capture success; Table 1). The outdoor enclosures (6.2 m2 plastic tubs lined with sand, and containing tile refuges, hardwood dowel perches, and a small pool) were contained within a predator-exclusion net, which allowed for natural weather and photoperiods common to the Sydney region (see SM Fig. 2). Throughout the experiment, all dragons experienced identical housing, husbandry conditions (fed vitamin-supplemented crickets 3 times weekly), thermal conditions, and water ad libitum.

Table 1 Number of Australian water dragons (Intellagama lesueurii) sampled for each round of behavioral assays split across each type of origin population (natural, semi-natural, and urban)

Behavioral assays

We assayed three behavioral traits (exploration, boldness, and neophilia) five times over the dragons’ first year of life (once every 2 months, excluding the winter brumation period of July and August). Dragon body size (snout-vent length) varied over this time period, averaging 48 mm (± 0.19 standard error (SE), min = 39 mm, max = 53 mm) initially, and growing to an average of 82 mm (± 1.21 SE, min = 64 mm, max = 133 mm) at the end of the year. Behavioral trait assays were conducted indoors over 3 days, and consisted of 1 assay per day. During each of the five rounds of assays, we were not always able to re-capture all lizards, resulting in some variation in sample sizes (Table 1). Our experimental room was not large enough to house all dragons at once, so we conducted assays in four batches (maximum of 32 individuals per batch, two batches per day, and 6 days total). Assays took place in a temperature-controlled room, set to the dragon’s preferred body temperature of 30 °C (Hosking 2010), unless otherwise stated. During assays, dragons were individually housed and behaviors were remotely video recorded using a security camera system (CCTV Security Systems, Melbourne, Victoria). Each behavioral assay (exploration, boldness, neophilia) was scored from the videos by a single researcher to ensure consistency, with the video scorer being blind to the lizard’s origin population (see below for scoring criteria).

Day 1: Explorative behavior

Our measure of explorative behavior quantified the amount of time (s) a dragon spent moving (i.e., exploring) in a novel arena. To assay these behaviors, we introduced dragons into a novel environment, similar to an open-field test (Perals et al. 2017; Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019). The testing arenas were always the same size (rectangular arenas; 690 W × 470 L × 455 H mm) and had two black refuge boxes (120 W × 175 L × 38 H mm) at opposite ends. We varied the substrate between each of the five repeated measures (plain paper, eucalyptus mulch, sugar cane mulch, topsoil, and pine-bark mulch; see SM Fig. 3A) to ensure the arena was novel each time. At the beginning of each trial, we introduced the dragon into the arena within a central, containment refuge. The dragon was allowed to acclimate within this refuge for 5 min, whereupon the refuge was lifted, and the assay began. Each exploration assay ran for 30 min. The dragons then remained in these enclosures for the duration of the assay period (3 days). From video recordings, we scored the time a lizard spent moving within the trial (s). This value was used as our “exploration score”; as the value increases, it reflects more explorative behavior (Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019).

Day 2: Boldness

Our measure of boldness was the amount of time (s) it took a dragon to leave an unfavorable refuge after a simulated predatory attack. We created a thermal difference within the testing arena by lowering the temperature in the experimental room to 22 °C, and positioning a heat lamp directly over one of the refuge boxes, creating a “hot” refuge (Carazo et al. 2014; Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019). We also positioned an ice pack beneath the enclosure, directly under the other refuge, creating a “cold” refuge (Carazo et al. 2014; Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019; see SM Fig. 3B). By doing this, we created a high- and low-quality refuge. At the beginning of each trial, we introduced the dragon into the arena within a central, containment refuge and left it to acclimate for 5 min. We then simulated a predatory attack by removing the containment refuge and “chasing” the dragon with a blue, gloved hand until it entered the “cold” refuge (Riley et al. 2017; Damas-Moreira et al. 2019). We then remotely video recorded the dragon’s behavior for 1 h. We measured a lizard’s boldness as the amount of time (s) it took the dragon to leave the “cold” refuge. This value was our “boldness score”, with lower times indicating higher boldness. If the dragons did not exit the refuge within the duration of the trial, we assigned it a value of 3600 s.

Day 3: Neophilia

Our neophilia assay quantified how close (cm) a dragon would approach a novel object. Within each enclosure, a bullseye (10-, 20-, 30-, and 40-cm diameter rings surrounding a central 5-cm diameter circle) was printed on paper and taped to the base of the arena (prior to all behavioral assays beginning; see SM Fig. 3). The two refuge boxes from the previous assay were removed, and a novel object was placed at the center of the bullseye (see SM Fig. 3C). During the five neophilia assays, each individual saw a different novel object each time. The objects chosen are common refuse items found in urban areas that were similarly sized (between 6 and 8 cm diameter), and was different across each assay period. The novel objects were presented in this order: (1) unused 350-ml paper coffee cups, (2) unused aluminum 160-ml pie tins, (3) empty 600-ml water bottle, (4) unopened bag of 19-g potato chips, and (5) unopened 330-ml soft drink can. Similar to the previous two assays, each dragon was placed within a central containment refuge at the start of an assay, and left for 5 min to acclimate. To begin the assay, the central containment refuge was removed, and individuals were left for 30 min to interact with the novel object.

From the videos of the neophilia assay, we noted the proximity of the individual to the novel object using the rings of the bullseye to indicate distance to the object (e.g., outer-most ring = 20 cm and inner-most ring = 5 cm). Dragons that climbed the novel object were given a score of 0 cm, and individuals beyond the outermost ring were assigned a score of 25 cm. The closest distance a dragon approached the novel object over the 30-min period represented its “neophilia score” and was the value used in our analysis; lower scores indicate a higher level of neophilia.

Statistical analyses

Behavioral traits

Before analysis, we explored our data following the protocol detailed in Zuur et al. (2010). Two of our three behavioral traits, boldness and neophilia, followed a normal distribution and had no outliers. We used a rank transformation to normalize our exploration score (Kar et al. 2016). Before analyses, we also ensured there was no strong collinearity between model predictor variables (i.e., a R2 of greater than 0.70).

We examined differences in dragon behavioral traits using linear mixed effect models (LMM, using the function lmer in the lme4 R package; Bates et al. 2015; R Core Team 2016). We ran separate LMMs for each of the three behavioral traits. The LMMs with exploration and neophilia as the response variable included the fixed effects of dragon age (days since hatching; continuous), origin population type (categorical: natural, semi-natural, or urban), and batch (categorical: 1, 2, 3, or 4). The LMM with boldness as the response variable had the additional continuous fixed factor of time spent scaring the lizard (s). Continuous fixed factors were mean-centered using a z-transformation before analysis, which standardizes the variables and facilitates interpretation of main effects in the presence of interactions (Schielzeth 2010). In all LMMs, we accounted for dependencies within our data from sampling each lizard repeatedly (random intercept and slope for lizard identity across age), sampling individuals from the same clutch (random intercept for lizard clutch), the same captive enclosures (random intercept for tub identity), and the same study population (random intercept for study site). To allow comparisons among all origin site types, we re-leveled the reference for origin population category and re-ran the model (Nakagawa 2004). The assumptions of normality of residuals, for both fixed and random effects, and heterogeneity of variance were verified for all LMMs (Zuur et al. 2009), α was set at 0.05, and the R function confint was used to bootstrap 95% confidence intervals for parameter estimates. We also calculated unconditional means and 95% CIs (corrected for non-independence) for each origin population type using the function Effect in the R package effects (Fox 2003; Fox and Hong 2009). Assessment of unconditional means and the magnitude of their differences (i.e., effect size) can reflect biological significance (Nakagawa and Cuthill 2007; Gerstner et al. 2017).

Consistency in behavior

We examined the consistency of an individual’s behavioral traits to investigate if repeatability was affected by origin population type. To accomplish this, we first subset the data by origin population category, resulting in three separate datasets. For urban-origin dragons, we had 126 observations from 28 individuals across 17 clutches, 14 enclosures, and 4 populations. For semi-natural-origin dragons, we had 225 observations of exploration, and 228 observations of boldness and neophilia from 52 individuals across 32 clutches, 15 enclosures, and 4 populations. For natural-origin dragons, we had 82 observations of exploration, and 83 of boldness and neophilia from 23 individuals across 14 clutches, 13 enclosures, and 4 populations.

We calculated adjusted repeatability (Radj|age; Biro and Stamps 2015) for each origin population type while controlling for the same covariates that were within their respective LMMs (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2010; Biro and Stamps 2015). We calculated 95% confidence intervals by bootstrapping the data 1000 times with the boot function from the R package boot (Davison and Hinkley 1997; Canty and Ripley 2017). Radj|age was considered significantly more than what would occur by chance alone if the 95% confidence intervals did not overlap 0. We compared Radj|age between treatments by visually examining overlap between 95% CIs and the Radj|age value for natural, semi-natural, and urban sites. Being conservative, we considered a difference as significant if the 95% CIs did not overlap. Theoretically, Radj|age ranges between 0 (individuals never expressing the same trait value over repeated measures) and 1 (individuals always expressing the same trait value over repeated measures; Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2010), although the average repeatability observed in the field of animal behavior is 0.37 (Bell et al. 2009).

Results

Behavioral traits

Exploration did not significantly differ among origin population category (Table 2; comparison between semi-natural and urban: β = 0.118, 95% CI = − 0.612, 0.427, t = − 0.444, P = 0.657), and neither did neophilia (Table 2; comparison between semi-natural and urban: β = − 0.628, 95% CI = − 3.173, 2.130, t = − 0.445, P = 0.656). Dragons that originated from semi-natural sites were significantly bolder than natural sites (Table 2). Boldness did not significantly differ between urban and natural sites (Table 2), nor semi-natural and urban sites (β = 10.810, 95% CI = − 408.829, 448.212, t = 0.048, P = 0.962). However, both the semi-natural and urban dragons exited the hide (the metric for boldness) about 8.3 min (492 s and 502 s, respectively) sooner than individuals in the natural-origin population category (Table 2; Fig. 1).

Table 2 Outcomes of linear mixed effect models testing if behavioral traits (exploration, boldness, and neophilia) were affected by dragon origin site type (natural: NT, semi-natural: SN, and urban: UB). If fixed factors were not included in the analysis, this is represented with three hyphens (- - -). If an effect was significant (at an ∞ of 0.05), it is bolded. Other abbreviations found below are as follows: Nobs means number of observations, Nind reflects the number of individuals, Nmom reflects the number of clutches, Ntub is the number of housing enclosures the individuals came from, and Nsite is the number of specific origin populations we sampled
Fig. 1
figure 1

Australian water dragons from semi-natural sites were significantly bolder than those from natural sites (b). Here we present unconditional means and 95% CIs of each behavioral trait, a exploration, b boldness, and c neophilia, for each origin population category (black, gray, and white points represent natural (NAT), semi-natural (SEMI-NAT), and urban (URB) populations, respectively) of Australian water dragons (Intellagama lesueurii)

Consistency in behavior

Explorative behavior of dragons was not significantly repeatable in any origin population (urban: Radj|age = 0.44, 95% CI = 0, 0.68; semi-natural: Radj|age = 0.14, 95% CI = 0, 0.40; natural: Radj|age = 0.02, 95% CI = 0, 0.45). Similarly, neophilia of dragons was not significantly repeatable in any origin population (urban: Radj|age = 0.05, 95% CI = 0, 0.34; semi-natural: Radj|age = 0.06, 95% CI = 0, 0.30; natural: Radj|age = 0, 95% CI = 0, 0.45).

Dragons originating from urban populations had moderate repeatability in boldness (Radj|age = 0.32, 95% CI = 0.02, 0.63), while boldness was not significantly repeatable in dragons from semi-natural (Radj|age = 0.18, 95% CI = 0, 0.41) and natural (Radj|age = 0.25, 95% CI = 0, 0.61) populations. Repeatability was not different among origin population types in any of the behavioral traits measured.

Discussion

We found that water dragons from semi-natural populations were significantly bolder than those from natural-origin populations. Also, a difference between urban and natural-origin populations, although non-significant, trended in the same direction. We expected lizards to be bolder in relation to the extent of urbanization experienced by their origin population (i.e., urban dragons to be most bold, semi-natural dragons to be moderately bold, natural dragons to be least bold). Interestingly, the parameter estimates reflect this logic. The difference in boldness between urban and natural populations (parameter estimates and effect sizes) was actually slightly greater (by 10 s) than the difference in boldness between semi-natural and natural environments (Table 2; Fig. 1). The lack of significance is likely a consequence of greater variance in boldness among individuals within the urban environment. We suggest that the difference in boldness between dragons from urban and natural-origin population categories is likely still biologically relevant, because both the semi-natural and urban dragons exited the hide at approximately the same time, which was substantially earlier than individuals in the natural-origin population type.

Our findings support idea that wildlife persisting in, or colonizing, urban environments have a tendency to be bolder (reviewed in Lowry et al. 2013; Miranda et al. 2013; Sol et al. 2013) and suggests increased boldness in our urbanized water dragons is innate, either as a heritable trait or because of maternal effects. These findings further align with recent studies that demonstrate urbanization may be selecting for heritable behavioral traits (e.g., increased aggression and boldness, Evans et al. 2010; Müller et al. 2013; Holtmann et al. 2017; Sprau and Dingemanse 2017). If heritable, then increased boldness in urban areas may promote fitness, through facilitating increased foraging and mating opportunities. This fitness benefit may drive selection for enhanced boldness within novel environments (Dingemanse and Réale 2005; Réale et al. 2007). The design of our common garden experiment was able to remove a host of potential confounding factors, such as nest environment, prior experience, or habituation (Evans et al. 2010; Lampe et al. 2014; Vincze et al. 2016; Siviter et al. 2017), which does support our assentation that increased boldness is an urban-derived heritable trait. Yet, we cannot rule out the possibility of site-specific maternal effects. We recommend future research into the behavioral traits of urban and natural populations take into account any differences in the allocation of nutrients and maternal hormones into developing eggs, as this may alter hatchling behavioral traits (Groothuis et al. 2005; Räsänen and Kruuk 2007; Bertin et al. 2009). Maternal effects on behavioral traits may also include differences in maternal basking opportunities between populations, as seen in other Australian agamids (Amphibolurus muricatus; Schwanz 2016).

With respect to neophilia and exploration, we did not find differences in among origin population categories. These behavioral traits may not be strongly favored for within Sydney urban environments. Alternatively, there may have been a flaw in the trial design (e.g., testing arena size or means of measuring) or there may be ontogenetic changes in the timing of expression of these behaviors (e.g., perhaps dragons do not express variation in neophilia or exploration behavior until they are older). Urban, wild-caught brown anoles (Anolis sagrei) are bolder, less aggressive, and more explorative compared with natural-origin populations (Lapiedra et al. 2017); however, in this study, they could not rule out the effects of rearing environment and prior experience on the behaviors they were observing. Expression of behavioral traits like exploration and neophilia may be highly plastic in urban habitats (Bókony et al. 2012; Sol et al. 2013). For example, there is a positive correlation between boldness and aggression in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia), but this relationship breaks down in urban areas (Scales et al. 2011), which may result from individuals modulating their behavior based on specific costs and benefits associated with differing habitats (Sol et al. 2014). Overall, more research is necessary to understand the selective forces that are shaping the behavior of urban wildlife (Lowry et al. 2013) and what determines the roles that both plastic and fixed behavioral traits play for species persisting in urban landscapes.

The only behavioral trait we observed to be consistent was boldness in individuals from urban environments which was significantly, but moderately, repeatable. Repeatability, or consistency, in behavior across time generally reflects selection for the expression of that trait within that environment (Dingemanse and Réale 2005). For this reason, the repeatable boldness in urban populations supports our assertion that urban selection favors consistently bolder individuals. For all other behavioral trait and environment type combinations, dragons exhibited low within-individual repeatability of behavioral traits, which suggests that plasticity in their expression may be favorable. However, an alternative hypothesis for the general lack of repeatability in behavior is that the behavior of juveniles may simply be more plastic (Favati et al. 2016; Riley et al. 2017), with more fixed behaviors traits forming as they mature. This would also suggest that rearing environment and prior experience may inform the development of behavior in these dragons. Formation of consistent behavior, related to urban-derived behavioral syndromes, has been documented in adult brown anoles (Lapiedra et al. 2017), and we suggest that further research on adult dragon behavioral expression across urban populations is required to determine if they would yield comparable results. Potentially, practical issues with study design may be another explanation for why repeatability of dragon behavioral traits was not found across habitats and environments. For example, we may have not selected an ecologically relevant time frame for the quantification of trait consistency (Dohm 2002). Overall, further investigation into the differences in behavior and the expression of behavioral traits in anthropogenic landscapes will shed light into how selective forces act on individuals during urban evolution.

In summary, urban landscapes are both expanding globally and a major contributor to biodiversity loss (McKinney 2002; Seto et al. 2012). In light of the novel landscapes humanity has created (Ellis and Ramankutty 2008), it is imperative we understand the role of urban evolution in allowing wildlife to adapt to an increasingly urban world (Dingemanse and Réale 2005; Lowry et al. 2013; Holtmann et al. 2017; Johnson and Munshi-South 2017). Furthermore, we need to understand how these naturally evolving divergent behavioral traits can be applied to conservation actions and wildlife management to enhance our ability to protect species that are less capable at persisting in urban areas, which is currently a major threat to wildlife worldwide (Greggor et al. 2016). Our study provides experimental evidence of an innate, urban-derived divergent behavioral trait (boldness) in a vertebrate, removed from the confounding effects of developmental environment and prior experience, and advances our understanding of both urban evolution (Johnson and Munshi-South 2017) and the role of behavior in evolution, particularly in novel environments.