Introduction

Group-living is widespread in animals. It may enhance foraging success (Beauchamp 1998), safety from predators (Foster and Treherne 1981), thermoregulation (Gilbert et al. 2006), energy economy (Herskin and Steffensen 1998), mating opportunities (Wagner 1992) and offspring care (Koenig and Dickinson 2016). In turn, gregarious animals may suffer from increased competition for resources (food: Janson 1988; mates: Wedell et al. 2002), disease transmission (Côté and Poulinb 1994) and the risk of infanticide (Crockett and Janson 2000). To optimize the benefits of group living, individuals can engage in repeated beneficial interactions with a subset of group members (Melis et al. 2006a; Schino and Aureli 2016). Such “social bonds” between individuals may reflect an adaptive strategy increasing the benefits of group living while limiting its costs (Silk et al. 2010).

Social bonds have been referred to as close associations, long-term alliances, affiliative relationships or friendships (see Silk 2002 for review). They can be defined by the quality and patterning of interactions between group members (Hinde 1976), reflecting dyads that specifically engage with each other in a socio-positive way (Massen et al. 2010), and they may be characterized by strong, supportive, equitable and enduring relationships between two individuals (Silk et al. 2010). Social bonds encompass a variety of relationships that are not limited to kin (e.g. parent-offspring) or mates (pair bonds). Measurements of bond strength commonly take into account the content, quality, frequency and patterning of interactions, with allogrooming/allopreening and proximity reflecting the most common indices (Silk 2002). There may be short-term and long-term fitness benefits for involved partners; potential short-term benefits include positive outcomes of cooperation (Melis et al. 2006b), reduced harassment (Cameron et al. 2009) and relaxation (Crockford et al. 2013), whereas potential long-term benefits encompass enhanced birth rates (Cameron et al. 2009), increased offspring and adult survival (Silk et al. 2009, 2010) and reduced levels of infanticide (Weingrill 2000).

Social bonds may be expected to emerge predominantly under a combination of several parameters. These include certain sizes and structures of groups, frequent interactions among group members, the capacity of individual recognition and long-term memory and a potential of receiving marked benefits from bonds. The latter may accrue, for instance, from the reciprocal exchange of services, which may include costly help. In addition, kin discrimination and the existence of a specific rank order have been proposed to be important (Seyfarth and Cheney 2012). Indeed, in some species, the strongest bonds take place between kin (for example in elephants; Archie et al. 2006). Furthermore, bonds with high-ranking individuals might be more valuable for a subject than bonds with low-ranking social partners, as high-ranking individuals might variously support and benefit their bonded partner, which might favour bond formation and persistence (see Schino 2001 for a meta-analysis on primates).

Most empirical evidence for close bonds thus far comes from primates (Seyfarth and Cheney 2012), although there is a growing body of literature on other animals (for example in cows (Val-Laillet et al. 2009), dolphins (Lusseau et al. 2003), elephants (Archie et al. 2001), hyenas (Holekamp et al. 1997), horses (Cameron et al. 2009), ravens (Braun and Bugnyar 2012) and voles (Beery et al. 2009)). However, in order to understand patterns and functions underlying social bonds, we need information from a greater variety of animals.

Here, we used undomesticated wild-type Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) as model organism. Rats live in burrow systems (Telle 1966) and are highly social, which is reflected by the fact that isolated rats show depression-like behaviours (Hurst et al. 1997, 1998). Under natural conditions, rats live in complex colonies containing up to 200 individuals of different ages, sex, rank and relatedness (Davis 1953). Rats individually recognize each other (Gheusi et al. 1997) and form hierarchies among group members (Blanchard et al. 1984). They cooperate with each other, for instance, by food sharing, allogrooming and huddling (Barnett 1963; Krafft et al. 1994), they show mutual coordination (Schuster and Perelberg 2004; Tan and Hackenberg 2016) and they reciprocate food donations and allogrooming with partners (Rutte and Taborsky 2008; Schweinfurth et al. 2017). Reciprocal interactions have been shown to have long-term benefits: rats showing a balanced amount of initiated and received affiliative behaviours live longer and suffer fewer mammary tumours compared to those that show an unequal score (Yee et al. 2008). Thus, rats are apparently good candidates to search for the existence of long-lasting social bonds. Thus far, it remains unclear whether reciprocal exchanges are predominantly shown between closely bonded partners, or whether individuals immediately return received favours without establishing social bonds. We therefore tested whether rats have preferred interaction partners and whether such potential association patterns within a group are stable over time and different contexts. Rats were observed in their groups in two different spatial settings, and proximity and affiliative behaviours were recorded. Additionally, the hierarchy within each group was assessed to test whether preferential associations are explained by the subjects’ rank. To test for temporal consistency of potential social bonds, we repeated the procedure after an interval of 2 days. If rats show social bonds, we predicted to find preferences to stay close to a specific group member with which they would show most affiliative behaviours. Further, the interaction patterns within each group should be stable over different contexts and time points. Finally, we predicted bonds to be dependent on hierarchy, so that especially high-ranking individuals would be favoured bonding partners.

Methods

Experimental subjects and housing conditions

We used outbred adult female wild-type Norway rats (source: Animal Physiology Department, University of Groningen, the Netherlands) weighing on average 300 g, at an age of 1.5 years. Directly after weaning (5 weeks after birth), rats were assigned to their housing group and remained there for their lifespan. The rats were individually marked with stripes on the tail using a black permanent marker (edding3000). The ambient temperature was 20 °C ± 1 °C, with a relative humidity of 50–60% and a 12:12 h light/dark cycle, with lights on at 20:00 h after 30 min of dawn and with a respective dusk period in the morning. As rats are primarily nocturnal (Norton et al. 1975) and lack red light receptors (Yokoyama and Radlwimmer 1998), all experiments were conducted under red light conditions during the dark phase of the cycle. Rats had been accustomed to all situations (hierarchy assessment and both experimental contexts) prior to the experiment.

Experimental setup

We tested for the importance of a hierarchy structure for social bonds using 21 long-term groups of 5 female rats each, resulting in 210 dyads. To clarify whether the rats form social bonds, we performed three behavioural tests. First, we assessed the rank order within each group. Thereafter, rats were observed with their group members in their familiar environment (context 1: “home cage”). To test for the stability of interaction patterns across different contexts, they were then observed in a large room (context 2: “open arena”), where they could more easily avoid non-preferred group members, or cluster with their potentially bonded partners. To test whether interaction patterns are stable over time, we repeated the entire procedure (t 1: hierarchy → home cage → open arena) after 2 days (time point: t 2). We chose a time interval of 2 days because female rats may change hierarchy structures (Blanchard et al. 1984; Fang and Clemens 1999) according to their 4 days oestrus cycle (Hardy 1972). The experimental timeline is depicted in Fig. 1, and the behavioural tests are described in more detail below. It was not possible to record data blind because our study involved focal observations.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental setup. First, the hierarchical structure of each housing group (n = 21) was assessed by a food monopolization test. The individual monopolizing the food item (here a piece of banana) was assumed to be dominant over the partner. Thereafter, rats were videotaped for 1 h in their home cage. Here, we recorded which individual interacted with whom and which behaviours were exchanged. Finally, rats were released in an open arena with five huts as shelters for hiding (indicated by dashed rectangles). Here, the experimenter directly observed interactions between rats of the same housing group. After 48 h, the whole procedure was repeated. All possible dyads of 21 housing groups involving five individuals each were tested

Hierarchy assessment

To determine the hierarchical structure within the 21 housing groups at time points t 1 and t 2, we conducted a food allocation test with all rats of each group (modified after Ziporyn and McClintock 1991). This is a well-established test to determine the hierarchy structure of female rats, which may be non-linear. For this, we placed a high value food item (piece of banana; cf. Dolivo and Taborsky 2015a) covered with a perforated plastic cup in the middle of an experimental arena (80 × 40 × 40 cm, made of glass). Two out of five rats of each group were then placed in the arena. After both rats had explored the plastic cup, we removed the rats and uncovered the food item. Directly after uncovering the food item, we again placed the two cage mates into the arena at its opposite ends, both with the same distance to the available food item. The winner was defined by gaining and monopolizing the food item. In cases of uncertainty, the test was repeated. Each rat was tested against all four littermates and the order was fully randomized beforehand using the Excel function (RANDBETWEEN). Rats had been food deprived for at most 10 h prior to testing during the inactive phase of the rats’ daily cycle (whereas water access was provided ad libitum) in order to increase the motivation level, so that both partners were motivated to gain the food reward.

Measuring social interaction patterns in context 1: home cage

The home cage of each group was carried to a different room, and all hiding possibilities (except hay) were removed. A camera (Sony-handycam, HDR-CX550) with night vision was installed above the cage, recording the rats for 70 min. The rats were left alone during this time to exclude any external disturbance. After 10 min of habituation, we started the data collection for 1 h from the video record. We conducted scan samples of all behaviours every 2 min and noted down the nearest neighbour and all affiliative behaviours between group members, such as sitting-in-contact, allogrooming and huddling (Table 1).

Table 1 Ethogram of behavioural data for interaction scores

Measuring social interaction patterns in context 2: open arena

All five rats of a group were released together in a big arena (4 × 4 m for the first batch of 9 housing groups, 2.5 × 2 m for the second batch of 12 housing groups) containing five shelters spread over the floor. The experimenter was sitting on an elevated tower to exclude any interactions with and disturbance of the rats. As in the home cage context, we conducted the same scan sampling of proximity and affiliative behaviours.

Data analysis

Based on proximity and affiliative behaviour data (Table 1), we calculated interaction scores (after; Silk et al. 2006) for every dyad (n = 210), with i = donor, j = receiver and i (total) = every given behaviour of i to group members

$$ {\mathrm{Interaction}\ \mathrm{score}}_{\mathrm{ij}}=\left(\frac{{\mathrm{Proximity}}_{i j}}{{\mathrm{Proximity}}_{i\left(\mathrm{total}\right)}}\times 100\right)+\left(\frac{{\mathrm{Affiliative}\ \mathrm{behaviours}}_{i j}}{{\mathrm{Affiliative}\ \mathrm{behaviours}}_{i\left(\mathrm{total}\right)}}\times 100\right) $$

We divided dyad values by the overall interactions of the active individual to correct for overall activity. This led to four interaction scores for each rat while being in the home cage and in the open arena at time points t 1 and t 2.

All analyses were done using R (version 3.1.0.). As data on dyadic interactions cannot be independent from each other, all analyses corrected for this effect and are based on dyads as the statistical unit.

To investigate whether the hierarchy changes over time, we conducted a mixed effects null model with binomial data, using the command “glmer” with the package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015). As the response variable, we included consistency of food monopolization (the same rat of a dyad monopolized the food item at t 1 and t 2). To control for multiple testing of individuals, we included “dyad” and the identity of both rats of each dyad, resulting in three random effects.

We predicted that if rats have “closely bonded partners”, not all rats would similarly interact with each other, resulting in a skewed distribution of social interactions. Such skewed (e.g. j-shaped; Silk et al. 2010) distributions indicate that most group members interact at low levels and only a small number interact at high levels, which would point toward closely bonded partners. We therefore calculated the skewness of the data and tested whether the skewness is different from zero, which is expected under a normal distribution, using a one-sample t test. This procedure is described in detail in Crawley (2007). For calculating this general pattern of social interactions, we used the interaction scores summed up for both contexts and both time points for all dyads, leading to a maximum value of 800.

To analyse the association patterns for consistency over time and context, we organized the data for each housing group in matrices for contexts and time points. The matrices were asymmetric as most of the behaviours were unidirectional, such as allogrooming and nearest neighbour data (Table 1). This resulted in four asymmetrical matrices per group (for an example, see supplementary material Table S1). We used these asymmetrical matrices to correlate behavioural interactions between contexts and time points using Mantel Z tests, which controls for dependency within and between rows and columns of the same individual, following Hemelrijk (1990). Additionally, we analysed the influence of hierarchy on association patterns using the same method. We are not aware of an R-package to conduct this test and therefore wrote an own script based on Hemelrijk (1990) (see supplementary material for the R script). For the Mantel Z tests, 10,000 permutations were conducted, using the R-package permute (Simpson et al. 2016). The tests revealed one p value for each housing group and each comparison. Afterwards, we conducted the Fisher’s combined probability test \( \left({X}_{2 k}^2\sim -2\sum_{i=1}^k \ln \right({p}_i \))) to check for overall significance (Sokal and Rohlf 1995).

Results

Consistency of rank order

Rats that monopolized a food item at time point t 1 were more likely to do so also at time point t 2 within the same dyad, indicating a stable hierarchy over time (GLMM β = 0.682 ± 0.16, p < 0.0001).

Social bonds

The null expectation, if rats do not have preferred interaction partners within their group, was that roughly 25% of all interactions should have occurred with each of the four cohabitants in the same cage. In accordance with this prediction of a random, i.e. unbiased interaction pattern, the mean interaction score was 197.28, which corresponds to 24.7% of the possible maximum value (800; Fig. 2). This lack of a significant skew in the distribution of interaction partners (one-sample t test df = 210, p = 0.50) suggests that bonds between specific group members were missing.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of social interaction frequencies within groups of five rats. Summing up social interaction scores of each rat from two social contexts at two points in time generated 420 data points resulting from 210 dyads (passive and active partners as some behaviours are unidirectional such as allogrooming). A value of 800 on the abscissa would indicate that two rats had exclusively interacted with each other, while a value of 0 would indicate no interactions at all. The mean value of social interactions between two randomly drawn partners of a group was 197.28, with a maximum of 421. Hence, the interaction frequencies between group members were not significantly skewed, indicating that all group members interacted at similar levels with each other

Social relations were neither stable over contexts (Fisher’s combined probability tests: home cage vs. open arena tests at t 1 n = 21, p = 0.65; home cage vs. open arena tests at t 2 n = 21, p = 0.99) nor over time (Fisher’s combined probability test: home cage t 1 vs. t 2 n = 21, p = 0.99; open arena t 1 vs. t 2 n = 21, p = 0.97; Fig. 3). Rank order had no influence on social relations at any of the four situations (Fisher’s combined probability test: home cage [t 1] n = 21, p = 0.96; home cage [t 2] n = 21, p = 0.86; open arena [t 1] n = 21, p = 0.72; open arena [t 2] n = 21, p = 0.98; see also Fig. S1 of the supplementary material).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Consistency of social relations over time and in different contexts. p values obtained from Mantel Z tests for each comparison, with the respective α levels indicated by dotted lines. Matrices of each housing group (n = 21) were compared within two contexts (home cage and open arena) over time to test for temporal consistency (upper graphs). Furthermore, matrices were compared over the two different contexts at time points t 1 and t 2 to test for consistency over different contexts (lower graphs)

Discussion

Contrary to predictions derived from the hypothesis that female Norway rats exhibit social bonds among specific group members, test subjects did not interact more often with certain individuals of their group. In addition, interaction patterns between dyads were neither stable over time nor over different contexts, and they were independent of the hierarchical structure that itself remained stable over time. Therefore, our data do not indicate that female rats form social bonds.

In rats, both sexes form dominance hierarchies. However, in contrast to male Norway rats that form stable hierarchies mainly based on body weight, females form less stable and not strictly linear hierarchies strongly influenced by their oestrus cycle (Adams and Boice 1983; Ziporyn and McClintock 1991). Our results show that food monopolization among female rats living in long-term stable social groups was consistent over time. Despite this stability of rank order, we did not find effects of rank on individual relationships between our test subjects.

Given that rats are highly social animals living in large groups and showing considerable levels of cooperation, it may at first glance seem surprising that there is no evidence for the existence of social bonds among individual group members. Under natural conditions, however, rats might form small subgroups within colonies which could pose an alternative to close dyadic bonds. Information about natural colonies of Norway rats is scarce, and there is mixed evidence for the existence of subgroups in large rat colonies. Barnett (1963, pp. 83–100) described family groups to exist only until pups are weaned, but Calhoun (1962, pp. 172–176) observed the occurrence of flexible subgroups in rat colonies, with individuals sometimes belonging to several such groups. If substructures exist within large colonies, rats might have close relationships to all members of their subgroup and perhaps behave more cooperatively towards in-group members than to individuals not belonging to the same subgroup, instead of forming dyadic bonds. The group size in our study was small and might have resembled the size of subgroups within larger rat colonies, which can contain up to 200 individuals (Telle 1966). This might explain why our test subjects did not discriminate among the members of their group.

Our study does not suggest that the seemingly altruistic cooperation exhibited by Norway rats (e.g. Rutte and Taborsky 2008; Schneeberger et al. 2012; Dolivo and Taborsky 2015a, b; Wood et al. 2016) is governed by close social bonds. This is in line with previous evidence showing that reciprocal cooperation among rats may simply reflect their discrimination based on preceding interactions, which causes them to preferably cooperate with individuals that recently have been helpful (cf. Dolivo et al. 2016). This may generate fitness benefits: rats have been shown to survive longer, for instance, when reciprocating affiliative behaviours (Yee et al. 2008).

A lack of stable relationships has also been described in other social animals. Pigs, for instance, have preferred association partners over short time periods, but these relationships break down over a longer time frame (Durrell et al. 2004). Female house mice have preferred partners to communally nurse with (Weidt et al. 2008), but still most of their interactions can be explained by stochastic interactions (Perony et al. 2010). In addition, many group-living lemur species develop weak or no social bonds (Kappeler 1997). In primates, the existence of close and consistent social bonds in general has been challenged (Henzi et al. 2009; but see Silk et al. 2010): rather than establishing long-term relationships, individuals might also react on each other’s immediate behaviour (Barrett and Henzi 2001). Such short-term contingencies may engender fitness benefits in complex societies without relying on the formation of long-term social bonds, because reciprocally cooperating social partners can be subject to correlated payoffs, which reflect immediate contingencies (Taborsky et al. 2016). Especially in societies characterized by short life-spans and generation times and by the formation of flexible fission-fusion groups, short-term contingencies might have more positive fitness effects than long-term social bonds.

We conclude that, although rats show sophisticated cooperative behaviours and contrary to the hypothesis that this may reflect the existence of stable social bonds, evidence for such relationships is lacking. This is in line with previous work suggesting that rats base their decision to cooperate with a social partner on recent encounters rather than on cumulative social experiences. It is an interesting challenge for future studies to clarify which ecological or social circumstances favour either the establishment of social bonds or the more immediate responses to the behaviour of group members.