Introduction

Social structure of wildlife populations is typically derived from observational studies on direct social interactions [e.g., affiliative interactions in primates, Griffin and Nunn 2011; MacIntosh et al. 2012, group association in dolphins, Lusseau et al. 2006 and ungulates, Cross et al. 2004; Vander Wal et al. 2012, food sharing in vampire bats, Carter and Wilkinson 2013]. In relatively solitary species, individuals spend a considerable amount of time alone and have minimal direct interactions with conspecifics except during mating and occasional aggressive encounters (Scott and Carrington 2011). Examples of such species include raccoons, red foxes, orangutans, and some species of bees, wasps, and bats. For these wildlife populations, social interactions may be limited to certain areas within their habitat, such as refuges (e.g., roost, den, burrow, nest) or watering holes that provide increased opportunities of direct contact between individuals. Monitoring these resources can therefore help establish relevant social patterns among individuals.

In addition to establishing social structure, refuges provide shelter, protection from predators, and serve as sites for nesting and mating. Refuge use patterns of individuals are therefore central to survival, mating, and foraging success and can serve as efficient indicators of population disturbances. Unlike traditional population dynamics indicators such as mortality and birth rate, refuge use behavior can respond instantaneously to sub-optimal conditions (Morris et al. 2009; Berger-Tal et al. 2011). Altered patterns of refuge use may thus indicate a disturbance or change in population fitness and provide an early warning to conservation biologists. Changes in habitat or refuge use have indeed been linked to the presence of natural population stressors such as increased predation (van Gils et al. 2009), drought (Kerr and Bull 2006; Gough et al. 2012), and disease transmission risk (Behringer and Butler 2010), as well as anthopogenic population stressors of translocation (Jachowski et al. 2012) and urbanization (Moule et al. 2015).

While the importance of refuge use in social interactions, survival and mating success, as well as indicators of environmental and anthropogenic stressors has been long appreciated, biologists are only beginning to understand individual level heterogeneity in refuge use and its population-level consequences in relatively solitary species (Fortuna et al. 2009; Leu et al. 2010; Godfrey 2013). The general absence of studies quantifying pairwise interactions due to preferences in refuge-use implies a lack of knowledge of the baseline social organization that could be used to evaluate changes in robustness or health of these wildlife populations. To overcome these shortcomings, we explore a modeling framework that combines network theory with statistical models to infer the presence of and mechanisms behind the social organization in the desert tortoise, Gopherus agassizii, formed by their refuge-use preferences. The desert tortoise is a long-lived, terrestrial species that occurs throughout the Mojave Desert north and west of the Colorado River. Individuals of this species use subterranean burrows as an essential adaptation to obtain protection from temperature extremes and predators. Because tortoises spend a majority of their time either in or near burrows, most of their social interactions are associated with burrows (Bulova 1994).

Social behavior in desert tortoises is not well understood, though evidence suggests the presence of dominance hierarchies (Niblick et al. 1994; Bulova 1997) which may influence social structure and burrow choice in desert tortoises. In addition to social hierarchies, previous research suggests factors such as sex (Harless et al. 2009), age (Wilson et al. 1999), season (Bulova 1994), and environmental conditions (Duda et al. 1999; Franks et al. 2011) may influence burrow use in desert tortoises. If conspecific cues and environmental factors exhibit strong influence on burrow use, population stressors impacting these characteristics could alter typical burrow behavior. The two major population threats that have been identified in desert tortoise populations include upper respiratory tract disease (URTD) caused by Mycoplasma agassizii and Mycoplasma testudineum (Brown et al. 1994; Sandmeier et al. 2009; Jacobson et al. 2014), and extreme environmental conditions, particularly drought (Longshore et al. 2003; Lovich et al. 2014). In addition to these threats, the primary management strategy in desert tortoises is to translocate animals out of areas affected by anthropogenic disturbances (Department of the Interior 2011). Translocation in other reptilian species, however, has had limited success due to high rates of mortality (Dodd and Seigel 1991; Germano and Bishop 2009) and may also act as a population stressor. In desert tortoises, all three population stressors have been linked to differences in individual behavior (Duda et al. 1999; Nussear et al. 2012; McGuire et al. 2014). Although previous studies provide insights toward potential factors that may affect burrow use, we lack a mechanistic understanding behind the role of these factors in driving heterogeneity in burrow use patterns in desert tortoises. A large impact of population stressors on refuge use can affect mating and foraging opportunities of desert tortoises and also reduce their likelihood of survival.

In this study, we combine data-sets from nine study sites in the desert tortoise habitat, spanning more than 15 years to derive burrow use patterns of individuals in these populations. We first construct bipartite networks to infer their social associations due to asynchronous use of burrows. We then use generalized linear mixed models to explain the mechanisms behind heterogeneity in burrow use behavior of individuals and the effect of population stressors. As the desert tortoise is a long lived species, evaluating the impact of population stressors on burrow use patterns provides an efficient alternative to using traditional demographic metrics (such as mortality). We also investigate the use of burrows through a bipartite network model to identify why certain burrows are more popular than others in desert tortoise habitat. Overall, our analysis of refuge-based associations provide further insights into the structure and dynamics of social organization in a species traditionally considered as solitary and provides mechanisms behind individual variation within these associations.

Methods

Dataset

We combined datasets from nine study sites monitored from 1996 to 2014 across desert tortoise habitat in the Mojave desert of California, Nevada, and Utah (Fig. 1). Each site was monitored over multiple years, but not all sites were monitored in each year of the 15-year span. At each site, individuals were monitored at least weekly during their active season and at least monthly during the winter months using radio telemetry. The total number of animals sampled and average number of observations per tortoise at each site are included in Supplementary Table S1. All tortoises were individually tagged, and during each tortoise encounter, data were collected to record the individual identifier, date of observation, GPS location, micro-habitat currently used by the animal (e.g., vegetation, pallet, or a burrow), any visible signs of injury or upper respiratory tract disease. As the dataset involved monitoring the tagged individuals, it was not possible to record data blind. The unique identification (id) of burrows was recorded when a tortoise was located in a burrow. New burrow ids were assigned when an individual was encountered at a previously unmarked burrow.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Critical habitat range of the desert tortoise within the Mojave desert, USA as determined by the US Fish and Wildlife Services in 2010 (http://www.fws.gov/). Critical habitat is defined as those geographical areas that contain physical or biological features essential to the conservation and management of the species (Department of the Interior 1973). Points represent centroids of survey sites where tortoises were monitored using radio-telemetry. Point size is proportional to the number of animals monitored at the site. Site abbreviations: BSV Bird Spring Valley, CS Coyote Springs, FI Fort Irwin, HW Halfway, LM Lake Meade, MC McCullough Pass, PV Piute Valley, SG St. George, SL Stateline Pass

Network analysis

We constructed bipartite networks of asynchronous burrow use in desert tortoises for active (March–October) and inactive season (November–February) of each year at five sites (CS, HW, MC, PV, SL) where no translocations were carried out. An example of a burrow use bipartite network is shown in Fig. 2. The network consisted of burrow and tortoise nodes and undirected edges. An edge connecting a tortoise node to a burrow node indicated burrow use by the individual (Fig. 2). To reduce bias due to uneven sampling, we did not assign edge weights to the bipartite networks. Connections in a bipartite network represent interaction between the two different node types, and thus edges connecting two tortoise nodes or two burrows nodes were not permitted. Tortoise nodal degree in the bipartite network, therefore denotes the number of unique burrows used by the individual and burrow nodal degree is the number of unique individuals visiting the burrow. Networks were generated using the Networkx package in Python (Hagberg et al. 2008).

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Bipartite network of burrow use patterns at MC site during the year 2012. Node type indicated by color (Blue = adult males and red = adult females). Node positions are fixed using Yifan Hu’s multilevel layout in Gephi (Bastian et al. 2009). In this paper, we quantify burrow switching and burrow popularity as the degree of tortoise nodes and burrow nodes, respectively, in the bipartite network. For example, burrow switching of the female tortoise X is five and burrow popularity of burrow Y is one. b Single-mode projection of the bipartite network into tortoise social network

We further examined the social structure of desert tortoises by converting the bipartite network into a single-mode projection of tortoise nodes (Tortoise social network, Fig. 2). For these tortoise social networks, we calculated network density, degree centralization, modularity, clustering coefficient, and homophily of individuals by degree and sex/age class. Network density is calculated as the ratio of observed edges to the total possible edges in a network (Scott and Carrington 2011). Degree centralization measures the variation in node degree across the network, such that high values indicate a higher heterogeneity in node degree and that a small proportion of nodes have a higher degree than the rest (Scott and Carrington 2011). Modularity measures the strength of the division of nodes into subgroups (Girvan and Newman 2002) and clustering coefficient measures the tendency of neighbors of a node to be connected (Bansal et al. 2009). The values of modularity and clustering coefficient can range from 0 to 1, and larger values indicate stronger modularity or clustering coefficient. We generated 1000 random network counterparts to each empirical network using double-edge swap operation in NetworkX (Hagberg et al. 2008) to determine if the observed network metrics were significantly different from random expectation. The generated random networks had the same degree sequence as empirical networks, but were random with respect to other network properties.

We next examined the spatial dependence of asynchronous burrow associations by using the coordinates of burrows visited by tortoises to calculate the centroid location of each tortoise during a particular season of a year. Distances between each tortoise pair (i, j) were then calculated as \(d_{ij} = d_{ji} = \sqrt {(x_{i} - x_{j})^{2} + (y_{i} - y_{j})^{2}}\) where (x, y) is the coordinate of the tortoise centroid location. Pearson correlation coefficient was used to calculate the correlation between observed edges in social network and geographical distances between the tortoises. We compared the observed correlation to a null distribution of correlation values generated by randomly permuting spatial location of burrows 10,000 times and recalculating correlation between social associations and distance matrix for each permutation. Correlations were calculated using the MantelTest package in Python (Carr 2015).

Regression analysis

We used generalized linear mixed regression models with Poisson distribution and log link function to assess burrow use patterns. To capture seasonal variation in burrow use, we aggregated the response counts over six periods (Jan–Feb, Mar–Apr, May–Jun, Jul–Aug, Sep–Oct, and Nov–Dec). Patterns of burrow use were analyzed in two ways. First, we investigated factors affecting burrow switching, which we define as the number of unique burrows used by a tortoise in a particular sampling period. Second, we investigated burrow popularity, defined as the number of unique individuals using a burrow in a particular sampling period. Model variables used for each analysis are summarized in Table 1. All continuous model variables were centered (by subtracting their averages) and scaled to unit variances (by dividing by their standard deviation). This standard approach in multivariate regression modeling assigns each continuous predictor with the same prior importance in the analysis (Schielzeth 2010). All analyses were performed in R (version 3.0.2; R Development Core Team 2013).

Table 1 Model variables considered to characterize burrow use patterns in the desert tortoise, Gopherus agassizii

Investigating burrow switching of desert tortoises

In this model, the response variable was burrow switching, defined as the total number of unique burrows used by desert tortoises during each sampling period. An individual was considered to be using a burrow if it was reported either inside a burrow or within 25 m 2 grid around a burrow. The predictors included in the model are described in Table 1. In addition to the fixed effects, we considered three interactions in this model (i) sampling period × sex/age class, (ii) sampling period × seasonal rainfall, and (iii) local tortoise density × local burrow density. Tortoise identification and year × site was treated as random effects.

Investigating burrow popularity

For this model, the response variable was burrow popularity defined as the total number of unique tortoises using a focal burrow in a sampling period. The predictors included in the model are also described in Table 1. In this model, we also tested for three predictor interactions, including (i) sampling period × seasonal rainfall, (ii) sampling period × local tortoise density, and (iii) local tortoise density × local burrow density. We treated burrow identification and year × site as random effects.

Population stressors

Disease

We considered tortoises exhibiting typical signs of URTD including nasal discharge, swollen (or irritated/ sunken) eyes, and occluded nares to be indicative of an unhealthy animal. As diagnostic testing was not the focus of the studies collecting the data, we were unable to confirm the infection status of individuals. Knowledge of the confirmed infection status of animals, however, was not central to our study as our aim was to measure the behavioral response of symptomatic individuals only. We included health conditions of tortoises in the regression model as a categorical variable with two levels—healthy and unhealthy. An individual was considered to be unhealthy if it was reported to display clinical signs of URTD at least once during the sampling period.

Translocation

We accounted for translocation in the regression model by giving each surveyed tortoise one of the following five residency status at each sampling period: Control (C), Resident (R), Translocated (T), Ex-resident (ER), or Ex-translocated (ET). Translocations were carried out at four (BSV, FI, LM, SG) out of nine sites in our dataset for purposes described in previous studies (Drake et al. 2012; Nussear et al. 2012). All animals native to the site were categorized as Controls (C) during sampling periods before translocation occurred. For sampling periods post translocation, all native animals were categorized as Residents (R), and introduced animals were categorized as Translocated (T). One year after translocation, translocated and resident tortoises were considered to be Ex-translocated (ET) and Ex-residents (ER), respectively, to account for potential acclimatization of the introduced animals (Nussear et al. 2012). We note that one of the four translocation sites (SG) did not have native animals prior to translocation. No translocations were carried out at the rest of the five sites, so all animals surveyed at those sites were labeled as controls in all sampling periods.

Drought

The desert tortoise habitat typically receives most of the rainfall during the winter season. We therefore used winter rainfall to assess drought conditions in desert tortoise habitat. We defined winter rain during a year as average rainfall from November to February and used it as a proxy of drought condition for the following year. We note that summer rainfall in desert tortoise habitat varies from west to east, where summer rainfall becomes a larger component of the total annual precipitation in the East Mojave desert (Henen et al. 1998). Therefore, although we used winter rainfall as a proxy of drought conditions, we considered the effects of summer precipitation implicitly by including seasonal rainfall as a separate predictor (see Table 1).

Model selection and validation

Following Harrell (2002), we avoided model selection to remove non-significant predictors and instead present results of our full model. Using the full model allows model predictions conditional on the values of all predictors and results in more accurate confidence interval of the effects of interest (Harrell 2002). The Bayesian information criterion (BIC) of model selection was used to identify the best higher order interactions. A potential drawback of including all independent variables in the final model is multicollinearity. We therefore estimated generalized variance inflation factor (GVIF) values for each predictor. GVIF is a variant of traditional VIF used when any predictor in the model has more than 1 degree of freedom (Fox and Monette 1992). To make GVIF comparable across dimensions, Fox and Monette (1992) suggest using GVIF (1/(2.Df)) which we refer to as adjusted GVIF. We sequentially removed predictors with high adjusted GVIFs, recalculated adjusted GVIF, and repeated the process until all adjusted GVIF values in the model were below 3 (Zuur et al. 2010).

We carried out graphical diagnostics by inspecting the Pearson residuals for the conditional distribution to check if the models fit our data in each case. We detected under-dispersion in both the regression models. Under-dispersed models yield consistent estimates, but as equidispersion assumption is not true, the maximum-likelihood variance matrix overestimates the true variance matrix which leads to over-estimation of true standard errors (Winkelmann 2003). We therefore estimated 95 % confidence intervals of fixed and random effects using bootstrapping procedures implemented in ‘bootMER’ function in package lme4.

We tested for the significance of fixed factors in both the models using the likelihood ratio test (R function mixed from afex package Singmann 2013). For significant categorical predictors, we used Tukeys HSD (R function glht from the multcomp package, Hothorn et al. 2008) as a post hoc test of significant pair-wise differences among means. All reported P values of post hoc tests have been adjusted for multiple comparisons using the single-step method (Hothorn et al. 2008).

Results

Network analysis

Bipartite networks of asynchronous burrow use across all sites demonstrated considerable variation in the degree of tortoise nodes and burrow nodes (Fig. 3). Tortoises visited more unique burrows on average (4.03 ± 3.43 SD) and had a greater range of burrows visited in active seasons (1–9) than in inactive seasons (average = 1.46 ± 0.72 SD, range = 1–5). Less than 40 % of tortoises used more than one burrow during Nov–Feb (inactive) months (Fig. 3a). Most burrows in desert tortoise habitat were visited by a single tortoise during active and inactive season (Fig. 3b). Heterogeneity in the number of animals visiting burrows, however, tended to be slightly more during the months during the months of March- November (active =1.21+−0.56 SD) than November-February (inactive =1.08+−0.35 SD).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Frequency distribution of a Tortoise degree, i.e., unique burrows used by desert tortoises and b Burrow degree, i.e., unique tortoises visiting burrows during the active (Mar-Oct) and inactive (Nov-Feb) season of all surveyed years and sites, excluding zero degree nodes. y-axis represents normalized frequency counts of tortoises/burrows with normalized frequency counts of the average tortoise/burrow degree

The tortoise social network (constructed as a single mode projection of tortoise nodes from the bipartite network) demonstrated moderate clustering coefficient (0.36 ± 0.21 SD) and modularity (0.53 ± 0.15 SD). Twenty three out of the 24 networks that we analyzed had higher clustering coefficient and 17 social networks were more modular than random networks (Supplementary Table S3). Thirteen social networks out of the total 24 demonstrated significant degree homophily (when nodes with similar degree tend to be connected) and 11 of those had positive associations (Supplementary Table S3). Positive degree homophily suggests that tortoises using many unique burrows often use the same set of burrows and are therefore connected in the social network. Tortoise social networks also had a moderate positive degree centralization which indicates a small subset of individuals used more burrows than the rest in the sampled population. Within sexes, positive degree centralization was observed both within males (0.20 ± 0.08 SD) and females (0.17 ± 0.06 SD). Homophilic association by sex ranged from −0.6 to 0.11 indicating a preference for one sex to associate with the opposite. These negative sexwise associations, however, were not different than those expected by chance.

The association between tortoises in their social network was inversely correlated with geographical distances between them, indicating that individuals closer to each other preferred using the same set of burrows. The magnitude of correlation ranged from −0.22 to −0.89 with an average value of −0.49 (Fig. 4). The P value of the permutation test for all sites across active seasons of all surveyed years was less than 0.05, indicating a significant effect of geographical location on social associations (Supplementary Table S4). This result of spatial constraints driving social interactions is not surprising as the geographical span of surveyed sites were much larger (>1500 m) than the normal movement range of desert tortoises (Franks et al. 2011). However, the moderate value of correlations suggests other factors (such as environmental, social, density) could play an important role in desert tortoise’s asynchronous burrow associations.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Spatial constraints on asynchronous burrow associations during active seasons at the study sites with control animals. At each site, correlation is calculated between geographical distance and edge occurrence in the tortoise social network, and averaged over each surveyed year. Error bars represent standard errors with n = 8 (CS), n = 3 (HW), n = 2 (MC), n = 7 (PV), and n = 2 (SL). P value associated with each correlation measure is < 0.05

Regression analysis

Based on the observed heterogeneity in bipartite networks, we next investigated the relative effect of natural variables and population stressors on burrow switching patterns of desert tortoises (viz degree of animal nodes in bipartite networks) and popularity of burrows in desert tortoise habitat (viz degree of burrow nodes in bipartite networks). Supplementary Table S5 presents the best models of BIC values for the interactive predictors that explain burrow switching in desert tortoises and burrow popularity. The three interactions tested for burrow switching models were sampling period × sex/age class, sampling period × seasonal rainfall, and local tortoise density × local burrow density. We tested all possible combinations of the three interactions. The best model contained an interaction of sampling period × seasonal rainfall (Supplementary Table S5). For the burrow popularity model, we tested all possible combinations of the sampling period × seasonal rainfall, the sampling period × the local tortoise density, and the local tortoise density × local burrow density interactions. The best model included the sampling period × the local tortoise density and the local tortoise density × the local burrow density interaction term.

Multicollinearity tests revealed all three measures of temperature (average, max, and min) to have adjusted GVIF values of >3. The three predictors were therefore dropped from both the models. We also removed the sampling period × tortoise density interaction from the burrow popularity model as it inflated adj GVIF value of tortoise density to >3. σ 2 estimate of tortoise identification and burrow identification random effect was negligible (tortoise identification: σ 2 = 0, CI = 0–0.004, burrow identification: σ 2 = 0, CI = 0–0.01). Both random effects were therefore removed from the regression models.

Effect of animal attributes

Sex/age class had a significant effect on burrow switching (\({\chi ^{2}_{2}}\) = 16.75, P = 0.0002). Overall, adults used more unique burrows than non-reproductives. Among adults, males used a slightly higher number of unique burrows than females (Fig. 5, Supplementary Fig. S3a). There was no effect of body size on individuals’ burrow switching behavior (\({\chi ^{2}_{1}}\) = 0.2, P = 0.65).

Fig. 5
figure 5

The effect of various predictors on the two models of burrow use patterns in desert tortoises. Error bars indicate 95 % confidence intervals around the estimated coefficient value. For continuous predictors, the vertical dashed line indicates no effect—positive coefficients indicate increase in burrow popularity/switching with increase in predictor value; negative coefficients indicate decrease in burrow popularity/switching with higher values of predictors. For each categorical predictor, the base factor (solid data points) straddles the vertical line at 0 and appears without a 95 % CI. Positive and negative coefficients for categorical predictors denote increase and decrease, respectively, in burrow popularity/switching relative to the base factor

Effect of burrow attributes

Out of the six burrow attributes included in the model, burrow age and surface roughness around burrow had the highest impact on burrow popularity, i.e., number of unique individuals visiting the burrow (burrow age \({\chi ^{2}_{1}}\) = 46.07, P< 0.0001; surface roughness, \({\chi ^{2}_{1}}\) = 14.37, P = 0.0002). Burrow popularity was positively correlated with surface roughness indicating that burrows in flat sandy areas were visited by fewer unique tortoises than burrows in rough rocky areas (Fig. 5). Older burrows were visited by more unique individuals, with burrow popularity increasing e 0.08 times with each increment of age (Fig. 5). Burrows in areas with higher topographical position as indicated by GIS raster images were also more popular (\({\chi ^{2}_{1}}\) = 5.71, P = 0.02).

Effect of environmental conditions

Sampling period had a large effect on number of unique burrows used by desert tortoises (\({\chi ^{2}_{5}}=\) 160.96, P < 0.0001) as well as on burrow popularity (\({\chi ^{2}_{5}}=\) 176.25, P < 0.0001) as compared to other model predictors. Burrow switching of desert tortoises was highest during the months of May-June and September–October when they are typically more active, and lowest in winter months (Fig. 5). In the late summer (July–August), tortoises demonstrated slightly lower burrow switching than during the active season, but higher than the winter season. Within a particular year, the direction of the effect of seasonal rainfall varied across different sampling periods (sampling period × seasonal rainfall: \({\chi ^{2}_{5}}=\) 107.46, P < 0.0001). For example, high rainfall during the months of March–April reduced burrow switching in desert tortoises. On the other hand, individuals exhibited higher burrow switching with higher rain during the months of July–August (Supplementary Fig. S3b).

In contrast to the large variation in individuals’ burrow switching behavior between sampling periods, popularity of burrows did not vary during a large portion of the year (May–December). Total unique animals visiting burrows tended to be lower in the months of January–February and March–April, as compared to other months of the year (Fig. 5, Supplementary Fig. S4c). Seasonal rainfall had a positive correlation with burrow popularity (\({\chi ^{2}_{1}}=\) 6.02, P=0.01).

Effect of density conditions

An increase in the number of active burrows around individuals promoted burrow switching, whereas an individual used fewer burrows when there were more tortoises in the vicinity (Fig. 5). In the burrow popularity model, higher tortoise density around burrows increased the number of individuals visiting these burrows (Fig. 5). There was a significant interactive effect of the two density conditions on burrow popularity (\({\chi ^{2}_{1}}=\) 177.37, P < 0.0001)—increase in burrow popularity with higher tortoise density was low when there were more burrows in the vicinity of the focal burrow (Supplementary Fig. S4d).

Effect of population stressors

Population stressors of drought, health, and translocation had variable influences on burrow switching of desert tortoises (Fig. 5, Supplementary Fig. S4d). As compared to residents and controls, translocated animals demonstrated lower burrow switching during the year of translocation and also in the subsequent years (Fig. 5, Supplementary Fig. S5a). We did not find any differences between burrow switching levels of individuals exhibiting clinical signs of URTD and clinically healthy individuals (\({\chi ^{2}_{1}}=\) 2.51, P = 0.11). Burrow switching levels of all surveyed animals during drought years (indicated by lower winter rainfall), however, tended to be slightly lower in comparison to non-drought years (burrow switching: \({\chi ^{2}_{1}}=\) 3.5, P = 0.06).

Discussion

Although direct social interactions among solitary species are relatively infrequent, individual preference for certain shared refuge and foraging spaces may lead to a highly structured social system (Leu et al. 2011). In such species, knowledge of social network structure formed through refuge or forage associations can identify key influential individuals (Fortuna et al. 2009; Leu et al. 2011), and provide early-warning signals for environmental (or anthropogenic) disturbances (Jachowski et al. 2012; Moule et al. 2015) that may ultimately affect population fitness. In this study, we infer social associations between individuals of a relatively solitary species, the desert tortoise, by their asynchronous use of burrows. While descriptive approaches are common in the field of animal social networks (Pinter-Wollman et al. 2013), we sought to gain a mechanistic understanding behind individual variation in burrow-use associations of the desert tortoises. The degree of an individual in a bipartite network has biological and ecological importance as it indicates a decision to switch refuges. Refuge switching is associated with a tradeoff between the costs of increasing exposure to heat, predators, increased risk of infection, and the benefits of finding food and mates. The outcome of observed refuge switching patterns is important as theoretical models predict reduced survival of populations due to suboptimal refuge use decisions (Cooper 2015). Modeling optimal burrow switching that maximizes fitness in desert tortoises is challenging as it is difficult to quantify fitness costs in a long-lived species. Our study instead provides an approach to build baseline models of burrow use patterns. Any large deviation to baseline levels may indicate lower survival, foraging, and reproductive success for tortoises and thus burrow switching can serve as an immediate indicator of population stressors affecting long-term fitness consequences.

We show that social networks in desert tortoises formed due to burrow use preferences cannot be explained by random associations. In several wildlife systems, spatial constraints can play a large role in shaping social networks (Davis et al. 2015), and non-random associations may not be definitive evidence of social organization in a population. Desert tortoise social associations, however, were only moderately correlated to spatial distances, which corroborates earlier studies that report social organization in desert tortoises (Niblick et al. 1994; Bulova 1997). In general, the social networks were also clustered (0.23–0.59) and modular (0.34–0.68). However, higher clustering coefficient values have been reported in other social species [e.g, 0.54–0.57 in bottlenose dolphins Mann et al. 2012, 0.57–0.87 in guppies Croft et al. 2004, 0.81 in squirrels Manno 2008, and 0.57–0.67 in primates Pasquaretta et al. 2014] and even in a few relatively solitary species that have been studied [e.g., 0.7 in raccoons Hirsch et al. 2013, 0.59 in brushtail possum Porphyre et al. 2011]. The low (but significant) clustering coefficient value in desert tortoise social networks suggests that they do not form tight social bonds as compared to other social wildlife species. In social species, the network structure is known to affect population stability (Kurvers et al. 2014) and resistance to disease outbreaks (Cross et al. 2004; Godfrey et al. 2009; MacIntosh et al. 2012). Modular social networks of desert tortoises in particular can have important implications in the spread and persistence of infections. For example, few connections between subgroups in a social network can effectively localize new infections to a few individuals. For chronic infections such as URTD, these pockets of infection, however, can serve as sources of re-infection to other uninfected subgroups, eventually leading to persistent infection across the entire population.

Our analysis of burrow use heterogeneity in desert tortoises reveals that the period of the year and density of burrows around an individual are the main drivers behind the individual’s burrow switching decision. Low burrow switching levels in tortoises during winter and summer months reflects reduced movement to avoid severe weather conditions (Eubanks et al. 2003). Individuals visit more burrows in the months of May–June and September–October which coincides with high activity of nesting and mating in adults. Seasonal rainfall also influences burrow switching in desert tortoises. Tortoises use fewer burrows in high rainfall conditions in March–April months, which possibly reflects reduced activity during cold weather associated with spring storms. Infrequent summer rains, on the other hand, increase tortoise activity as individuals emerge from burrows to rehydrate (Nagy and Medica 1986; Peterson 1996). Our results of high burrow switching during summer rains (July–August) are consistent with these reports of increased activity. We also find that non-reproductive tortoises use fewer burrows than adults, which may reflect differences in costs and benefits associated with switching burrows. Leaving a refuge can present a greater risk to non-reproductives that are more vulnerable to predation (Wilson 1991), are prone to thermal stress due to their smaller size (Mushinsky et al. 2003), and do not benefit from the mating opportunities gained by burrow switching. Indeed, previous studies have found juveniles forage closer to their burrows and minimize time spent out of burrows (Mcrae et al. 1981; Mushinsky et al. 2003; Halstead et al. 2007). Future studies and management plans of desert tortoises may consider these differences in burrow switching behavior between adults and non-reproductive tortoises in order to mitigate increased predation risk by pervasive predators such as ravens.

While it has been shown that a small fraction of burrows in desert tortoises are visited by multiple animals (Bulova 1994; Harless et al. 2009), the mechanisms behind burrow popularity were previously unknown. Our results suggest that popular burrows can be identified using certain burrow characteristics such as surrounding topographical variables and age. As true burrow age is often hard to determine, we demonstrate the use of historical survey data to estimate proxy age of burrows. Once identified, these popular burrows can be surveyed throughout the year as there is only a minor effect of time of the year and seasonal rainfall on burrow popularity. Knowledge of active and popular refuges can have two important implications for the conservation and management of wildlife species. First, population density estimates usually rely on observations of animals located outside refuge space (Witmer 2005). For species that spend most of the time in a year in a refuge, survey of popular refuges can augment the current survey methods to get a more accurate estimate of population density. Secondly, declines of popular refuges can indicate reduced social interactions and mating opportunities for individuals. Reduced refuge popularity can also be indicative of higher a mortality risk—Esque et al. (2010) found higher mortality of desert tortoise in flat open areas where burrows, as our results indicate, are less popular compared to rough higher elevation sites. Active popular burrows can therefore be used (a) as sentinels of population health and (b) to identify critical core habitat for conservation and adaptive management of a wildlife species.

Of the three potential population stressors that we included in our model (disease, drought, translocation), translocation caused a change in burrow switching behavior of desert tortoises. Although translocated animals are known to have high dispersal tendencies (Nussear et al. 2012; Hinderle et al. 2015) and hence are expected to encounter and use more burrows, we found translocated individuals use fewer unique burrows than residents. Our results are supported by evidence of translocated tortoises spending more time on the surface and taking shelter under vegetation rather than using burrows (Hinderle 2011). The use of fewer burrows coupled with high dispersal rates can increase exposure of translocated animals to thermal stress and dehydration, potentially increasing mortality. Therefore, to improve translocation success, a fruitful area of investigation for future research will be to determine potential causes of this change in burrow use behavior in translocated tortoises. We used winter rain as a proxy of drought conditions as the Western Mojave receives most of its annual rainfall during the months of November–February and is important for the availability of food for desert tortoises in the spring (Duda et al. 1999; Lovich et al. 2014). Our results show a slight (but not significant) reduction in burrow use by tortoises during drought years. Reduced burrow switching may correspond to a smaller home-range of desert tortoises observed during drought years (Duda et al. 1999). Low winter rainfall condition is also known to increase predation of desert tortoises due to diminished prey resources (Peterson 1994; Esque et al. 2010). Lower burrow use during drought years can be therefore a behavioral response of desert tortoises to avoid predation or to reduce energy expenditure and water loss in years of low resource availability (Nagy and Medica 1986). Contrary to previous studies (McGuire et al. 2014), we did not find any effect of disease on burrow use behavior, possibly because we could not distinguish severe clinical signs with milder forms in our data. Although there was no evidence of disease influencing burrow use behavior in the present study, we note that it is likely for burrow use behavior (and in particular the burrows themselves) to drive infectious disease patterns in desert tortoises either directly, through cohabitation instances, or indirectly, by serving as focal sites of social interactions.

Conclusions

Our study demonstrates non-random associations in desert tortoises based on refuge use patterns. We formulate statistical models of burrow switching and popularity of burrows to investigate the mechanisms, including environmental, topographical, density factors, and population stressors behind refuge use preferences of desert tortoises. In combination, these models help infer the mechanisms behind heterogeneity in refuge use from the perspective of individuals as well as from the perspective of the refuges. This approach is particularly useful for species that are not overtly gregarious. For these species, refuge switching often correlates with reproductive and foraging success, and patterns of refuge use can be an important aspect to consider before implementing any management or conservation strategy. For example, popular refuges can be used to identify core habitat areas. In addition, sudden changes in the refuge switching behavior of individuals can be used as an early warning signal of disturbances that may ultimately affect population fitness. More broadly, our study provides insights toward the presence of and mechanisms behind non-random social structure and individual variation in a relatively solitary species by analyzing refuge-based associations. The structure of networks in social species is known to affect population stability and resilience to infectious diseases. Future studies are needed to establish such functional roles of social networks in relatively solitary species.