Introduction

Animals use acoustic signals to convey different types of information such as species identity, individual identification, condition, sexual or aggressive motivation and territorial ownership. The variability of spectral and temporal features contained in different calls or in a single call may convey different types of messages. For example, signal attributes that convey a species’ identity show little variation among individuals within a species. Conversely, acoustic features that convey individual identity evidence strong stereotypy within an individual but show larger variation between individuals (Bee et al. 2001). Additionally, features such as signal repetition rate or intensity may indicate motivation or condition and thus provide fundamental information for mate choice and for male–male interactions (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998).

In species in which males defend territories in complex spacing systems to obtain mates, such as in male calling aggregations, advertisement acoustic signals should inform receivers about species and sex identity, sender’s location, motivation and individual quality (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998). These different messages can be communicated in some species by large and complex acoustic repertoires while other species may exhibit more limited repertoires and rely on the plasticity of certain call features (e.g. Burmeister et al. 1999). Consequently, species with small repertoires can be excellent models to understand the interplay between variable and more stereotyped signal features not only conveying species identity but also mediating male–male competition and mate choice.

Many teleost fish species rely on their calls to acquire mates (e.g. Malavasi et al. 2003) and to keep intruders away from their territories (Ladich and Myrberg 2006). Teleost fish produce relatively simple sounds in small acoustic repertoires (Amorim 2006) and in a few species the vocal motor networks and the auditory systems have been well studied (Bass and Mckibben 2003). Hence, teleosts provide simple models to study the role of acoustic signals in reproductive and agonistic decisions in vertebrates.

Fish from the family Batrachoididae (toadfishes and midshipmen) have emerged as one of the groups where acoustic communication is best studied in this taxon (Bass and Mckibben 2003). During the breeding season, male batrachoidids produce advertisement calls (boatwhistles or hums) from their nests that are important for regulating male spacing and for mate attraction (Fish 1972; Brantley and Bass 1994). Nesting males form clusters and vocalise close together in a chorus (Bass 1996; Amorim et al. 2006). The gulf toadfish (Opsanus beta) changes calling rate and sound duration both during territorial intrusions and during increased crepuscular chorus activity, thus depicting a remarkable ability for teleost fishes to modulate calls according to the social context (Thorson and Fine 2002; Remage-Healey and Bass 2005). Further, a recent study based on passive acoustics has shown that the boatwhistles of the Lusitanian toadfish Halobatrachus didactylus are highly stereotyped and show individual differences when considering short time periods (5–10 min) (Amorim and Vasconcelos 2008). Individuality in fish vocalisations is unusual and has only been mentioned for batrachoidids and fishes of the Mormyridae family (Crawford et al. 1997; Amorim and Vasconcelos 2008; but see also Myrberg et al. 1993). Calling rate and acoustic features of batrachoidids’ vocalisations are also affected by environmental factors such as water temperature and lunar cycles (Brantley and Bass 1994; Maruska and Mensinger 2009). In summary, previous studies suggest that the batrachoidid advertisement call may carry different messages in one relatively simple signal.

Male Lusitanian toadfish nest frequently in intertidal estuarine areas and are thus faced with fluctuating environmental parameters, such as water level and temperature, which may directly affect acoustic communication. In fact, temperature influences muscle contraction and thus may affect sound production (Connaughton et al. 2000). Sound propagation is highly influenced by tide, since not only the sound attenuation increases significantly with lowering water level (Fine and Lenhardt 1983; Mann 2006) but also background noise often changes with tide. Because mate attraction in this species relies on a single call type that is broadcasted in a physically variable and acoustically complex environment, it provides an excellent model to study how environmental and social factors affect the variability of this advertisement signals. Here, we test the hypothesis that the advertisement call of the Lusitanian toadfish shows enough inter-male variability and intra-male plasticity to convey information of male identity and motivation in chorusing aggregations. We registered vocalisations from groups of Lusitanian toadfish in an intertidal area to examine individual differences (stereotypy) among well-identified Lusitanian toadfish males in different time spans, from minutes to 1 week. We examined how environmental constraints (tide level), male calling rate and social environment modulate the Lusitanian toadfish’s advertisement call and discuss the possible role of signal stereotypy and plasticity.

Material and methods

Study species

The Lusitanian toadfish, H. didactylus (Batrachoididae) is a benthic marine fish that inhabits coastal areas and brackish environments from the Gulf of Guinea to the Tagus estuary, Portugal, appearing occasionally up to the Bay of Biscay (Roux 1986). Breeding males build nests under rocks in shallow water and attract females to spawn with long advertisement calls (boatwhistles) from May to July, forming conspicuous choruses (dos Santos et al. 2000; Amorim et al. 2006). Females show low fecundity since they lay only a few hundreds of large eggs in a single batch on the roof of a nest (Modesto and Canário 2003; Costa 2004) whose survival is assured through male parental care.

Sound recording and analysis

We deployed 60 artificial concrete shelters every 1.5 m in rows along the shoreline in an intertidal area of the Tagus estuary (Portugal, Montijo, Air-Force Base 6; 38°42′N, 8°58′W). The shelters had a hemicylinder shape capped at one end (internal dimensions, 50 cm long, 30 cm wide and 20 cm high) and were readily occupied by toadfish in the breeding season. These nests were only exposed to air during spring low tides. The water level in the nesting area varied between 0 m and 2.8 m. Three groups of six to eight males (n = 22) that spontaneously occupied these artificial concrete nests were recorded over a period of 8 days in June/July 2006 and 2007, during the peak of the reproductive season. Subject males had a mean total length of 42.9 cm (range, 37.9–47.7 cm) and a mean eviscerated weight of 1,207 g (857–1,612 g). Each male was recorded for an average of 35 h (11–56 h). Nests with the subject fish were placed 1.5 m apart in two rows and were at least 15 m apart from other nests that could be occupied by other males. Nests’ entrances were closed with a plastic mesh that allowed prey items to enter but prevented males from abandoning the nest during recordings. The plastic mesh did not affect acoustic signals and allowed possible visual interactions. One hydrophone (High Tech 94 SSQ hydrophone, sensitivity −165 dB re 1 V/μPa, frequency response within ± 1 dB from 30 Hz to 6 kHz) was placed at about 10 cm from the entrance of each subject male’s nest and about 10 cm from the substrate. Simultaneous multi-channel recordings were made to a laptop connected to USB audio capture devices (Edirol UA25, Roland; 16 bit, 6 kHz acquisition rate per channel) controlled by Adobe Audition 2.0 (Adobe Systems Inc., 2005). Recorded sounds could be attributed to each male due to the high acoustic attenuation observed in the simultaneous multi-channel recordings between neighbouring males. Water temperature was measured every 3 h during recording periods and averaged 23°C (range, 19.5–28°C). All subject fish experienced similar water temperature variability during recordings.

Boatwhistles have been described in detail in Amorim and Vasconcelos (2008). This sound has a variable duration from a few hundred milliseconds up to over a second and is composed of three different segments or phases characterised by different durations, pulse periods, relative amplitude and dominant frequencies (Amorim and Vasconcelos 2008). The tonal phase (P2) of the boatwhistle is the longest and the most characteristic of boatwhistles in the Lusitanian toadfish and in other batrachoidids (Thorson and Fine 2002; Amorim and Vasconcelos 2008). We analysed boatwhistles for total sound duration (milliseconds, measured from the start of the first pulse to the end of the last pulse), pulse period of the tonal segment P2 (milliseconds, average peak to peak interval of six consecutive pulses in the middle of P2), dominant frequency of P2 (hertz, the frequency with maximum energy in P2), dominant frequency modulation (the ratio of the dominant frequencies of the initial and the tonal phases) and amplitude modulation (the ratio of the mean amplitude of the initial and the tonal phases). These acoustic parameters are depicted in Fig. 1. A previous study has shown that these are the most important acoustic parameters to discriminate among individuals (Amorim and Vasconcelos 2008). Sound analysis was carried out with Adobe Audition 2.0 and Raven 1.2.1 for Windows (Bioacoustics Research Program, Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Oscillogram (a), sonogram (b) and power spectrum (d) of a boatwhistle. Sound duration (thick continuous line), the initial phase (P1, fine continuous line) and the tonal phase (P2, fine dashed line) of the boatwhistle are depicted in the oscillogram. Dominant frequency (DF) of the tonal phase is shown in the sonogram and in the power spectrum. Detail of the boatwhistle tonal phase waveform depicting the pulse period (PP) (c)

Note that since the hydrophones were just 10 cm away from the calling males, any detected effects of tide level on acoustic parameters were not related to water level transmission loss dependence (see for example Fine and Lenhardt 1983; Mann 2006).

Statistical analysis

We calculated the mean and standard deviation (SD) for each of the five boatwhistle acoustic variables emitted by 13 males in two time frames: short, 10 boatwhistles per male emitted over a time period of no longer than 10 min; long, mean of 41 (range, 11–95) boatwhistles per male produced during up to 8 days. We subsequently computed the overall means and SDs for each acoustic variable using the previously calculated mean values for each male. We determined the within-male variability for the five acoustic variables by calculating the within-male coefficient of variance (CVw = SD/mean) for each male and subsequently computed the mean for all males. We also determined the between-male coefficient of variation (CVb) by dividing the overall SD by the respective overall mean. The ratio CVb/CVw was then calculated to obtain a measure of relative between-male variability for each boatwhistle feature. When this ratio is larger than one, it suggests that the acoustic parameter is more variable between individuals relative to its variability within individuals and could be used as a cue for individual discrimination (Christie et al. 2004). Kruskal–Wallis tests were computed to compare differences among males for each acoustic feature in the two time frames.

Discriminant function analysis (DFA) was carried out as a multivariate tool to determine if males could be discriminated based on their sounds considering these five acoustic variables and to verify which acoustic features better distinguish males. DFA also gives a measure of discrimination accuracy by revealing the percentage of sounds correctly assigned to each individual (Mundry and Sommer 2007). DFA were performed both on the short time frame (ten boatwhistles per male emitted within 10 min by 14 males) and on the longer time frame (ten boatwhistles per male emitted during up to 8 days by the same males, n = 13) data sets. Data were standardised [(xi−mean)/SD] to remove differences of magnitude observed between individuals for a given acoustic parameter. We confirmed that DFA assumptions were met with the inspection of residual plots (predicted vs residuals values, normal probability plots), by performing Levene’s and the multivariate Box M tests for homogeneity of variances/covariances and by checking tolerance levels to assess possible multicollinearity among variables. In addition, to validate the models obtained, a cross-validation method (‘leave-one-out’) was carried out (Mundry and Sommer 2007). In this method each sound is classified by the discriminant functions derived by the n−1 remaining sounds.

We also tested whether environmental and social factors and male calling rate had a significant effect in boatwhistle acoustic variables in a long time frame with multi-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). In this analysis, we used sounds registered in the longest possible time span per individual (up to 8 days) and during different tide levels and social environments. We considered an average of 34 sounds per male (range = 10–92) for 16 males. As data was unbalanced ANCOVA were based on sum of squares III. Tide level was included as a factor with three levels: 1 = full tide, 2 = ebb tide and 3 = low tide. Rising tide was not considered because of the reduced number of males calling during this tide level, which was significantly lower than at high and ebb tide, and lower, although not significantly, than low tide (Kruskal–Wallis test, n = 190, H = 26.11, p < 0.001; Fig. 2). Social environment was another factor with two levels: 1 = calling alone and 2 = calling in a chorus of at least two males. We included a third factor with two levels that represented male’s calling rate (1 = low rate <9 BWmin−1 and 2 = high rate ≥ 9 BWmin−1). Nine number of boatwhistles per minute (BWmin−1) was considered the cutpoint since the average calling rate for all fish was of 8.8 BWmin−1 (n = 576). This factor was included because exploratory analysis showed calling rate affected call features. We also considered calling rate as a dependent variable. In this case only tide and social factors were included in the models. Water temperature was included as a continuous variable (covariate) because it can influence acoustic parameters (Amorim et al. 2006). When the effect of this covariate was not significant, multi-way ANOVA were carried out instead. The final models complied with normality and homogeneity of variance assumptions. We also confirmed the absence of multicollinearity between the predictors (variance inflation factors were always smaller than 5 and tolerance levels larger than 0.1; Montgomery et al. 2006) since water temperature tended to be on average 2°C higher at low tide than in other tide levels.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Variation of the number of calling males (max = 8) with tide levels. Dots and error bars are means and standard errors. Different letters indicate pairwise significant differences given by Dunn tests, i.e. factor levels with the same letter show no significant differences

Non-parametric statistical tests were carried out when parametric assumptions were not met. Non-parametric statistics and ANCOVA tests were carried out with Statistica (9, Statsoft Inc., USA) and DFA were performed using SPSS (16.0, SPSS Inc., USA) for Windows.

Results

Stereotypy: short time frame

All five acoustic parameters showed significant differences between individuals (Table 1). There was a strong stereotypy in most acoustic parameters measured, and only the frequency modulation showed a within-male CVw larger than 0.11 (Table 1). Consistently, with the Kruskal–Wallis results, all five acoustic variables were more variable between than within males as all CVb/CVw ratios were larger than one (Table 1).

Table 1 Within-male variability (CVw) and between-male variability (CVb) for the five acoustic variables analysed from the same 13 Lusitanian toadfish males in a short (up to 10 min) and in a long time frame (up to 8 days)

A discriminant function analysis (DFA, n = 140, Wilks’ lambda = 0.0004, DF = 65, 580, p < 0.001) assigned boatwhistles to the correct male with an average success of 86% (range, 30–100%). The first two discriminant functions explained 74% of data variability with P2 pulse period weighing most heavily in explaining variation in the first function and sound duration and amplitude modulation in the second function (Table 2). After cross-validation the correct classification assigned by the DFA model was similar (80%).

Table 2 Standardised canonical DFA coefficients, eigenvalues and cumulative percentage of variance explained by the first two discriminant functions of a DFA classifying 14 Lusitanian toadfish males by their boatwhistles’ acoustic characteristics

Taken together, these data reveal that male Lusitanian toadfish show individual differences in the properties of boatwhistle in a time frame of a few minutes.

Variability: long time frame

Considering a period of time as long as possible for each male (up to 8 days), significant differences between males were kept for all five acoustic parameters (Table 1); however, only the pulse period of P2 showed a within-male CV smaller than 0.1 and all the CVb/CVw ratios were smaller than one showing that when a long time frame is considered all five acoustic variables become more variable within than between males. A discriminant function analysis ran on this long-term data set (DFA, n = 130, Wilks’ lambda = 0.09, DF = 60, 532, p < 0.001) showed that the average classification success decreased to 45% (range, 0–90%) with the first two discriminant functions explaining 72% of data variability. The percentage of correct classification after cross-validation (leave-one-out procedure) was 35%. In conclusion, significant differences among male calls in a long time scale were still found, although to a lesser extent than in a short time scale.

Sources of call variability: long time frame

Sound duration decreased significantly during low tide (Table 3; Fig. 3a) and this parameter was also significantly affected by the calling rate. The interaction between the factors calling rate and social environment was significant (Table 3) and sound duration significantly decreased in males calling at a low rate and significantly increased in males calling at a high rate with the raise of social complexity, i.e. in a chorus situation (Fig. 3b). There was a difference in sound duration between males calling at high and low rate only when the fish were singing in a chorus (Fig. 3b).

Table 3 Effects of tide level, social environment and male calling rate on boatwhistles’ acoustic variables
Fig. 3
figure 3

Effect of tide level on boatwhistle duration (a). Effect of social environment (calling alone or in a chorus) and male calling rate (low rate, filled circle and high rate, open circle) on sound duration (b). Circles are means computed for the covariate water temperature mean and error bars are 95% confidence intervals

P2 pulse period decreased significantly at low tide (Table 3; Fig. 4). The interaction term between tide and calling rate was significant. The decrease in the pulse period with water level was more marked in males calling at a high rate than at a low rate. Males calling at a high rate had a shorter pulse period in their calls and significant differences between call rate levels were observed at ebb and low tide levels (Fig. 4a). The interaction between tide level and social environment was also significant. Pulse period decreased gradually with water level in males calling alone, but in males calling in groups there was only an abrupt decrease at low tide. There was, however, no significant effect of social environment on pulse period as differences for this parameter between males calling alone or in a chorus were not significant in any tide level (Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effect of tide level and male calling rate (low rate, filled circle and high rate, open circle) (a) and of tide level and social environment (alone, filled square and chorus, open square) on P2 pulse period (b). Circles and squares are means and error bars are 95% confidence intervals

P2 dominant frequency was only significantly affected by calling rate (Table 3) and males calling at a high rate showed significantly higher values for this acoustic parameter than males calling at a low rate (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Effect of male calling rate on P2 dominant frequency. Dots are means computed for the covariate water temperature mean and error bars are 95% confidence intervals

Only the interaction term between tide level and calling rate had a significant effect on frequency modulation and males calling at a high rate showed higher frequency modulations than less vocal males at ebb tide (Table 3; Fig. 6). There was no significant effect of any variable or interaction term in amplitude modulation (Table 3).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Effect of tidal level and male calling rate (low rate, filled circle and high rate, open circle) on dominant frequency modulation. Circles and error bars are means and 95% confidence intervals

Social environment had an effect in the calling rate (ANCOVA, social environment: F 1,541 = 42.17, p < 0.001; covariate water temperature, F 1,541 = 6.16, p = 0.01). Data inspection showed that males that call alone mostly call at low rates, but males in a chorus call at all rates. There was a significant effect of the interaction between social environment and tide level (ANCOVA, interaction: F 2,541 = 11.55, p < 0.001, tide level: F 2,541 = 0.97, p = 0.38). Calling rate of males calling alone increased at low tide and with the exception of this tide level it was significantly lower than the calling rate for males in a chorus situation (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Effect of tidal level and social environment (alone, filled circle and chorus, open circle) on calling rate. Circles are means computed for the covariate water temperature mean and error bars are 95% confidence intervals

Discussion

Stereotypy vs. variability

There was strong stereotypy in the boatwhistles produced by Lusitanian toadfish males when considering periods of 10 min consistent with the study of Amorim and Vasconcelos (2008) based on unidentified fish. Males differed significantly in all five acoustic parameters and average correct classification of individuals based on their calls was high (86%) as shown by DFA, with P2 pulse period, sound duration and amplitude modulation being the best parameters to discriminate among males. Individual differences in signals are important to mediate social interactions, namely between neighbouring territorial males (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998). Individual recognition is especially relevant when animals defend long-term territories, such as breeding Lusitanian toadfish males do, because territory holders can reduce aggression towards familiar neighbours, which are less likely to intrude their territories (‘dear enemy effect’, Temeles 1994). Although this effect was not yet identified in toadfishes, examples exist in several taxa. For instance, territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbiana) show individual differences in their advertisement calls that mainly differ in the fundamental frequency (Bee and Gerhardt 2001a). Playback experiments have shown that this acoustic parameter mediates neighbour–stranger recognition in this species since they show less aggression in response to familiar calls (Bee and Gerhardt 2001b).

When considering a longer time frame (up to a week) there were still significant differences among Lusitanian toadfish males for all five acoustic features, but these become more variable within a male (Table 1). With the exception of P2 pulse period, all other parameters showed high intra-male variability over a week. This variability of the Lusitanian toadfish advertisement calls in a longer time frame suggests that boatwhistles are being modulated by either external factors such as the physical or the social environment or by internal factors such as the internal physiological state of the male (Remage-Healey and Bass 2005).

Interestingly, the P2 pulse period was the least variable acoustic feature both within and between males and kept the CVw and CVb under 0.1, regardless of the time span considered. Pulse period corresponds to the sonic muscle contraction period (Skoglund 1961; Fine et al. 2001), which is controlled by central vocal pattern generators in batrachoidids, and is stereotyped at the species level (Bass and McKibben 2003); nevertheless, P2 pulse period was the parameter that contributed the most to discriminating among individuals in a short time scale (DFA) suggesting it presents fine differences among individuals regardless of the time scale considered, which might be used for individual recognition among neighbouring nesting males.

P2 pulse period could also be indicative of male quality. In a recent study, Amorim et al. (2010) found that in the Lusitanian toadfish the pulse period reflects male condition and males with a higher body lipid content produced vocalisations with shorter pulse periods. These authors suggested that males that contract the sonic muscles at a very fast rate could reliably be indicating to neighbouring males or females their better quality (condition) with the ability to sustain sonic muscle contraction close to their physiological limit. Consistently, males of the nonpasserine bird Brown Skuas that produce long difficult calls close to their performance limit are honestly advertising a higher breeding success (Janicke et al. 2008).

Sources of variability

Tide level had a significant effect in boatwhistle duration and P2 pulse period, which showed lower values at low tide. Consequently, vocal conspicuity was reduced at low tide as the number of calling males (see “Material and methods”) and sound duration decreased. A decrease of calling effort at low tide is expected because low frequency sounds attenuate very rapidly in low water levels and the chance of being detected by distant females is reduced (Fine and Lenhardt 1983; Mann 2006). Nevertheless, males that were calling at a high rate showed a pronounced increase of the fundamental frequency (the inverse of the pulse period) with low tide thus increasing the chances of sound detection at low water levels (Mann 2006). To the best of our knowledge, the effect of tide levels on fish vocalisations has not been studied before, but Barimo and Fine (1998) have mentioned that oyster toadfish increase calling on incoming tide, suggesting that as other intertidal fish, batrachoidids may have endogenous activity rhythms related to tides with lowest levels of activity at low tide (Gibson 1982). Future work should address the stability of call rate and characteristics in Lusitanian toadfish males that inhabit deeper areas and are less subject to the harsh fluctuating physical environment of the intertidal area.

Social environment affected calling rate in the Lusitanian toadfish and chorusing males called at higher rates. Accordingly, calling rate is influenced by the vocal behaviour of nearby male conspecifics in other batrachoidids (Winn 1967; Fish 1972; Remage-Healey and Bass 2005). For example, Gulf toadfish males when experimentally placed in an active calling environment started to vocalise within 48 h (Remage-Healey and Bass 2005). Males in a chorus probably benefit from increased mate attraction, reduced assessment costs or reduced predation risks, although experimental evidence supporting these hypotheses is scarce (Gerhardt and Huber 2002). We have also observed that high-calling-rate and probably more motivated Lusitanian toadfish males produced longer boatwhistles than low-calling rate males, but only when singing in a chorus.. Burmeister and Wilczynski (2001) examined the influence of androgens on calling behaviour in the presence and the absence of social acoustic signals in male green treefrogs (Hyla cinerea) and concluded that the influence of androgens on the motivation to call depended on the social stimuli. It is likely that also in the Lusitanian toadfish, androgens have a differential effect in boatwhistle acoustic features, namely duration, dependent on social stimuli.

Male calling rate also had a significant effect on acoustic features of boatwhistles. High-calling rate males produced on average longer boatwhistles with higher fundamental frequencies. The higher calling rate and associated longer sound durations observed in the Lusitanian toadfish could be related to higher levels of circulating steroid hormones such as 11-ketotestosterone (11KT), which is a teleost-specific androgen (Remage-Healey and Bass 2006). Neurophysiological experiments have shown that steroid hormones exert a rapid and complex neuromodulatory effect in the activity of the vocal pattern generator and in the vocal behaviour of batrachoidids, modulating calling rate and call duration (Remage-Healey and Bass 2003, 2004, 2006). Also, nesting batrachoidid males have elevated 11 KT during vocal advertisement compared to non-calling periods (Knapp et al. 2001; Remage-Healey and Bass 2005). In addition, batrachoidid males presented higher calling rate, call duration and elevated levels in circulating 11KT after experiencing a territorial intrusion simulated by playback (Remage-Healey and Bass 2005). In this context, Lusitanian toadfish males with higher calling rates and longer sounds could reliably advertise high circulating androgen levels, which also mediates aggressive behaviour (social status) and reproduction (Remage-Healey and Bass 2006). Moreover, calling rate reflects male condition (body lipid content) in this species (Amorim et al. 2010), suggesting that calling rate and associated call features could be advertising male quality traits that are important for social status, territorial defence and possibly parental ability (Andersson 1994). From the female perspective, females that would choose males based on their vocal behaviour could both gain direct benefits (such as enhanced male parental care and better territories for the development of their offspring) and indirect (genetic) benefits (Andersson 1994).

In conclusion, our study has shown that the advertisement calls of the Lusitanian toadfish differ among individuals even when considering several days, but high stereotypy is found only in P2 pulse period when considering longer periods of time. Differences in this parameter could promote individual recognition and also advertise male quality if only some males can perform close to the physiological limit. Our study has also produced evidence to support the acoustic plasticity hypothesis since calling rate seems to be one of the major factors influencing boatwhistle characteristics and could be informative about male motivation and physiological condition (i.e. male quality). The social acoustic environment, on the other hand, seems to mainly influence calling rate (vocal facilitation). We suggest that absolute differences in calling rate and in call characteristics (mainly P2 pulse period, dominant frequency and sound duration), together with the differential way in which tide modulates boatwhistles according to male’s motivation level may be used in male–male assessment and in mate choice by females.