Introduction

Monogamous behaviour is often associated with biparental care for offspring (Smith and Wootton 1995; Reynolds 1996). Accordingly, environmental and social conditions that tend to increase the relative fitness benefits of polygyny may also promote the desertion of offspring by males (Smith and Wootton 1995; Gross 2005). The environmental context that influences the probability of offspring survival could also determine whether a female should continue caring for her brood after she has been deserted (Lazarus 1990). Evidence, however, suggests that deserted females often stay with their offspring (McNamara et al. 2002). Moreover, in most biparental species mate desertion is much more commonly documented in males than in females (for notable examples of species in which either sex is just as likely to desert as the other, see Bessinger and Snyder 1987; Griggio and Pilastro 2007).

The resulting sexual conflict over parental care has attracted considerable interest from biologists (e.g. Clutton-Brock and Vincent 1991; Queller 1997; McNamara et al. 1999; Kokko and Jennions 2008). A common approach to tackling this topic is to experimentally decrease the level of male participation in parental care activities, and then monitor the female’s response under controlled laboratory conditions (Houston et al. 2005). However, field-based studies are crucial if we want to understand how solitary females might compensate for the absence of male input in offspring care under ecologically relevant settings. In one such study, solitary Spanish sparrow (Passer hispaniolensis) females were found to invest greater effort into offspring provisioning compared with paired females but were still unable to attain a provisioning rate comparable to the combined effort of paired females and their mates (Marques 2004). In general, much of what we currently know about brood care compensation has come almost exclusively from studies on birds (reviewed in Harrison et al. 2009). By contrast, our understanding of this phenomenon in other taxa is less well developed.

Furthermore, previous studies of mate desertion have typically focussed only on changes in the amount of care whilst overlooking potential effects on the quality of care being given. For example, in bird studies, usually only feeding rates have been measured without taking into account the quality, or even quantity, of food being brought to the nest (Harrison et al. 2009). The latter could be important if, for example, there is a trade-off between quantitative and qualitative components of parental compensation. Hence, one should not assume, a priori, a positive correlation between these two aspects of care (i.e. quantity versus quality). More broadly, assessing only one, whilst ignoring the other, could bias our understanding of the consequences of mate desertion. Accordingly, in this field-based study, we used a non-avian study system (see ‘Methods’) to examine to what extent single females are able to adjust their offspring defence behaviour after their mates are no longer present and to distinguish between quantitative and qualitative aspects of compensatory care.

Methods

Our field-based study was carried out on two species of cichlid fish endemic to the crater lakes of Nicaragua, Central America: Amphilophus sagittae from Lake Xiloá (Stauffer and McKaye 2002; Elmer et al. 2009) and the arrow cichlid, Amphilophus zaliosus, from Lake Apoyo (Barlow and Munsey 1976; Barluenga et al. 2006). Several features of the biology of these two species make them particularly well suited for investigating differences in parental care behaviours between solitary and paired females. Both species belong to the Midas cichlid complex (Elmer et al. 2010), members of which were previously thought to be strictly monogamous and biparental (McKaye 1977; Rogers 1987, 1988; Barlow 2000), but have recently been found to also engage in female-only care under natural conditions (Lehtonen et al. 2010b). Like other members of this species complex, A. sagittae and A. zaliosus have a prolonged breeding season and establish stationary breeding territories (Barlow 1976; McKaye 1977; Rogers 1987; Barlow 2000), making them highly amenable to field observations. The free-swimming fry of these fish are independent enough to feed on their own by catching planktonic organisms (Barlow 1976) and, occasionally, grazing on mucus from the sides of their parents (Noakes and Barlow 1973; personal observation). Offspring, however, rely on their parents to defend them against conspecific and heterospecfic predators (Barlow 1976; Rogers 1987; McKaye and Murry 2008; Lehtonen et al. 2010a), and success in defence behaviours is critical to parental fitness (McKaye 1977; Rogers 1987, 1988; Barlow 2000). In this regard, both the quantity (i.e. rate of aggressive response) and quality of care (i.e. reaction distance and kinds of aggressive behaviours, see below for details) are likely to be important.

All observations were conducted between December 2007 and January 2008 at depths between 3.5 and 18 m using SCUBA. To address the question of female responses to mate desertion, we compared total work rate and other aspects of brood defence behaviour of naturally occurring solitary and paired females for A. sagittae (n single = 5, n paired = 21) and arrow cichlids (n single = 14, n paired = 15), and examined age distributions of the broods guarded by the two female types (i.e. solitary versus paired). The proportions of solitary and paired females included in the study reflected the frequency in which they were observed in the field (see Lehtonen et al. 2010b). Parental behaviour was assessed in relation to all but one of the arrow cichlid broods encountered during the course of the study. A. sagittae broods were chosen at random within the subset of encountered broods that were not older than 3 weeks (approximated age) in order to match brood ages of the two species as closely as possible. Because the likelihood of male desertion should increase with increasing survival prospects of the offspring (Smith and Wootton 1995; Gross 2005), it is reasonable to expect that solitary females would be guarding offspring that are older, and therefore larger, than average. However, if offspring survival of solitary females is clearly lower than that of paired females, we would expect to see the opposite pattern.

We adopted a sampling protocol that was successfully applied in an earlier study (Lehtonen et al. 2010a). Specifically, after a breeding territory was located, the observer swam to within approximately 2 m of the territory, taking care not to interrupt the parents or any neighbouring cichlid broods. The date, water depth, and female status (solitary or paired) were recorded. For most of the territories (Table 1), we also approximated total length of the female and age of free-swimming young (beginning at ‘age 0’ for juveniles that had just started to swim). These age estimations were based on the apparent size of juveniles, and were calibrated against our previous observations on growth rates both in nature and the laboratory. In addition, the activities of the parental fish were recorded for 15 min after an acclimatisation period of a minimum of 3 min. Each act of territory defence behaviour by the parent fish was classified according to one of the following three categories: ‘displays’ (flared fins or gills with gradual or no movement towards the fish invading the territory), ‘attacks’ (rapid movement towards the intruder until it fled) and ‘chase’ (an attack that was continued even after the intruder had started to flee). We consider it likely that these behavioural responses differ both in terms of costliness and effectiveness, with displays having the lowest level of expenditure and impact, and chases having the highest. For each of the three aggressive acts, we also noted the reaction distance, measured from the centre of the territory, as an approximation of territory size. For displays and attacks, this was estimated as the distance between the (approximated) centre of the brood and the invading fish at the time of the response. For chases, we approximated the distance between the centre of the brood and the parent at the end of the chase.

Table 1 Total length and brood age of solitary and paired females

Using the above data, we were able to assess (1) the total rate (quantity) of offspring defence (analysed as the sum of all displays, attacks and chases taking place within the 15-min observation period), (2) the quality of offspring defence in terms of reaction distances and relative frequencies of the three aggression categories (display, attack and chase) and (3) female success in brood defence as estimated from the ‘age score’ given to each brood (using data from all broods sampled across the entire breeding season). The total rate of offspring defence was analysed using linear models with species (Arrow cichlid or A. sagittae), status (single or paired) and their two-way interaction as predictor variables. Count data were square root transformed to satisfy normality, and t tests were used to test parameter estimates. In addition, we also analysed whether the total work rate differed between single females and pairs (with the latter based on the collective effort observed for both the male and female in the pair). To investigate whether the type of aggression differed between single and paired females, we analysed the counts of the three aggressive behaviours in a linear mixed model. This model was fitted by maximizing the log-likelihood, with species, status and aggression category as fixed factors, and the identity of the territory as a random factor. Non-significant terms were removed following model simplification using likelihood ratio tests (G 2 with α = 0.05, Crawley 2007). We then proceeded to test whether the relative frequencies of the three aggression categories differed between single and paired females. This was done by analysing the counts of all three types in a paired fashion (i.e. display vs. attack, display vs. chase and attack vs. chase), using generalized mixed models with a binomial error and a logit link function. Each comparison had the binomial occurrence of two of the aggression types as the response variable, status and species as a fixed factors, and territory as a random factor. Three models were each fitted using one of the three possible comparisons (display vs. attacks, display vs. chase and attack vs. chase). This procedure is equivalent to performing a multinomial (trinomial) analysis, but is easier to interpret. Model simplification was done using χ 2 tests (Crawley 2007). Finally, to assess differences in the reaction distance (proxy of territory size), female total length and fry age (log transformed) between solitary and paired individuals, we performed analyses of variance (ANOVAs), initially using observation date as a covariate. However, because the effect of observation date was, in all cases, non-significant, it was subsequently removed from analyses. ANOVAs were conducted using Systat 12 (SPSS Inc.) software, and for the rest of the analyses we used R 2.8.0 (R Development Core Team).

Results

Rate of offspring defence (quantity of care)

Solitary and paired females did not significantly differ in body size in either of the two species (Tables 1 and 2). Therefore, we did not include body size as a covariate in any of the remaining analyses. There was no significant interaction between species and status in the total number (per 15 min) of aggressive behaviours (F 1,51 = 0.362, p = 0.55). A model fitted without the interaction showed a significant effect of status (t 52 = 3.65, p < 0.001) but not of species (t 52 = 0.512, p = 0.61) on total aggression. In other words, solitary females exhibited a higher total rate of aggressive defence behaviours than paired females in both species (Fig. 1). When the contribution by the males were included in the analysis, there was no longer any effect of status on the number of aggressive behaviours (t 52 = 1.28, p = 0.21). This means that single females fully compensated for the lack of male participation by performing aggressive behaviours at levels similar to the combined efforts of male–female pairs (Fig. 1).

Table 2 Results of the two-factor ANOVAs
Fig. 1
figure 1

The total rate of aggressive responses towards potential brood predators. The white columns indicate female (either single or paired) total aggression rate and male participation is marked with black. Error bars of ‘pairs’ refer to the combined effort of both sexes. Sample sizes are given above each column

Quality of offspring defence

With regard to the three aggression categories (i.e. display, attack and chase), there was no significant three-way interaction between species, status and aggression category (G 2 = 1.98, p = 0.37), and no significant interaction between species and status (G 2 = 0.467, p = 0.49). However, there were significant interactions between aggression category and status (G 2 = 19.0, p < 0.001), as well as between aggression category and species (G 2 = 18.2, p < 0.001). In other words, the frequency of the three aggressive behaviours differed between single and paired females, as well as between the two species.

To examine what was driving these two interactions, the relative frequencies of the three aggression types were explored using binomial mixed models. None of the resulting models showed significant interactions between status and species (all χ 2 < 3.52, p > 0.05), so they were refitted without these terms. The reduced models revealed that, compared with singe females, paired females performed more chases relative to displays (z = 2.63, p = 0.009) and more chases relative to attacks (z = 2.51, p = 0.012, Table 3). However, the number of displays relative to attacks was not affected by female status (z = 0.505, p = 0.61). The two species did not differ in the relative frequency of chases to displays (z = 1.25, p = 0.21) or chases to attacks (z = 1.62, p = 0.11). However, arrow cichlids performed more attacks relative to displays compared with A. sagittae, regardless of female status (species: z = 4.43, p < 0.001; Table 3).

Table 3 Distributions of the three different aggression categories in single and paired females, represented both as a rate (aggression events/min) and relative frequency (%)

Reaction distances towards approaching predators were shorter for solitary than paired females (Fig. 2; Table 2) in both species (Fig. 2; Table 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Radius of breeding territories defended by females. White columns indicate single females and hatched columns-paired females. Sample sizes are also given

Success in offspring defence

Both species considered, the age of offspring guarded by solitary females was not different from that of paired females (Tables 1 and 2). However, in the case of A. sagittae, over the whole breeding season, juveniles guarded by single females were smaller than those of paired females (Tables 1 and 2; post hoc t test: t 23 = 2.21, p = 0.038).

Discussion

Earlier studies, mostly on birds, have indicated that when females adjust their workload following decreased male participation in parental care, they are able (or willing) to only partially compensate their provisioning rate in the absence of a partner (Houston et al. 2005; Harrison et al. 2009). In contrast, this study shows that solitary female cichlid fish were able to fully compensate for the absence of a partner in regard to the rate of aggressive responses towards potential offspring predators. Specifically, we found that solitary females attained approximately as high a response rate as that observed in paired fish (i.e. based on the collective effort observed for both the male and female in the pair). However, despite this extensive quantitative compensation in work rate, solitary females were not able to provide the same quality of care as that provided by paired females and their mates. In particular, we found that single females of both species exhibited shorter reaction distances and used less aggressive/costly brood defence behaviours (indicated by a low incidence of chases) than paired females. Defending a smaller area or being unable to leave the brood to chase away predators that pose the greatest threat can severely handicap the quality of brood defence. Hence, our findings underscore the importance of considering whether females can compensate for the quality, as well as quantity, of care, with the former having been largely neglected by the main body of studies in this field (Harrison et al. 2009).

In A. sagittae, the average size of juveniles defended by solitary females was smaller than that defended by paired females, as measured over the whole breeding season. Offspring of solitary females may simply not survive as well as those of paired females, due to the challenges solitary females face when trying to defend the brood against offspring predators. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that the fry of solitary females may also be growing at a slower rate due to impeded feeding. Even though cichlids do not directly capture food for their young, single A. saggitae females may be less efficient at guiding young to food within their (smaller) territory or to provide enough mucous for their offspring to graze.

In contrast to the patterns observed for A. saggitae, we did not find any differences in the average brood size of solitary versus paired females in the arrow cichlid (A. zaliosus). We propose that this is due to interspecific differences in survivorship of the broods of paired females. At the time of the study (breeding season of 2007–2008), arrow cichlid broods seemed to have a very low-survival rate (irrespective of the status of the female), while A. sagittae broods had a higher general survivorship (Lehtonen, McCrary and Meyer, unpublished manuscript). Furthermore, field studies conducted on other Neotropical cichlids have shown that males generally do not abandon the brood until after the juveniles have reached the free-swimming stage (e.g. Wisenden 1994; Jennions and Polakow 2001; Vélez et al. 2002; personal observations in Nicaragua). Hence, we would not expect differences in brood survival between the two female categories until the juveniles have already been free-swimming for several days.

Why should solitary cichlid females show a greater quantitative compensation for the lack of a mate than females of most other animals studied to date (Houston et al. 2005; Smiseth et al. 2005; Harrison et al. 2009; Suzuki and Nagano 2009)? One possibility is that previous studies have mostly focused on brood incubation and food provisioning, whereas if solitary cichlid females were not compensating their rate of brood defence so extensively, their offspring might have extremely low-survival prospects due to the high rate of predation and breeding territory competition (McKaye 1977; Rogers 1988). Furthermore, the relationship between offspring number and costs of parental care are likely to be different in fishes as compared with birds or, indeed, many other animal groups (e.g. Lazarus and Inglis 1986; Wisenden 1999; Rauter and Moore 2004). However, more research, preferably across a range of taxa, would be needed to assess how the cost-benefit dynamics of care affect the quantitative aspects of care compensation provided by single females.

As the non-invasive approach of this study did not allow experimental removal of males for ethical and practical reasons, it may seem possible that single females included in the study represented a non-random subset of females in the population. However, we consider it very unlikely that the ‘quality’ of solitary females was significantly biased or, indeed, that any such bias could explain the observed patterns in brood defence. Firstly, we found no significant difference in body size between solitary and paired females. This is important because body size is known to correlate with measures of quality in Neotropical cichlids, especially in regard to egg number and parental care (McKaye 1986; Wisenden 1995; Barlow 2000). Secondly, contrary to what should be expected if single and paired females differed in quality, shifts in the behaviour of the former did not consistently indicate lower (short response distances, a low prevalence of chases) or higher (high rate of aggressive responses) than average quality. Hence, our results suggest that solitary females modified their behaviour in order to cope with the altered demands of offspring care rather than any attributes of their intrinsic quality per se.

In conclusion, the results of this study show that when females are left to care for their offspring on their own, even a full compensation in parental work rate does not necessarily match the quality of care paired females are able to perform in conjunction with their mates. Our study therefore underscores the importance of considering not only quantitative but also qualitative aspects of care compensation.