Introduction

The ability to discriminate conspecific individuals on the basis of genetic relatedness, kin recognition, has been suggested for numerous animals in a range of taxa (for reviews, Fletcher and Michener 1987; Waldman 1988; Hepper 1991; Sherman et al. 1997; Holmes 2004). As predicted by inclusive fitness theory (Hamilton 1964), kin recognition prevails in social animals. However, sociality is not an inevitable condition for the evolution of kin recognition abilities. For instance, the ability to discriminate kin and non-kin may be selectively advantageous for non-social animals that use fluctuating resources and live in conspecific aggregations (Fellowes 1998). Kin recognition has been primarily studied and found in the contexts of nepotism, parental care, mate choice, schooling behavior, and aggression-related behaviors such as cannibalism.

Kin recognition systems consist of three separate components: expression (production of the kin label), perception (the recognition mechanism), and action (kin-biased behavior) (Reeve 1989; Sherman et al. 1997; Liebert and Starks 2004; Starks 2004). Regarding the perception component, four potential mechanisms of kin recognition have been suggested: (1) context-based recognition, where templates of true genetic kin are learned from environmental features (mainly spatial) that are shared with and are exclusively available to a kin-group, (2) recognition alleles, which cause both the production and recognition of a phenotypic cue, (3) prior association (familiarity), which allows identifying familiar individuals, and (4) phenotype matching (Blaustein 1983; Waldman 1988; Halpin 1991; Mateo 2004). Phenotype matching (term coined by Holmes and Sherman 1982) is an extension of or addition to prior-association-based recognition. It allows identifying familiar and unfamiliar kin due to having formed a prototypic template of kin by learning the phenotypes from familiar individuals (Sherman et al. 1997; Hepper and Cleland 1998; Heth et al. 1998; Mateo 2004) or from self (Hauber and Sherman 2001). The latter form of phenotype matching is commonly referred to as self-referent phenotype matching or “armpit effect” (Dawkins 1982). The process leading to prior-association-based recognition has also been termed direct familiarization, whereas the process leading to phenotype matching has been termed indirect familiarization (Blaustein and Porter 1996).

Prior-association-based recognition and phenotype matching are the mechanisms most often assessed and found. Among vertebrates, phenotype matching was revealed in mammals (e.g. ground squirrels, Heth et al. 1998; Holmes and Sherman 1982; Mateo 2002; hamsters, Mateo and Johnston 2003; beavers, Sun and Müller-Schwarze 1997; primates, Smith et al. 2003), birds (e.g., peacocks, Petrie et al. 1999) and fish (e.g., trout, Brown et al. 1993). However, the mechanism most often found in these taxa is prior association (for mammals, see e.g., Holmes and Sherman 1982; Halpin 1991; Pillay 2002; for fish, see review by Ward and Hart 2003; and for birds, e.g., Choudhury and Black 1994; Komdeur et al. 2004; van der Jeugd et al. 2002; Sharp et al. 2005). Among invertebrates, it is mainly social hymenoptera that have been shown to use recognition mechanisms based on prior association and/or phenotype matching (e.g., Buckle and Greenberg 1981 for sweat bees; Provost 1991 for ants; Getz and Smith 1986 for honey bees; Gamboa 2004 for review on social wasps). In contrast, studies examining the kin recognition mechanisms used by non-social arthropods are lacking. Knowing which mechanism, familiarity-based recognition or phenotype matching, is mediated by association is fundamental for interpreting any form of intraspecific interaction and identifying analogies/differences in life history and behavior among different organisms.

If it is usually related individuals that grow up in the same site (e.g., nesting species or non-social species laying their eggs in clutches), association may be used as a reliable indicator of kin (Blaustein and O’Hara 1986). In the predatory mite Phytoseiulus persimilis Athias-Henriot, the focal animal of this study, the probability of kin groups to occur is determined by the patchy distribution and life history of its prey, the herbivorous two-spotted spider mite Tetranychus urticae Koch (Gerson 1985; Sabelis 1985a; Sabelis and Dicke 1985). P. persimilis is a highly specialized predator of tetranychid mites such as the polyphagous T. urticae (for review, McMurtry and Croft 1997). T. urticae primarily lives on the abaxial leaf surfaces where the mites create distinct patches (Gerson 1985). Gravid P. persimilis females forage and deposit their eggs in the spider mite patches. Each predator female may lay multiple egg clutches, the size of which depends on the prey density within patches (e.g., Nagelkerke et al. 1996). Due to the high reproductive rate of P. persimilis (up to five eggs per female per day), offspring are often aggregated in prey patches (e.g., Sabelis 1985b). When prey within a patch diminishes, food competition intensifies, and the likelihood of cannibalism increases (Polis 1981; Elgar and Crespi 1992). Both adult and juvenile P. persimilis are cannibalistic (Schausberger and Croft 2000, 2001; Schausberger 2003, 2004), yet adult females tend to leave the patch before the spider mites are locally extinct (Vanas et al. 2006). Juvenile individuals are less dispersive and may cannibalize each other close to or after prey extinction (Sabelis 1981; Pels and Sabelis 1999; Schausberger 2003). Depending on the number and relatedness of predator females that colonized a spider mite patch, juveniles within a patch may be only kin—which is the majority of cases—or a mixture of kin and non-kin (e.g., Nagelkerke et al. 1996). Such circumstances seem ideal to promote the evolution of kin recognition abilities.

P. persimilis develops through four successive life stages before reaching adulthood: egg, larva, protonymph, and deutonymph. Larvae do not feed and are particularly vulnerable to cannibalism by older and/or larger life stages such as protonymphs (Schausberger and Croft 1999; Schausberger 2003, 2004, 2005). Previous studies on recognition of kin and/or familiar larvae by cannibalistic P. persimilis protonymphs (Schausberger 2004, 2005) suggested that (1) larvae learn the cues of associated individuals and later on treat familiar individuals as kin, (2) egg/larval associations and interactions may be manipulated by the ovipositing female via egg placement, which may affect the cannibalism risk for offspring and their performance as potential cannibals later in life, (3) cannibalistic protonymphs discriminate familiar and unfamiliar larvae, irrespective of genetic relatedness, and avoid eating the former. Association with conspecifics early in life has significance for who is treated as kin by juvenile cannibalistic P. persimilis later in life, but the mechanism used is not known. The aforementioned observations would be consistent with both prior association and phenotype matching, but not with the other two of the four possible mechanisms. This study therefore aims at distinguishing the two mechanisms.

Materials and methods

Study species, populations, and individuals

P. persimilis used in experiments were offspring of females withdrawn from five distinct populations (EL, ER, PANF, OR, and G). Populations EL and ER were founded with individuals originating from a mass production company of beneficial mites and insects (Biohelp, Vienna); population PANF was founded with individuals collected in Sicily, population OR with individuals collected in Oregon (USA), and population G with individuals collected in Greece. Populations were founded with 30 to 100 individuals each and maintained in the laboratory for about 0.5 to 3.5 years before starting the experiments. Each population was maintained on an artificial arena consisting of a plastic tile (15 × 15 cm) resting on a water-saturated foam cube in a plastic box and surrounded by water-saturated tissue paper. The predators were fed mixed stages of two-spotted spider mite, T. urticae, by adding infested bean leaves on to the arenas in 2 to 3-day intervals. Rearing units of different populations were kept in separate places to avoid cross contamination. Environmental conditions were 20 to 25°C, 60 to 80% RH, and 16:8 h L/D photoperiod.

Recognition mechanism experiments

To assess the ability to recognize familiar individuals and distinguish between recognition based on familiarity and recognition based on phenotype matching, I performed two experiments. In experiment 1, I tested whether cannibalistic P. persimilis protonymphs that were associated with either siblings or non-kin in the larval stage discriminate familiar and unfamiliar larvae. In experiment 2, I tested whether cannibalistic P. persimilis protonymphs that were associated with either sibling or non-kin referents in the larval stage discriminate unfamiliar sibling larvae and unfamiliar non-kin larvae. In either treatment of experiment 2, one of the two prey larvae was a sibling of the referents (Table 1). Each experiment consisted of three parts: obtaining sibling eggs, the association phase, and the choice test.

Table 1 Experimental design in choice tests where a cannibal was simultaneously offered a sibling larva and a non-kin larva. The cannibal was previously associated with siblings or non-kin (referents). Prey larvae were either familiar or unfamiliar to the cannibal

To obtain sibling eggs, gravid females were randomly withdrawn from populations and singly placed on egg laying arenas. Each egg laying arena consisted of a leaf placed adaxial surface down on a water-saturated foam cube in a small plastic box (5 × 5 cm) half filled with water. Strips of moist tissue paper were folded over the edges of the leaf to prevent the mites from escaping. Eggs were collected in 24-h intervals and stored in the refrigerator at 8°C until they were used in experiments.

Acrylic cages were used for the association phase and the choice tests. Each cage consisted of a cylindrical cavity (1.5-cm diameter) drilled in an acrylic plate (0.5-cm-thick). The cavity was covered with a mesh on the lower side and closed on the upper side with a microscope slide (Schausberger and Croft 2001). The association phase proceeded as follows: an egg (the prospective cannibal) was placed in an acrylic cage. Then, four to seven eggs (the prospective referents, either siblings or non-kin of the prospective cannibal) were added after 12 to 16 h. Referents are those individuals that provide the reference an internal template is based upon (Tsutsui 2004). Non-kin referents were laid by a female from a different population than the cannibal. Moreover, a further four to seven eggs, sibling of the referents, and thus, either sibling or non-kin of the prospective cannibal, were placed in a separate cage. They were used as unfamiliar siblings or unfamiliar non-kin in experiment 1 and unfamiliar siblings of the referents in experiment 2. Cages were then monitored for hatching larvae at irregular intervals. The prospective cannibal was more advanced in its development and hatched 12 to 16 h earlier than the referents. The prospective cannibal molted from the larva to the protonymph while the referents were still in the larval stage, and was thereafter subjected to a choice test.

To start a choice test, the prospective cannibal (a protonymph) and two prospective prey (two larvae) were transferred to a cage. In experiment 1, cannibals, previously associated with siblings, were caged with a familiar sibling larva and an unfamiliar non-kin larva; cannibals, previously associated with non-kin, were caged with a familiar non-kin larva and an unfamiliar sibling larva (Table 1). Each treatment of experiment 1 was replicated 27 times. In experiment 2, cannibals, previously associated with siblings, were caged with an unfamiliar sibling larva and an unfamiliar non-kin larva; cannibals, previously associated with non-kin, were caged with an unfamiliar sibling larva and an unfamiliar non-kin larva where the latter was a sibling of the non-kin referent (Table 1). Treatments of experiment 2 were replicated 21 and 27 times. Before transferring the prospective prey to the choice cages, larvae were marked by red and blue watercolor dots on the dorsal shield to make them distinguishable. In each choice situation, colors were randomly assigned to familiar and unfamiliar larvae and sibling and non-kin larvae, respectively. Cages were monitored at irregular intervals of 10 to 25 min until cannibalism on one of both larvae occurred. Status of the cannibalized larva (sibling or non-kin to the cannibal—experiments 1 and 2; familiar or unfamiliar to the cannibal—experiment 1; sibling or non-kin to the referents—experiment 2) and time elapsed until cannibalism occurred (experiments 1 and 2) were recorded. Egg laying arenas, association cages, and choice cages were stored at 23 to 25°C.

Data analyses

Replicates at which one of both prey larvae molted to a protonymph before the occurrence of cannibalism or at which no cannibalism occurred within 6 h after starting the experiment were excluded from analyses. Log-linear analyses were used to test the influence of the victim’s relatedness and familiarity to the cannibal (experiment 1) and the victim’s relatedness to the cannibal and to the referents (experiment 2) on the choice of the cannibal. A separate analysis was performed for each explaining variable, and prior association with siblings or non-kin was used as a covariate. The influence of the victim’s familiarity and relatedness to the cannibal (experiment 1) and the influence of the victim’s relatedness to the cannibal and to the referents (experiment 2), respectively, on the time elapsed until cannibalism occurred were analyzed by univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA).

Results

In experiment 1, P. persimilis protonymphs discriminated familiar and unfamiliar larvae and preferentially cannibalized the latter when given a choice. Familiarity had a significant effect on the choice of the cannibals (log-linear analysis, Z = 2.63, p = 0.009), whereas genetic relatedness between cannibal and victim had not (log-linear analysis, Z = −0.27, p = 0.787; Fig. 1). Moreover, cannibalism on sibling larvae occurred significantly sooner than cannibalism on non-kin larvae regardless of familiarity (Tables 2 and 3). In experiment 2, protonymphs did not discriminate unfamiliar sibling larvae and unfamiliar non-kin larvae, although they were familiar with siblings of one of the two stimulus larvae (log-linear analysis, relatedness between cannibal and victim Z < 0.01, p > 0.992; relatedness between referents and victim Z = 0.29, p = 0.772; Fig. 2). Protonymphs given a choice between two unfamiliar larvae cannibalized siblings as early as they cannibalized non-kin, irrespective whether previously associated with siblings or non-kin (Tables 4 and 5).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentage of protonymphs cannibalizing the sibling or non-kin larva when offered a familiar and an unfamiliar larva (experiment 1). Protonymphs were familiar with siblings or non-kin. Numbers on top of bars indicate the number of replicates

Table 2 Time elapsed (min, mean ± SD) until protonymphs cannibalized the sibling or non-kin larva when offered a familiar and an unfamiliar larva (experiment 1). Cannibals were either familiar with siblings or non-kin
Table 3 Univariate ANOVA for the effects of relatedness and familiarity between cannibal and victim on the time until cannibalism occurred when the cannibal was offered a familiar and an unfamiliar larva (experiment 1). Cannibals were either familiar with siblings or non-kin
Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage of protonymphs cannibalizing the sibling or non-kin larva when offered an unfamiliar sibling and an unfamiliar non-kin larva (experiment 2). Protonymphs were previously associated with siblings or non-kin. Numbers on top of bars indicate the number of replicates

Table 4 Time elapsed (min, mean ± SD) until protonymphs cannibalized the sibling or non-kin larva when both were unfamiliar to the cannibal (experiment 2). Cannibals were previously associated with either siblings or non-kin (referents)
Table 5 Univariate ANOVA for the effects of relatedness between cannibal and referents (referents’ relatedness) and between cannibals and victim (victim’s relatedness) on the time until cannibalism occurred when the cannibal was offered two unfamiliar larvae (experiment 2). Cannibals were previously associated with either siblings or non-kin (referents)

Discussion

Association early in life (in the larval stage) mediated recognition based on familiarity, but not phenotype matching in juvenile predatory mites, P. persimilis. In the larval stage, prospective cannibals learned the cues of associated conspecific larvae. After molting to protonymphs, cannibals treated familiar and unfamiliar larvae differently in that they preferentially cannibalized unfamiliar larvae irrespective of whether sibling or non-kin. Cannibals, however, did not discriminate unfamiliar larvae that differed in genetic relatedness, sibling and non-kin, although they were familiar with siblings of one of the two stimulus larvae.

The perceptual distinction among recognition mechanisms, in particular between recognition based on familiarity and phenotype matching, is a controversial issue (see, e.g., Tang-Martinez 2001; Mateo 2004). The main problem is that the neural process cannot be observed directly, but is inferred from the behavioral responses of the animals, i.e., the action component. Tang-Martinez (2001) argued there exists only a single genuine recognition mechanism. A more widely accepted view is that familiarity-based recognition and phenotype matching are different perceptual mechanisms (Holmes 2004; Mateo 2004). The former requires an exact match to an individual template, whereas the latter generalizes from individual templates that have been formed from individuals sharing phenotypic traits to a common representation of kin (Mateo 2004). Differential treatment of familiar and unfamiliar larvae by P. persimilis protonymphs reared under the same conditions may hence indicate individual recognition using genetically determined cues.

Why does association mediate recognition by familiarity but not phenotype matching in cannibalistic P. persimilis protonymphs? In general, early in life, a flexible and reversible recognition mechanism seems more favorable than a static one (Mateo 2004). Individual templates may be stored and used for limited time periods and then vanish again, whereas prototypic templates may be irreversible and stored throughout life (Tang-Martinez 2001; Holmes 2004; Mateo 2004). Errors in template learning and formation would thus have more dramatic consequences with prototypic than with individual templates. Prior-association-based recognition allows constant updating and modification of templates, generalizing from individual templates to form a prototype and extending the mechanism to phenotype matching later in life (Fletcher 1987; Liebert and Starks 2004; Mateo 2004). Familiarity-based recognition seems selectively advantageous for P. persimilis juveniles because recognition of larvae is relevant for only a short period of time, and a few larvae provide enough nutrients to proceed with development (Walzer and Schausberger 1999). Moreover, it allows mothers to reduce the risk of offspring cannibalism when colonizing a patch that harbors eggs from another female via selective egg placement (Schausberger 2005).

Time elapsed until sibling/kin cannibalism occurs seems context-dependent. Consistent with recent experiments (Schausberger 2004, 2005) sibling/kin cannibalism occurred significantly earlier than non-kin cannibalism when the cannibals were offered familiar and unfamiliar prey. Possible proximate and ultimate reasons for the occurrence of quick sibling cannibals have been put forward previously. They include preferential association of siblings/kin and consequently earlier sibling/kin cannibalism without representing a true preference and two types of cannibals with differing preferences (Schausberger 2004, 2005). Exceptionally quick sibling/kin cannibalism could also be interpreted as selfish behavior if it benefits direct fitness at the detriment of indirect fitness (Hamilton 1964, 1970; Mock and Parker 1997). As previously suggested (Schausberger 2004, 2005), those quick sibling cannibals appear to use self-referent phenotype matching to discriminate siblings/kin and non-kin because cannibals unfamiliar with siblings/kin still recognized “true” kin (inferred from time until cannibalism occurred), but treated non-kin as if kin (inferred from cannibalism avoidance). Scrutinizing the circumstances that promote the occurrence of quick sibling cannibals and/or make them apparent and determining their fate later in life remain intriguing tasks for future studies.

This study corroborates recent findings that cannibalistic P. persimilis protonymphs treat any familiar conspecific larva as kin irrespective of genetic relatedness (Schausberger 2005). Familiarity as the sole mechanism to determine kin and used as a proxy of relatedness has been shown for diverse organisms and contexts: Association attractiveness among salmon varies with familiarity and odor concentration (Courtenay et al. 2001), guppies choose shoal mates based on familiarity (Griffiths and Magurran 1999), familiar piglets show less aggressiveness than unfamiliar ones irrespective of genetic relatedness (Stookey and Gonyou 1998), juvenile sticklebacks compete less with familiar individuals without any indication that genetic relatedness plays a role (Utne-Palm and Hart 2000), and penguin chicks discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar calls but not between calls of familiar kin and non-kin (Nakagawa et al. 2001).

Accumulating evidence suggests that prior association is the mechanism more frequently encountered than phenotype matching (e.g., Komdeur and Hatchwell 1999; Mateo 2004). The two mechanisms may be distinct, or phenotype matching may be an extension of prior association. In no case do the two mechanisms exclude each other. There exist several examples that one and the same organism may use both mechanisms. Precedents are Belding’s ground squirrels (Holmes and Sherman 1982; Mateo and Johnston 2003; Mateo 2004), lambs (Ligout and Porter 2003), social paper wasps (Gamboa et al. 1986; Bura and Gamboa 1994; Gamboa 2004), and the focal animal of this study. P. persimilis may use recognition based on self-referent phenotype matching (Schausberger 2004; Enigl and Schausberger 2004), phenotype matching (Schausberger 2005), and prior association (Schausberger 2004, 2005, present study).

Different perceptual mechanisms may be used in different social contexts, and the ability to switch between mechanisms may have an ontogenetic or developmental component. As suggested for Belding’s ground squirrels, the ability to prior-association-based recognition is expected to precede an ability to phenotype-matching-based recognition (Mateo 2004). The mechanisms used by P. persimilis seem to have an ontogenetic component and are context-dependent: Prior-association-based recognition is used by protonymphs reared in association with conspecifics (Schausberger 2004, 2005, present), self-referent phenotype matching is used by protonymphs reared in isolation (Schausberger 2005) and virgin females in mate choice (Enigl and Schausberger 2004), and phenotype-matching-based recognition (self-referent and/or after learning the cues of own eggs) is used by ovipositing females (Schausberger and Croft 2001; Schausberger 2005). Determination of the chronological order, hierarchy, and context dependence of the kin recognition mechanisms that may operate in one and the same P. persimilis individual are intriguing tasks for future research.