Introduction

In order to safely perform basic activities of daily living, humans rely on the ability to rapidly react to their environment. Reaction time (RT) is defined as the delay between the presentation of a stimulus (perceptual phase) and the response by the human (motoric phase) (Magill 2011). There are two kinds of RT tasks: simple and choice. Simple RT involves one response corresponding to one stimulus. Choice RT involves a distinct response to each type of stimulus, typically presented in a random sequence. Simple RT is typically due to input–output delays of single neurons and of neural chains, and thus exhibit faster responses than choice RT tasks. Choice RT is considered to indicate a more complex processing requirement of the central nervous system (Nijhawan 2008).

A growing body of literature has demonstrated that anodal (facilitatory) transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the upper limb motor cortex (M1) has the ability to improve performance of complex functional motor tasks in healthy and patient populations (Nitsche et al. 2008). Anodal tDCS of the upper limb M1 has also shown to improve simple motor tasks of the upper limb such as RT performance (Hummel et al. 2006; Kang and Paik 2011; Leite et al. 2011). This finding has also been supported in individuals with neurological disorders (Fregni et al. 2006; Stagg et al. 2012). In the lower limb, anodal tDCS has shown to improve performance of complex tasks such as skilled visuomotor tracking, muscle strength, and walking. (Tanaka et al. 2009; Madhavan et al. 2011; Tanaka et al. 2011; Sriraman et al. 2014; Park et al. 2015). However, reports on the effects of anodal tDCS of the leg M1 on simple lower limb motor tasks (such as RT) are limited to one study to the best of our knowledge. Tanaka et al. (2009) instructed healthy participants to respond to a simple visual GO signal by pressing a pedal with the left leg as quickly as possible. No significant improvements in lower limb simple RT were reported after anodal tDCS of the leg M1. Given the increasing investigation of tDCS as a motor priming tool for walking, it is important that we try to understand the effects of tDCS on lower limb RT. Fast goal-directed lower limb movement is an important component of locomotion and is necessary for accurate execution of daily activities. Also, understanding the effects on simple motor tasks may enable tDCS to be beneficial to individuals with higher level of impairments who are unable to perform complex functional movements and may benefit from improvements in simple motor tasks.

The primary aim of the present study was to examine the effects of anodal tDCS of the leg M1 on lower limb (ankle) simple RT (SRT) and choice RT (CRT). Despite the many reported benefits of tDCS on motor function, little is known about the mechanisms by which tDCS produces these improvements. We attempted to probe further into these mechanisms by examining if tDCS of the lower limb M1 enables global changes in performance. Specifically, we examined the effects of anodal tDCS of the leg M1 on other cognitive and motor processes that are not directly related to changes in neuromotor networks of the leg M1. Hence, we examined the effects of anodal tDCS of the leg M1 on the upper limb M1 using an upper limb (wrist) simple and choice RT task. Also, we examined the effects of anodal tDCS of the leg M1 on cognitive processing using the symbol digit modality test (SDMT), which is highly dependent on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). We hypothesized that anodal tDCS of the lower limb M1 would improve ankle simple and choice RT, but will not influence wrist RT or SDMT.

Methods

Participants

Fourteen young healthy participants (six males, eight females, age range 20–32 years) with no neurological, musculoskeletal or cardiovascular disorders were recruited to participate in this study. A description of the study was provided, and written consent approved by the institutional review board was obtained from each participant. Before inclusion in the study, all participants were screened for contraindications to noninvasive brain stimulation procedures which included unexplained headaches, pregnancy, metal implants, cardiac pacemakers, history of seizures or epilepsy and medications likely to alter cortical excitability, such as antidepressants. All participants reported to be right leg dominant (preferred leg used to kick a ball).

Experimental design

We used a single blinded, sham-controlled, repeated-measures study design where each individual participated in two experimental sessions (anodal tDCS and sham stimulation) which were counterbalanced to avoid order effects. The two experimental sessions were separated by a period of 7–9 days. At the beginning of each session, participants were administered the symbol digit modality test (SDMT). Next, they were provided instructions on how to perform the RT tasks, and five practice trials for each task were given to ensure that the participant understood the instructions. Then, baseline RTs for the following were collected in a random order: ankle dorsiflexion (DF) SRT, ankle plantarflexion (PF) SRT, wrist SRT, ankle DF CRT, ankle PF CRT, and wrist CRT. Participants were then administered anodal tDCS or sham stimulation for 15 min while they performed a motor tracking task with the ankle. Post-RT measures (in random order) were collected 5 min after the end of stimulation. Post-SDMT was collected after the RT trials, typically within 20–25 min the end of stimulation.

RT task

Subjects were seated comfortably in a chair in front of a computer with the feet on the ground, hip and knee at 90°, and forearm resting comfortably on a table by the side of the chair. All RT trials were performed with the non-dominant foot and non-dominant wrist. Spike 2 software (Cambridge Electronic design, Cambridge, UK) was used to generate the visual stimulus for the RT tasks. The visual stimulus (GO signal) was a red rectangular analog pulse (5 V, 100 ms). The participants were seated in front of a monitor placed at eye level. When the experimenter hit ‘start,’ participants were presented with a moving red flat line which became the rising/falling GO signal. The inter-trial intervals were varied with a mean of 10 s (random sequence, range 5–15 s). The participants were instructed to provide the appropriate response according to the task (described below) as soon as they saw the GO signal. Participants were allowed to practice for five trials for each task (simple RT and choice RT) prior to testing in order to familiarize themselves with the procedure. No further instructions or feedback was provided after the practice trials. All participants quickly understood the task and did not need any further practice or instruction.

Ankle simple RT

Subjects were instructed to respond as quickly as possible to a GO signal presented at random intervals. During the ankle DF RT trials, the GO signal was a rising event and subjects were asked to respond by lifting the forefoot as quickly as possible. For the ankle PF RT trials, the GO signal was a falling event and subjects were asked to respond by lifting the heel as quickly as possible. Fifteen ankle DF SRTs and fifteen PF SRTs were collected.

Ankle choice RT

For the CRT trials, the GO signal was randomly selected by the computer program to be a rising or falling event. Subjects were instructed to provide the appropriate response (dorsiflexion or plantarflexion) that corresponded to the rising or falling GO signal, respectively. Fifteen dorsiflexion CRTs and fifteen plantarflexion CRTs were collected.

Wrist RT

The wrist RT task was similar to the ankle task except that the subjects were instructed to perform wrist extension and wrist flexion in response to the visual stimuli. As post-tDCS effects are time sensitive, only wrist extension simple RTs were performed to reduce the number of trials after stimulation. The choice RT task consisted of wrist extension and flexion, but only the data for wrist extension are reported.

Electromyography

The onset of muscle activity was used to calculate RT. Surface Ag/AgCl electrodes were placed over the muscle belly of the non-dominant tibialis anterior, medial gastrocnemius, and extensor carpi radialis muscles. Before placement of electrodes, the skin was cleaned with alcohol and shaved if necessary. The reference electrode was placed over the non-dominant olecranon process. A Delsys EMG system (Bagnoli 8, MA, USA) was used to sample EMG data at 2000 Hz, with amplification of *1000 and passed through a filter band at 10–500 Hz. Spike2 software was used to collect all EMG data.

Transcranial direct current stimulation

A simple form of constant current stimulator (Chattanooga Ionto, TN, USA) was used to deliver the stimulation. An oblong rectangular saline-soaked sponge electrode (5 cm × 2.5 cm) was placed over the non-dominant leg motor representation. Transcranial magnetic stimulation was used to locate the ‘hot spot’ of the non-dominant tibialis anterior muscle representation. The midpoint of the active electrode was then placed over this location which was on average 1 cm lateral and 1 cm posterior to Cz on the 10–20 international electroencephalogram system. The active electrode was positioned in a direction parallel to the midline (invisible line connecting the nasion and the inion). The reference electrode (7 cm × 5 cm) was placed over the contralateral supraorbital region. During anodal stimulation, 1 mA of current was delivered for 15 min. The current density at the stimulation electrode was 0.08 mA/cm2, and the total charge was 0.072 C/cm2. All participants performed a visuomotor task using ankle dorsiflexion and plantarflexion during anodal/sham tDCS. This motor task has been explained previously (Madhavan and Stinear 2010; Shah et al. 2013; Sriraman et al. 2014). In brief, participants were seated comfortably with their non-dominant foot attached to a custom-made ankle tracker device. They were required to track as accurately as possible a computer-generated sinusoidal waveform with continuous ankle dorsiflexion and plantarflexion movements with their non-dominant ankle for 15 min (which included 1 min of rest after every 4 min of tracking). We chose to deliver tDCS during a motor task than at rest as the effects of tDCS have shown to be task dependent and more effective when performing a skilled motor task (Sriraman et al. 2014). For sham stimulation, the electrodes were placed in the same position as the anodal stimulation and the same ankle visuomotor task was performed. However, the DC stimulator was turned off in 30 s after the initial ramping up of current.

Cognitive function

The SDMT was used to test cognitive function before and after tDCS. The cognitive demands of the SDMT include attention, visual scanning, and motor and psychomotor speed (Smith 1968, 1982). In this paper-and-pencil test, the participant was required to pair digits to assigned symbols using a reference key. The score is the total number of correct written responses in 90 s. Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance. At the start of the test, participants were given a brief practice set to familiarize with the instructions.

Data analysis

For error free trials, RT was calculated as the time interval between the onset of the visual stimulus and onset of activity in the muscle of interest. Onset latencies for the muscle of interest were determined using a semiautomatic computer algorithm that selected the first instant at which the mean EMG activity exceeded a threshold of 2 standard deviations (SD) above the mean background activity, calculated over a 200-ms period just prior to the GO signal. Onsets were first selected by the computer algorithm, and then visually approved and (when necessary) corrected.

Errors in RT were defined as either incorrect movement, or correct movement with unconventional response times (early or delayed) generated by extraneous factors (noted during data collection) or skipped movement, and were not included in the RT analyses. The mean error averaged for 14 participants was <1 (0.6 SD). Because of the small amount of errors, we did not do any further analyses on the error data. The mean RT for each individual for the pre- and post-sessions was calculated. A percentage change in RT was also calculated.

For the SDMT, the number of correct responses (out of 110 total) was calculated as a raw score for the pre- and post-trials.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS software v22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). The data (mean response times) from each task (ankle DF SRT, ankle PF SRT, wrist SRT, ankle DF CRT, ankle PF CRT, and wrist CRT) were analyzed separately. All scores were normally distributed (p > 0.05), as assessed by Shapiro–Wilk’s test of normality on the studentized residuals. Paired t tests were used to compare the baseline RT (pre) between anodal and sham conditions for all RT tasks. To examine the effects of stimulation, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with condition (anodal, sham) and time (pre, post) as the repeated factors was performed for each dependent variable. A Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied when assumptions of sphericity were not met. Post hoc tests using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were performed for significant main or interaction effects. Paired t tests were used to examine the effect of intervention (i.e., anodal stimulation) by comparing the normalized RT (percentage change from pre to post) between anodal and sham conditions. Critical p values were set at 0.05 for all tests. All data are expressed as means + standard error.

Results

No participant reported any adverse events during the experiments. No participant noticed a difference between the real and sham stimulation conditions. A few participants reported initial mild discomfort (verbalized as ‘itching’ beneath the reference electrode) for both the anodal and sham conditions which typically disappeared within a minute of stimulation. The EMG traces showing the RT trials from a representative subject are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

EMG signal from a representative subject showing the shorter RT induced by anodal tDCS. Gray dotted lines represent trials before anodal tDCS (PRE), and solid black lines represent the trials after anodal tDCS (POST). Rectified and smoothed muscle activity of the tibialis anterior muscle during a CRT task is depicted. Mean of three CRT trials is shown. Arrows indicate RT

Baseline RT

No differences in the baseline RT scores between the anodal and sham stimulation conditions were noticed: ankle DF SRT (t 13 = 1.717, p = 0.110), ankle PF SRT (t 13 = −1.112, p = 0.286), wrist extension SRT (t 13 = 0.378, p = 0.711), ankle DF CRT (t 13 = 1.457, p = 0.169), ankle PF CRT (t 13 = 0.94, p = 0.36), and wrist extension CRT (t 13 = 0.578, p = 0.57). Baseline RT (mean ± SEM, average of both conditions) for each task was as follows: ankle DF SRT 314 ± 8 ms, ankle PF SRT 320 ± 9 ms, ankle DF CRT 385 ± 13 ms, ankle PF CRT 416 ± 14 ms, wrist SRT 310 ± 8 ms, and wrist CRT 372 ± 10 ms.

Simple reaction time (SRT)

Ankle DF SRT

Ankle DF SRT revealed a statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and time (F 1, 13 = 6.06, p = 0.029). The mean SRT for the anodal condition was 15 ms (4 %) lower at post compared to pre, a difference that was not statistically significant (p = 0.160). There was a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points for the sham condition (F 1, 13 = 7.98, p = 0.014). Ankle DF SRT was 20 ms (8 %) higher at post than pre for sham. Paired t tests comparing normalized ankle DF SRT between conditions revealed a significant difference (t = −2.32, p = 0.01) between anodal (−4.13 %) and sham (7.5 %) conditions.

Ankle PF SRT

No significant interaction or main effects were noticed (p > 0.05) for ankle PF SRT. No significant difference in normalized ankle PF SRT between conditions was also noticed (p > 0.05).

Wrist SRT

A statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and time (F 1, 13 = 5.91, p = 0.03) was noticed for wrist SRT. Similar to ankle DF SRT, there was a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points only for the sham condition (F 1, 13 = 13.99, p = 0.002). Wrist SRT was 40 ms (13 %) higher at post than pre for sham. Paired t tests comparing normalized wrist SRT revealed a significant difference (t = −2.41, p = 0.01) between anodal (4.4 %) and sham (13.4 %) conditions. Percentage changes in SRT are shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Simple reaction time. Mean percentage changes in simple reaction time (SRT) induced by anodal (solids black bars) and sham (gray bars) tDCS for ankle dorsiflexion (DF), ankle plantarflexion (PF), and wrist extension are shown. Single asterisk indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) between pre- and post-time points within each stimulation condition (post hoc effect). Double asterisk indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) between anodal and sham conditions for normalized RT

Choice reaction time (CRT)

Ankle DF CRT

The ANOVA revealed a statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and time (F 1, 13 = 10.71, p = 0.006) for ankle DF CRT. There was a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points for the anodal condition (F 1, 13 = 4.44, p = 0.05). Ankle DF CRT was 31 ms (7 %) lower at post than pre. There was also a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points for the sham condition (F 1, 13 = 9.162, p = 0.01). Ankle DF CRT was 28 ms (9 %) higher at post than pre. Paired t tests comparing normalized ankle DF CRT between conditions revealed a significant difference (t = 3.12, p = 0.004) between anodal (−7.4 %) and sham (8.7 %) conditions.

Ankle PF CRT

The results for the ankle PF CRT were similar to DF CRT. There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and time (F 1, 13 = 20.979, p = 0.001). Post hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between the pre- and post-time points for the anodal condition, (F 1, 13 = 48.85, p = 0.011). Ankle PF CRT was 53 ms (12 %) lower at post than pre during anodal stimulation. There was also a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points for the sham condition (F 1, 13 = 27.2, p = 0.001). Ankle PF CRT was 45 ms (12 %) higher at post than pre during sham. Paired t tests comparing normalized ankle PF CRT between conditions revealed a significant difference (t = −4.72, p = 0.001) between anodal (−12.1 %) and sham (12.2 %) conditions.

Wrist CRT

There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and time (F 1, 13 = 4.82, p = 0.047) for wrist CRT. Post hoc analyses revealed a statistical difference between the pre- and post-time points for the sham condition (F 1, 13 = 10.54, p = 0.006). Wrist CRT for sham was 30 ms (9 %) higher at post than pre. No significant change between pre and post was noted for the anodal condition. Paired t tests comparing normalized wrist CRT between conditions revealed a significant difference (t = −2.1, p = 0.02) between anodal (−1 %) and sham (9.2 %) conditions. Percentage changes in CRT are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Choice reaction time. Mean percentage changes in CRT induced by anodal (solids black bars) and sham (gray bars) tDCS for ankle dorsiflexion (DF), ankle plantarflexion (PF), and wrist extension are shown. Single asterisk indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) between pre- and post-time points within each stimulation condition (post hoc effect). Double asterisk indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) between anodal and sham conditions for normalized RT

Symbol digit modality test (SDMT)

The two-way ANOVA for SDMT scores revealed no significant effects of interaction of condition and time (F 1, 13 = 0.879, p = 0.366), or main effect of condition (F 1, 13 = 0.089, p = 0.77). But a significant effect of time was seen (F 1, 13 = 42.843, p = 0.000). Both conditions revealed a significant increase (11 %, pre 72.3 ± 4 to post 79.3 ± 4) in SDMT scores after anodal/sham stimulation.

Discussion

In the present study, we examined the effects of anodal tDCS of the lower limb M1 on lower limb RT in healthy young adults. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the influence of anodal tDCS on ankle RT using a comprehensive experimental paradigm that included simple and choice motor RT tasks for the stimulated area of interest, a remote RT task and a cognitive measure, which has not been accounted for in previous lower limb studies. The results of this study suggest that a 15-min application of anodal tDCS to the lower limb M1 during a visuomotor ankle task improves ankle choice reaction time (CRT), as denoted by faster CRT responses after anodal stimulation compared to baseline. We did not observe an effect of anodal stimulation (i.e., change from baseline) for SRT, remote motor (wrist) RT, or cognitive task (SDMT). Interestingly, we noted that the normalized RT was significantly faster for the anodal condition when compared to the sham condition for ankle DF SRT, wrist SRT, and all CRT tasks.

Differential effects of anodal stimulation on ankle SRT versus CRT

One of the important findings of this study was the differential effects of anodal tDCS on ankle simple and choice RT. Based on findings from previous tDCS studies of the upper limb M1, we expected anodal tDCS of the lower limb M1 to improve both simple and choice ankle RT. However, this hypothesis was only partly supported. A RT is a measure of speed and efficiency of central processing, with a CRT reflecting more complex brain processing than a SRT. During RT, three basic process unfold: sensory coding (perceiving the stimulus), decision formation (determining the required action to the stimulus), and motor execution (issuing a motor command and performing the desired action) (Sternberg 1969). A SRT task, where there is only one stimulus and one response, represents the most basic pathway of translating the external input into a speeded motor output, whereas in a CRT task, where different stimuli require different responses, more executive level of processing is required as it involves additional steps of stimulus identification and selection of responses. During CRT tasks, neurons in cortical and subcortical areas have been shown to alter their firing rates during preparatory delay (Hanes and Schall 1996; Kilavik et al. 2014). The mechanisms of action of anodal tDCS have been attributed to short-term depolarization of neuronal membranes and modulation of neuronal firing with resulting long-term changes in intracortical neuronal excitability within the cortex (Stagg and Nitsche 2011). The increased neuronal modulation in the stimulated M1 could have made it more receptive to commands from other brain regions to allow complex tasks to be performed more efficiently. Anodal tDCS may have enabled better integration between different sets of nuclei necessary for the execution of a complex cognitively demanding task, thereby enhancing CRT and not SRT.

A second possible explanation could be attributed to the electrode montage used in the present study. The supplementary motor area (SMA) is situated in front of the lower limb M1 and is important for establishing motor programs, especially in complex task situations following visual cues (Cunnington et al. 2002; Grezes and Decety 2002). The SMA is deeply involved in movement preparation as supported by changes in SMA neuronal involvement before movement execution (Tanji and Mushiake 1996). A recent study showed that anodal tDCS of the SMA selectively interferes with movement preparation without affecting voluntary movement, suggesting that these two components of movement execution have separate pathways (Bolzoni et al. 2015). It is possible that the electrode location or the direction of current flow in the present study may have influenced cortical networks in the SMA, and thus enhancing motor planning and not movement execution, thereby causing a selective improvement in CRT.

Alternatively, it is possible that young healthy subjects display a ceiling effect in SRT, thereby affecting the ability to show improvements in SRT or that our study was underpowered for SRT measurements. It is important to note here that we measured RT as muscle activity onset and not movement onset. It is possible that we may be underestimating the effects of anodal tDCS on SRT as we did not measure its effect on movement execution.

Slowing of response times during sham stimulation

Another important finding of this study was the slower RT during sham stimulation which was not observed during anodal stimulation. We found that the RT was slower (as evidenced by the longer response times) for the sham condition for both the simple and choice RT tasks. In addition, we noticed that the amount of change in RT during the anodal stimulation was significantly different (faster) than sham stimulation. A similar finding of slower reaction times during sham stimulation was observed by Hummel et al. (2006) in their study on stroke survivors. The slower RT could be due to increased peripheral and/or central fatigue accompanying the demanding motor tracking task. Previous studies have shown that short periods of rhythmic contractions performed at submaximal rates are followed by a period of reduced corticomotor excitability and increased intracortical inhibition (Benwell et al. 2006; Teo et al. 2012). It is possible that the fatigue-related effect that was observed during the sham condition may have been negated by facilitation of neuronal activity during anodal stimulation.

An alternative explanation for decrease in RT performance during the sham condition could be attributed to task switching. Subjects asked to perform a task as quickly as possible will perform poorly if they have recently completed a different task than if they performed the same task (Gilbert and Shallice 2002). Neuroimaging studies have reported that functionally identified regions of interest in the brain (motor, prefrontal, parietal, and/or temporal cortices depending on the task performed) are more active during task switching, suggesting that this activity may simply reflect the energy required to prepare neural components for the next action (Rogers et al. 1998; Kimberg et al. 2000). Studies have also reported that task switching could activate a common frontal parietal network, which may interfere with working memory (Sternberg 1969; Dreher and Grafman 2003). In the current study, participants were performing a skilled visuomotor task during the anodal/sham stimulation prior to the post-stimulation RT trials. The sham condition may have decreased performance because the new motor pattern interfered with their awareness and ability to recall and perform the RT task. It is possible that the detrimental effect of task switching was decreased due to the enhanced cortical excitability induced by anodal tDCS that may have given the neural networks the necessary energy to prepare for the new task. This is an important finding requiring further experiments to examine the effects of anodal tDCS on task switching.

Effects on wrist RT

We included wrist RT trials as a control condition to understand the specificity of anodal tDCS. As modeling studies have shown that the effects of tDCS are typically highest over the area directly beneath the electrode with decreasing intensity in the surrounding areas (Miranda et al. 2006, 2009), we anticipated the effects of anodal tDCS to be focal over the leg motor area, which is anatomically distant from the upper limb M1. We did not observe faster wrist CRT responses during anodal stimulation, perhaps partly invalidating our SMA hypothesis mentioned above. If the SMA was solely responsible for changes in ankle CRT, we would expect to observe an improvement in wrist CRT as well. We argue that a concurrent increase in neuronal facilitation of the M1 is necessary to demonstrate improvements in CRT. The absence of change in wrist CRT during the anodal condition supports specificity of anodal stimulation which has been observed in a previous study (Tanaka et al. 2009).

The slowing of responses during the sham condition was also apparent in wrist SRT and CRT tasks. Previous studies have shown that fatigue due to exercising one limb has global consequences affecting unexercised and distant muscles. Halperin et al. (2014) demonstrated that knee extension fatiguing exercise affected force production in the unexercised elbow flexor muscle. Similarly, Aboodarda et al. (2015) reported that elbow flexor fatigue affects knee extensor muscle activity and force. Hence, it is plausible to suppose that fatigue caused by the ankle visuomotor task affected wrist RT in the sham condition.

Absence of change in cognitive function

In the present study, anodal tDCS over the leg M1 did not significantly affect SDMT. The SDMT is a brief test of information processing and attention, sensitive to cognitive impairment in patients with neurological disorders (Benedict et al. 2008; Charvet et al. 2014). We included this cognitive outcome measure to provide further behavioral evidence about the selectivity of tDCS on specific neuronal populations stimulated by the electrode, and to rule out the possibility that the change in RT may be due to changes in homologous distant neuronal networks, due to a general increase in awareness, or due to a placebo effect of stimulation. If the anodal stimulation had a global or placebo effect, we would expect to see corresponding changes in SDMT scores. The shortening of ankle CRT responses in the anodal condition was not accompanied by changes in SDMT scores. Improvements in information processing and executive function, as assessed by SDMT, have been typically correlated with activation of the DLPFC (Rorie and Newsome 2005), suggesting that anodal stimulation of the lower limb M1 did not have an effect on DLPFC in the current study. We were surprised to note that the fatigue and/or task-switching effects causing slowing of motor responses in the sham condition did not impair performance of the SDMT. The relationship between exercise-induced fatigue or mental fatigue and cognitive function is still unclear. One study has indicated that short periods of physical exercise improved cognitive functioning in adults (Hancock and McNaughton 1986); others either did not find any benefits (Cote et al. 1992) or even reported deterioration of cognitive function (Cian et al. 2001). Also, the relationship between tDCS and cognitive function is not yet clear. Pope et al. (2015) showed that tDCS over prefrontal cortex may have differential effects depending on the complexity of the task. In their study, after anodal tDCS they observed an enhancement of a cognitive subtraction task but not of a less challenging addition task (Pope et al. 2015). It is possible that the SDMT is not sensitive or challenging enough to be affected by fatigue. Alternatively, as the SDMT was administered 20 min post-stimulation, the effects of stimulation may have diminished and thus not apparent in SDMT performance. The SDMT has also been shown to have a practice effect in young healthy adults, which was revealed in our study as a main effect of time, and may have diminished any differences between the two stimulation conditions (Pereira et al. 2015). More streamlined experiments are needed to examine the effects of tDCS of motor areas on cognitive and executive function.

Conclusions

In summary, our data showed that anodal tDCS over the leg motor area during an ankle motor tracking task enhances ankle CRT. These effects were not evident for ankle SRT, wrist SRT, wrist CRT, and cognition (SDMT). The effect of anodal stimulation on ankle CRT and not ankle SRT is attributed to an effect of anodal tDCS on complex motor processing and/or its influence on the SMA. Interestingly, we noted that there was a general slowing of RT in the sham condition which was suppressed during anodal stimulation, suggesting perhaps a suppression of fatigue-related processes or task-switching effects due to neuronal facilitation in the anodal condition. These results support the role of tDCS for lower limb motor neurorehabilitation, especially for patients with higher impairments who many benefit from increased performance of simple motor skills. The effects of anodal tDCS on complex motor processing and its possible influence on fatigue and task switching require further exploration to better understand the mechanisms of tDCS.