Introduction

The ability of humans to perceive and remember self-motion as they navigate through a 2D or 3D environment relies upon the integration of multimodal sensorimotor information, including static or dynamic visual cues, proprioception, vestibular cues, and corollary discharge (Mittelstaedt 1983,1999; Berthoz 1991, Glasauer and Mittelstaedt 1998; Vidal and Bülthoff 2009). It is reasonable to assume that these same sensory cues might contribute to the perception of what the physical environment offers in terms of potential motor actions (Sciutti et al. 2012). Addressing the question of how the CNS integrates spatial information from multiple sensory cues is fundamental to understanding the workings of the human brain (Pozzo et al. 1998).

In this study, we examined the role of gravitational information in the perception of self-motion in 3D. We used a paradigm in which a human subject observed visual stimuli corresponding to what they would see as they moved through a curved tunnel. The tunnel could turn in the horizontal plane to the right or to the left, or it could bend upward or downward in the sagittal plane. The task for the subject was to indicate the amplitude of the turn, based on the visual information provided. This task is of interest for the study of the macroscopic properties of sensorimotor integration because it requires a succession of processing: (1) temporal integration of the sensory input (spatial updating) during the transit through the tunnel, (2) working memory related to the perceived angle, and (3) retrieval (recall) of this angular information.

Previously, such a task revealed a significant asymmetry in pitch-induced perception on Earth. Downward stimuli produced a stronger pitch perception than upward, while leftward and rightward yaw turns were perceived equally (Vidal et al. 2006). This up-down asymmetry was also observed when subjects observed a static image of the tunnel (Vidal et al. 2006) and is similar to asymmetrical estimates of the slope of a hillside viewed either from above or from below (Proffitt et al. 1995). To address the question of what reference frames are used to carry out the task, Vidal et al. (2006) recorded responses from human subjects who performed the tasks on Earth in either an upright, seated posture or while lying on their side. In doing so, the investigators decoupled the local, egocentric reference frame defined by the subject’s body from the external, Earth-fixed reference frame defined by gravity and somatosensory cues from the environment. They showed that (1) the up-down pitch asymmetry could arise in either reference frame, (2) asymmetries in OKN responses and the orientation with respect to gravity interacted to determine the response, and (3) the affordances offered by the visual scene could also interact with the sensory cues themselves to determine the perceived angle, depending on the orientation of the body with respect to gravity.

In the experiments reported here, we extended this study by testing the effects of gravity, or lack thereof, on the perception of the angle of the turn. We asked cosmonaut subjects to perform the experiment before, during and after long-duration space flight, and we manipulated vestibular versus haptic cues by having subjects perform the task in weightlessness, either in a rigidly fixed posture with respect to the space station or during free-floating. We addressed two specific questions with these experiments, that is, “Does the lack of gravity or graviceptor input alter the sensory processing underlying the perception of self-motion?” and, if so, “Is it the lack of sensory signal per se that causes the disruption, or are any modifications to the perception brought about by the differing affordances and consequences of actions that occur in the unique conditions of weightlessness?”

Methods

Figure 1 illustrates the experimental task. Subjects looked straight ahead through a form-fitting face mask and a cylindrical barrel frame at the screen of a laptop computer onto which the images of virtual movements were displayed. The screen was centered on the line of gaze at a distance of ~30 cm from the eyes. The barrel had a diameter of 16.5 cm, yielding a circular field of view subtending 30° in all directions. The form-fitting mask, the barrel, and, if necessary, turning off the lights in the room prevented any external visual distractions and removed all external visual references (Cheron et al. 2006).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Simulation of passive self-motion inside a tunnel with a bend in an upward or downward (ac) and leftward or rightward (bd) direction. a, b Response indicator in the form of a schematic outside view of the tube, viewed from the side for pitch (a) and from the top for yaw (b); c, d entry (1), bend (2), and exit (3) images for turn angle of 25° for pitch (c) and for yaw (d)

Subjects observed on the laptop screen a visual flow corresponding to simulation of passive self-motion inside tunnels in the form of a pipe of constant circular cross section, with stone-textured walls. Each tunnel contained an initial linear segment, a single turn in the middle, either in pitch or in yaw (Fig. 1), with one of 6 possible turn angles ranging from 25° to 87.5° in increments of 12.5° and a final linear segment. Subjects were driven passively at constant speed through the virtual 3D tunnel structure. Images were non-stereoscopic but included perspective cues generated by the OpenGL graphics libraries. Using a typical subject height of 1.75 m as a reference, the visual flow corresponded to a virtual speed of 2.21 m/s (around 8 km/h), corresponding to a fast walking speed for humans. (Additional comments on the realism of the virtual motion can be found in Vidal et al. 2003). After exiting each tunnel, subjects were asked to report their perception of the turn’s angular magnitude by adjusting a response indicator depicting an outside view of a tube on the laptop screen that could be bent by manipulating a trackball. The tube was viewed from the side for pitch turns and from the top for yaw turns (Fig. 1a, b). The tube was initially presented at 0° (corresponding to a straight tunnel), and the subjects were instructed to bend the tube to the perceived turn amplitude by rolling the trackball. They pressed a button to indicate when they had reproduced the deviation angle corresponding to the angle perceived during the simulated movement. After a pause of 5 s, subjects could initiate the subsequent trial with the push of a button.

An experimental session consisted of 48 trials, divided into four uninterrupted blocks of 12 trials. A given block included either exclusively pitch turns or exclusively yaw turns. All subjects began with a block of pitch turns, and then alternated, for a block sequence of pitch-yaw-pitch-yaw. Each of the 6 possible amplitudes and two possible directions (leftward and rightward for yaw and upward or downward for pitch) occurred just once and in a random order in each block. At the end of a block, feedback about the subject’s performance was displayed before a short pause. This feedback was the error (in degrees) measured between the real turn angle and the reported response, averaged over all trials in the block. Through this score, subjects were made aware of overall performance but received no information about what specific errors were committed. The experiment was preceded by four practice trials: two trials with pitch turns and two trials with yaw turns. During these trials, subjects learned how to use the computer interface, but received no feedback about performance. The full experiment lasted approximately 50 min for a complete session, which included the instructions and practice trials.

On Earth, subjects performed the experiment while sitting on a chair of adjustable height facing the computer/barrel/mask that was placed on a table. During space flight, cosmonauts performed the experiment in two conditions. In the attached condition, cosmonauts used belts, foot straps, and a tabletop to maintain a sitting like posture in front of the laptop, like the one used on Earth. In the free-floating condition, subjects held the experimental apparatus (laptop computer and tunnel) in their hands with an elastic band holding the mask against the face. An assisting cosmonaut then positioned the subject in the center of the free working volume within one of the space station modules. The subject was released, and both subject and apparatus floated free from any contact with the station. The assisting cosmonaut ensured that no contact with the walls of the station occurred. To accomplish this, the assistant applied short tugs on the clothing of the subject to adjust the position trying to avoid giving strong directional cues. Very few such corrections (1–2 per session per subject) were required.

Figure 2 shows the testing schedule for the cosmonauts. Each cosmonaut was tested on 5 successive periods. Prior to flight, cosmonauts were tested on Earth in 2 pairs of sessions over the 2 months preceding liftoff (BF1, BF2). The two sessions within each period were separated by at least 1 day. These subjects were then tested on 2 days over the course of their spaceflight aboard the ISS, with at least 1 day between sessions. Additionally, subjects performed the experiment twice on each day of testing on orbit, once in each of the two different experimental conditions (“attached” (A) and “free-floating” (FF)). The order of passage for the two postural conditions was counterbalanced across subjects. After their return from ISS, cosmonauts were tested on Earth again, on two different days during the week immediately following the landing (PF1) and two more times 1–3 weeks later (PF2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Timeline of testing performed by each cosmonaut. The experiment was performed at different periods: 2 before, 1 during and 2 after flight aboard the ISS. Within each period, subjects were tested on two separate days, with at least 1 day in between. On each test day, subjects performed a total of 48 trials separated into 4 blocks of 12 trials each. Within each block, trials were all in pitch (up or down) or in yaw (left or right), and the order of trials was the same for each session (pitch-yaw-pitch-yaw). On each test day on board the ISS, subjects performed two sessions, one in an attached posture and one in free-floating. The order of these sessions on board the ISS was counterbalanced across subjects, with 3 performing attached first and 4 performing free-floating first

Seven male cosmonauts (C1–C7) participated in this investigation. The mean age (±SD) of the cosmonauts was 42 ± 3 years. Six cosmonauts had previous experience in space flight; one cosmonaut (C2) had no such experience. All cosmonauts were in excellent health, as regularly determined by a special space flight medical commission during all periods of the investigation. Six out of 7 cosmonauts performed the two sessions within the first week of arriving on orbit (not before flight day 2 and not after flight day 7), and the seventh performed the experiment on flight days 16 and 18. The duration of exposure to weightlessness varied between subjects; four of whom spent 10 days on orbit (Russian–Belgian (ODISSEA) and Russian–Spanish (CERVANTES) “taxi” missions), while the other 3 cosmonauts spent 6 months aboard the ISS (Increments 9, 10, and 11).

Ten naive subjects (6 men and 4 women), 34 ± 8 years also participated in this experiment; most were students or laboratory staff, and all were right handed. These control subjects performed experimental sessions on the ground following the same schedule as cosmonauts. All participants gave prior written consent before starting this investigation.

Data analysis

The perceived turn angle reported by subjects, and the response latencies (corresponding to the time that elapsed between the initial presentation of the response indicator and the moment that the subjects pressed the button to record the response) were recorded for each trial (48 trials/session), distributed over 4 blocks of 12 trials each. We analyzed primarily the relative angular error as the difference between the real angle and the subject’s response divided by the real angle (see Vidal et al. 2006), signed according to the following convention: positive if the reproduced angle was overestimated (overshot) and negative if it was underestimated (undershot).

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was used to test for statistical significance of the observed response errors. First, we applied separate ANOVA to pitch and yaw data for control subjects and for cosmonauts from their first session on the ground, with direction (up vs. down for pitch, left vs. right for yaw) and turn magnitude (25°, 37.5°, 50°, 62.5°, 75°, or 87.5°) as within-subject factors. Then, based on an observed main effect of turn direction for pitch, but not for yaw turns (see Results), and on previous studies (Vidal et al. 2006), we conducted specific planned comparisons on the computed “up/down asymmetry index” as the average error for pitch downward minus the average error for pitch upward stimuli, as a function of the real turn magnitude and as a function of the experimental session for each subject. Results from pre-flight tests on the ground showed that the up/down asymmetry was limited to the smallest stimulus angles (25°, 37.5°, 50°). We therefore defined the “small-angle up/down asymmetry” as the up/down asymmetry index averaged across these three angles. We tested whether any changes in perception could be directly attributed to the lack of gravity by comparing the small-angle up/down asymmetry for either free-floating or attached with the ground baselines. We also tested for a direct influence of gravitational cues versus an indirect influence based on haptic cues by comparing the small-angle up/down asymmetry for the attached versus free-floating posture on orbit.

Results

Figure 3 illustrates the relative angular error observed for the different real turn magnitudes in control subjects and cosmonauts. Control subjects who performed the experiment on the ground showed a marked asymmetry in the relative angular error for pitch (downward vs. upward) that was strongest for small angles and decreased progressively as the magnitude of the turn increased (Fig. 3a). A two-factor ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of turn direction (F 1,9 = 47.2, p < 0.001) and of turn magnitude (F 5,45 = 4.03, p < 0.01) and a significant cross effect between these two factors (F 5,45 = 17.8, p < 0.0001). Scheffé’s post hoc analysis showed that the difference between upward and downward response errors was significant (p < 0.01) for the smaller magnitudes (25°, 37.5°, 50°), but not for the larger magnitudes (p > 0.20). We assessed the stability of these perceptual measures across repeated sessions by computing a two-way ANOVA on the up/down asymmetry index from the control subjects, with turn magnitude (six levels) and experiment period (five levels) as within-subject factors. We found a significant main effect of turn magnitude (F 5, 40 = 13.631, p < 0.001), but no main effect of experiment period and no cross effect. In other words, the pattern of up/down asymmetry was stable over time for control subjects on the ground. The same control subjects on the ground showed no apparent differences between leftward and rightward bends (Fig. 3d), with no significant main effects or interactions between the factors direction (left or right) and magnitude on relative angular error.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The relative angular errors of perceived pitch (ac) and yaw (df) turns for control subjects (a, d), cosmonauts on Earth before flight (b, e), and cosmonauts free-floating in the ISS (c, f). Gray line, filled circle: upward/leftward, Black line, open circle: downward/rightward. Values are mean ± SE. Stars indicate statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences between upward and downward as indicated by post hoc tests

To test for effects of gravity on the perception of self-motion in our virtual task, we analyzed the responses of cosmonaut subjects before, during and after their stay in the International Space Station. As a control, we first assessed whether the conditions of spaceflight altered the subjects’ perception of the visual stimuli in a generic fashion, independent of a direct or indirect influence of gravity on the up-down asymmetries observed on Earth. We considered the perception of horizontal turns as baseline and compared the performance on these stimuli on Earth before the flight to weightlessness. As was the case for control subjects on the ground, a two-way ANOVA revealed no significant effect of turn direction (left vs. right) or of turn magnitude (25°, 37.5°, 50°, 62.5°, 75° or 87.5°) for tests performed on the ground prior to the flight (Fig. 3e) while free-floating in weightlessness (Fig. 3f) or while in the attached condition in weightlessness (not shown). A one-way ANOVA comparison across these conditions (before flight, free-floating, attached) for leftward and rightward turns combined; showed no significant effect of these experimental conditions on relative angular error (F 2, 12 = 0.1024, p = 0.90) even though response latencies were somewhat shorter, on average, for the two weightless conditions, compared to latencies of trials performed on the ground before flight (F 2, 12 = 4.73, p = 0.0305). We can, therefore, conclude that cosmonaut’s performance on the perceptual task was not affected by a change in alertness or any other uncontrolled factor during the sessions performed in weightlessness.

We then looked for a specific effect of gravity on perceptual responses to pitch stimuli. Although somewhat less pronounced than for the control subjects, the cosmonauts manifested an up/down asymmetry in their responses on the ground prior to flight (Fig. 3b). A two-way ANOVA applied to the cosmonaut’s responses to pitch stimuli on the ground showed a main effect of turn direction across all six turn magnitudes that was almost significant (F 1, 6 = 5.4680, p = 0.05797), and a planned comparison—justified by our previous observations on control subjects—showed that cosmonauts manifested a small-angle up/down asymmetry index significantly different from zero (p < 0.002). While free-floating, the cosmonauts showed a qualitatively smaller difference between upward and downward turns (Fig. 3c). There was no main effect of turn direction (F 1, 6 = 0.72, p = 0.4267) nor any cross effect (F 5, 30 = 1.51, p = 0.2137) in a two-way ANOVA (direction × magnitude) applied to pitch data, nor did the small-angle up/down asymmetry index differ significantly from 0 (p = 0.14). Interestingly, the suppression of the up/down asymmetry arose because the relative angular error increased for upward turns in weightlessness compared to ground, not because the error decreased for downward pitch.

To provide further statistical support for this observation, we looked for significant changes in the asymmetry index across experimental sessions performed on ground and in weightlessness for the cosmonauts. To look for an immediate effect of gravity on perceptual responses, before any adaptation of task performance to the novel environment, we took the first block of trials performed by each subject in each of the weightless conditions. We compared these data to the first block of trials performed in each of the periods performed before and after flight, so that we compared trials performed with a similar level of practice within each period. We considered trials performed in each period on the ground and the attached and free-floating postures on orbit as separate conditions, resulting in two-factor ANOVA with six levels for the factor “condition” (BF1, BF2, A, FF, PF1, PF2) and six levels for the factor “turn magnitude.” With this analysis, we found the usual main effect of turn magnitude (F 5, 30 = 3.2028, p = 0.01959) on the up/down asymmetry index, but we also found a significant main effect of experiment condition (F 5, 30` = 2.7670, p = 0.0359) and no cross effect.

The main effect of experimental conditions indicates that the lack of gravity could have had an effect on the perceptual asymmetry for pitch. This was confirmed by planned comparisons on the small-angle up/down asymmetry. Figure 4a shows the corresponding small-angle up/down asymmetry index before, during and after the space mission, for the first block of trials performed in each condition. For most conditions, the asymmetry index was significantly different from zero for the small angles. The one exception is the trials performed in the free-floating condition on orbit. The small-angle asymmetry index for free-floating (mean index = −1.76) was not significantly different from zero (p = 0.81) and was significantly reduced (t test, p < 0.025) compared to its pre-flight value (mean index = 19.68). Interestingly, in the attached condition, the perceptual asymmetry for pitch was preserved in weightlessness: the small-angle up/down asymmetry index in this condition (mean index = 26.96) was significantly different from zero (p = 0.011) and did not differ from values before or after the flight. We also found that the small-angle up/down asymmetry differed significantly between the attached and free-floating conditions in weightlessness (p < 0.001). Note that similar statistical results for all these comparisons were obtained whether we considered data from just the first block of trials performed during the first day of testing in each condition, or whether we considered all trials (both blocks) performed on the first day.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Small-angle up/down asymmetry index (downward minus upward for turn angles 25°, 37.5°, and 50°, combined) for the first block (a) and for the last block (b) of each experimental session. Values are averaged across 7 cosmonauts (mean ± SE). Stars indicate statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences from zero (filled stars) or between conditions (empty stars)

To test whether practice had any influence on changes in perceptual asymmetries either on the ground or in weightlessness, we then compared the first and last blocks of trials performed within each testing period. Figure 4b shows the small-angle up/down asymmetry for the second block of trials performed on the second day of testing within each period, that is, the most-practiced trials performed by each subject within each testing period and gravitational conditions. On the ground, practice had little or no effect on the up/down asymmetry, either before or after flight, that is, we found no statistical differences in the small-angle up/down asymmetry between the first block and last block of trials performed in each pre-flight and post-flight period. If anything, the average asymmetry index increased from the first to the second day of testing in each of these sessions, although this increase was not statistically significant. Similarly, the small-angle up/down asymmetry index did not change between the first and last block of trials in the free-floating condition on orbit; in neither case was the small-angle up/down asymmetry index different from zero. Conversely, the asymmetry diminished with repetition for the attached condition in weightlessness. The small-angle up/down asymmetry index decreased significantly between the first (index = 29.6) and last (index = 6.0) block of trials performed in the attached condition in weightlessness (p = 0.013), such that it was no longer significantly different from zero (p = 0.45) or from the free-floating condition (p = 0.83) at the end of the second day of testing. Similar results were obtained when all trials on the second day of testing were compared to all trials from the first. Thus, whereas the perceptual asymmetry for pitch was immediately diminished in free-floating and remained low throughout the in-flight period, in the attached condition, the asymmetry disappeared only after a certain amount of time or practice in weightlessness (see “Discussion”).

Finally, we note that in post-flight testing, the small-angle up/down asymmetry index was again statistically different from zero, both for the first and for the last block of trials performed in each test period (or for the first and last days combined), indicating the up/down asymmetry was restored on return to ground.

Discussion

In these experiments, we studied how gravitational information influences the perception of scene geometry and the perception of self-motion based on visual cues. Three main results emerged from these psychophysical analyses. (1) The strong asymmetry between downward and upward pitch turns observed on Earth showed an immediate and significant reduction once in weightlessness in the free-floating condition but not in the attached condition. (2) On the second test day of orbital flight, the perceptual asymmetry was also suppressed in the attached condition. (3) These effects do not stem from a change in alertness or any other uncontrolled factor during the flight sessions, as shown by the similar performance observed for yaw turns on ground and in flight. Taken together, these findings provide evidence for two distinct mechanisms by which gravity (or the absence of) acts upon the perceptual asymmetry of pitch turns, depending on whether weightlessness is experienced within a stable somatosensory reference frame or not.

Global performance is conserved in weightlessness

The last of these results is in accordance with previous data (Vidal et al. 2003) showing that the accuracy to recreate an external image of the remembered 3D shape of the tunnel was not perturbed in weightlessness. Similarly, the ability to reproduce the orientation of a visual line was preserved during the same space missions (Lipshits et al. 2005). These results may at first sight seem surprising in the context of visual sensory deficits reported in early Soviet spacecraft mission (decrement in the ability to estimate the direction of line patterns, colors perception, and contrast sensitivity (Popov and Boyko 1967), alterations to the gaze holding system (Kornilova et al. 1983; Clément et al. 1993), and modifications to receptor physiology during spaceflight (Fuglesang et al. 2006). Difficulties in reading checklists onboard the Shuttle have been reported and analyzed (NASA’s EDOMP; Clément and Reschke 2008), but except for contrast sensitivity, no statistically significant modifications have been found for other parameters such as phoria, eye dominance, flicker fusion frequency, and stereopsis. It should be noted, therefore, that our experiments were performed both on Earth and in the ISS in a fully controlled visual field, protecting the subject from changes in visual environment (solar illumination and absence of atmospheric scattering of light). In these controlled conditions, visual perception per se does not appear to be substantially modified in weightlessness.

If not retinal physiology, then what?

It appears, then, that the processing of visual information originating in the retina is not the cause of either the perceptual asymmetry observed on Earth or its disappearance, given that neither anisotropy in the perception of visual orientation (Lipshits et al. 2005) nor basic physiological function of the retina (Task and Genco 1987) appear to be modified in weightlessness. What mechanisms might, therefore, underlie our observations that tilt perception is modified in space? As in the previous ground studies, both low-level reflex processes and high-level cognitive function may come into play.

Eye movements

In our paradigm, the optokinetic system plays a central role and may partly explain the vertical anisotropy in favor of the downward pitch. Indeed, in experiments performed on the ground, suppressing eye movements reduced, but did not eliminate, the perceptual asymmetry between upward and downward turns (Vidal et al. 2006). On Earth, the gain of the optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) with slow phases directed upward (corresponding to downward pitch turns in our task) is larger than those corresponding to the OKN with slow phases directed downward and may thus contribute to the asymmetrical perception on Earth (Clément and Reschke 2008). It is also known that otolith stimulation greatly influences the vertical OKN (Igarashi et al. 1987; Clément and Lathan 1991; Gizzi et al. 1994). Interestingly, it was demonstrated that the vertical OKN asymmetry recorded in terrestrial conditions was reversed in weightlessness during the early period of the flight and showed a trend toward symmetry after 2 weeks in weightlessness (Clément et al. 1986, 1993; Clément 2003). This vertical OKN reversal might explain why upward pitch turns became overestimated as much as downward pitch turns, leading to the suppression of the up/down asymmetry in the free-floating condition.

In this context, the eye velocity of the OKN slow phase may be considered as an indicator of the effectiveness of image motion on the retina not only on the motor action of the eye but also on the final perception. The otolith inputs play a regulating role in the vertical asymmetry by exerting an upward drive on eye movement (Clément and Reschke 2008). It was also reported by the inspection of the position of the nystagmus beating field that the eye position of gaze was displaced upward during the early period of flight (Clément 2003), indicating a possible otolith-dependent change in eye position. This position signal is elaborated by a complex network of recurrent connections (the oculomotor neural integrator) performing the mathematical integration of the different velocity signals coming from vestibular, optokinetic, and pursuit system (Robinson 1989, Major et al. 2004). The vestibular–prepositus complex (Cheron et al. 1986a, b; Cheron and Godaux 1987) and the interstitial nucleus of Cajal (Crawford and Vilis 1993) play a crucial role in the gaze holding system. In addition, the OKN response is highly sensitive to any type of disturbance of the neural integrator (Cheron et al. 1986b).

A change in the preferred vertical direction of gaze may also explain the reversal in vertical vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) (Clarke et al. 2000) and in vertical OKN (Clément et al. 1986, 1993; Clément 2003) in weightlessness. Indeed, judgements of slope based on the observation of static images also evoke up/down asymmetries of perception (Proffitt et al. 1995; Vidal et al. 2006), although not as strong as in the presence of OKN-inducing visual flow. In fact, static gaze and OKN are linked, and so it is perhaps not surprising to see both correlated with the perceptual asymmetry. In accordance with Alexander’s Law, the eye velocity of the slow phase increases when the gaze is displaced in the direction of the beating phase of the nystagmus and decreases with gaze in the opposite direction (Lackner and DiZio 2000). An upward displacement of the OKN beating field in weightlessness would produce both an increase in the downward slow phase velocity and a decrease in the upward slow phase velocity, which would invert the original up-down asymmetry observed in terrestrial condition. Another neural network devoted to the velocity storage mechanism and related to the optokinetic after nystagmus (OKAN) is assumed by the commissural fibers linked to the vestibular–prepositus complex of both sides (Godaux and Cheron 1991). This vestibular network is also known to be influenced by gravity (Dai et al. 1991, 1994) and may influence the perception of up and down.

Modification to the control of eye position and movement in weightlessness provides an attractive hypothesis to explain our experimental observations in weightlessness. The comparison of the attached and free-floating conditions on orbit tell us that this is not, however, the whole story. Vestibular inputs to the CNS were essentially the same in these two conditions. Otoliths are unloaded, whatever the orientation of the head in the local environment, and any rotations or linear accelerations of the head in the free-floating condition were of very low amplitude, below the threshold of the vestibular organs. Yet, the asymmetry differed significantly between the attached and free-floating postures on the first day of testing in weightlessness. Vestibular drive to eye movement circuitry alone cannot, therefore, fully explain the differences in visual perception between ground and weightlessness.

Multisensory cognitive context

We argue instead for a top-down effect of cognitive context, based on the fusion of multisensory cues as the basis for our observations (cf. Vidal et al. 2006). Humans are thought to maintain an internal representation of the subjective vertical (Mittelstaedt 1983, 1999; Berthoz 1991) that results from a convergence of otolithic cues, somatosensory cues (haptic, proprioception), and visual cues about the environment’s vertical (trees, walls…). Due to the multisensory nature of this mechanism on the one hand, and to the ambiguity of otolith and other gravito-inertial signals during motion on the other, this vertical reference frame is not strictly coupled to gravity and may be perturbed during spaceflight (Young et al. 1984; Parker et al. 1985). Therefore, one can imagine that in the absence of gravity, this subjective vertical is built upon the remaining cues, to the point that a clear vertical feeling may remain. Indeed, numerous studies have shown that perceptual anisotropies (Appelle 1972; Lipshits and McIntyre 1999; Luyat and Gentaz 2002) rely on a multisensory vertical, rather than on a strict gravitational reference frame.

In our experiments, subjects could rely on visual memory even though external visual cues were excluded from view. When a participant gets ready to perform these experiments, he or she sees the walls and the entire surrounding visual scene. If the subject is aware of the fact that body orientation will not change with respect to this initial view, as on the ground or in the attached position on orbit, the remembered visual scene can serve as a stable reference frame for orientation estimation. In the free-floating condition, subjects could not, however, be assured that the body axis would remain aligned with respect to the remembered visual field. The two different conditions used on orbit (attached and free-floating) also differed by the fact that when in a secured seated position subjects could rely on tactile receptors and the stable attachment of the body to the floor of the space station to perceive their body orientation, and hence the orientation of the tunnel’s bend, with respect to the stable platform of the ISS. Together with the remembered visual cues, this additional somatosensory information could be enough to maintain a strong sense of verticality, preserving in the attached condition of the perceptual asymmetry. Conversely, subjects in the free-floating condition had no contact with the stable reference frame provided by the station. In this situation, the subject will presumably adopt the body axis as the local vertical and will process pitch and yaw turns accordingly in this local reference frame. Nevertheless, tactile or haptic cues may be critical for the interpretation of ambiguous otolithic signals, even on Earth (Bortolami et al. 2006). Thus, in free-floating, up and down are ambiguous, leading to symmetric overshoot of perceived tunnel angle for pitch up and pitch down tunnels. Being locally “horizontal,” one would still expect no overshoot and no asymmetry for left or right yaw with respect to the body axis. A similar, multisensory argument has been applied to explain illusions of being inverted in weightlessness (Simons and Gardner 1963; Graybiel and Kellogg 1967; Lackner 1992), anticipation of the effects of gravity on moving objects even when gravity is not in play (McIntyre et al. 2001; Miller et al. 2008), up/down asymmetries in the perception and interception of moving objects (Senot et al. 2012; Moscatelli and Lacquaniti 2011; Zago et al. 2009; Le Seac’h et al. 2010), and differential tuning of kinematics for upward and downward arm movements (Le Seac’h and McIntyre 2007). Tactile stimulation has even been proposed as a means to combat sensations of spatial disorientation (Van Erp et al. 2006).

It is fascinating to note, therefore, that the asymmetry effect disappeared even in the attached posture at some point between the first and second time the experiment was performed on orbit. Note that we initially analyzed the first and last block of trials performed in weightlessness, corresponding to the trials performed with the least and the most amount of practice in each condition, respectively. We reasoned that subjects might quickly adapt responses to weightless conditions with practice. Nevertheless, the results were the same when we considered together all trials from the first day of testing in weightlessness; that is, the asymmetry index was reduced in weightlessness on average in the free-floating condition, but not in the attached condition, and this despite the fact that half of the subjects performed the attached condition first, and the other half performed the free-floating condition first.

One might surmise, therefore, that modifications to the perception of turn angle required a longer exposure to weightlessness when in the attached posture, since responses in the two conditions differed mainly depending on the test day. But we could not find evidence that the exposure time to weightlessness, by itself, was the critical factor; indeed, the one subject who performed the tests relatively late during the flight still showed a large asymmetry for his first testing session even after spending 14 days on orbit. Instead, it appears that the asymmetry disappeared after a certain amount of practice on the task in the attached condition in weightlessness. One possible mechanism for this effect might stem from the high correlation between haptic and vestibular cues that usually occurs in normal gravity. According to recent theories, this correlation would be imbedded in multisensory integration networks, such that the CNS essentially replaces or reconstructs missing gravitational cues with redundant information from other sensory signals (Droulez and Darlot 1989; Pouget et al. 2002; Tagliabue and McIntyre 2011). These associations between sensory signals from different modalities are presumably malleable, such that with repeated practice in the absence of up/down vestibular cues, the haptic cues lose significance, eventually resulting in similar responses between the attached and free-floating conditions overtime. We cannot say, however, that the only adaptation is for the haptic cues in the attached condition. Although still not significantly different from zero, measurements of asymmetry in free-floating on the second day of testing (Fig. 4b) showed more inter-subject variability, the average was somewhat higher than on the first day (though not significantly), and the asymmetry was no longer significantly different from pre-flight values. This suggests that some subjects may have adapted toward a return to asymmetric perception of pitch turns even during free-floating, perhaps based on a local reference aligned with the body (Miller et al. 2008; Dyde et al. 2006; Le Seac’h et al. 2010). Note that the asymmetry returned immediately on return to Earth, showing that clear, coherent graviceptor cues are sufficient to reinstate the perceptual asymmetry. Still, it is intriguing to observe that our group of cosmonauts, 6 of whom had previous experience in orbital space flight, manifested a lower level of asymmetry than our control subjects on the ground. The effects of long-term exposure to weightlessness might nevertheless have a permanent effect on this particular percept.

Top-down influences and affordances

While the multisensory interpretation may explain how the up/down perceptual asymmetry is modulated by the presence or absence of gravity, it does not provide a direct explanation as to why this asymmetry should exist in the first place. The answer may lie in the analysis of sensory information in the context of what potential actions are afforded by the visual scene. On Earth, humans have developed the capacity to stabilize their posture in the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions in order to overcome gravity constraints and prevent falling (Redfern et al. 2001; Yu et al. 2010). For an upright stance and bipedal locomotion, the risk of falling on any given slope is greater when moving down versus moving upward. The tendency for human subjects to overestimate downward slopes is, therefore, likely to be a manifestation of this perceived danger. The fact that the overestimation increases for dynamic stimuli, implying that the subject is in motion, is coherent with this interpretation. But “falling” only has meaning in a normal gravitational environment for which there is a stable, clearly defined up and down. It is, therefore, possible that subjects confounded “up” and “down” with respect to a body-centered vertical axis (see above) or perceived an equal danger for upward and downward slopes, in the free-floating condition, leading to the symmetric overestimation of pitch angle that we observed in both directions (Fig. 3c). In the attached condition, subjects could substitute stable haptic cues for actual graviceptor information and perceived downward versus upward slope accordingly, at least initially. Yaw rotations, however, are benign in terms of postural stability in a normal gravitational field and thus should be immune to this perceptual distortion when the body axis is aligned with the vertical. Indeed, we saw no asymmetry for yaw stimuli perceived either on the ground (Fig. 3d, e) or in free-floating (Fig. 3f), supporting the notion that in the absence of gravitational or haptic cues, subjects adopt the body axis as the local vertical reference in weightlessness (Le Seac’h et al. 2010). Yaw stimuli did, however, elicit an up/down asymmetry both with respect to gravity and with respect to the body axis when subject was lying on their sides (Vidal et al. 2006). Explaining the asymmetry in terms of affordances in local versus external reference frames argues, therefore, for top-down modulation of the low-level sensory signals that lead to the perception of slope.

Neural pathways, self-motion perception and weightlessness

The distinction between low-level circuits and high-level cognitive function is, in reality, probably not so clear-cut as theoretical discussions of brain function might imply. A more compelling question is that of where the required multisensory processing is carried out. During self-motion, the retina is specifically activated by the optic flow (Angelaki and Hess 2005), and these inputs related to the directional velocity of the image on the retina are conducted via the nuclei of the optic tract (NOT) and the nuclei reticular tegmentum pontis (NRTP) up through the vestibular complex and the cerebellum and then forwarded to the vestibular cortical network where the final perception is probably elaborated (Kahane et al. 2003; Indovina et al. 2005; Maffei et al. 2010). There are thus many loci likely to be affected by weightlessness.

The convergence in the vestibular nuclei of the inputs related to head movement and those related to the moving image on the retina may be considered as a first reasonable site influenced by weightlessness. On Earth, if the head is fixed in space, the output from this neural site is able to produce the sensation of a downward pitch only on the basis of upward optical flux. But the absence of head movement does not mean that the information from the vestibular afferents is not implicated in the downward pitch sensation. When the head is horizontal, the resting discharge pattern of the otolith organs of the inner ear provides a static reference; deviations from that baseline are integrated in the convergent vestibular network, giving rise to perception of body movement. In weightlessness, the otolith organs are unloaded, and the static otolith-spinal and otolith-oculomotor influences are missing. The loss of these anchoring cues might thus explain the alteration in the perception of upward and downward pitch.

Conclusions

With this experiment, we demonstrate that the asymmetrical perception of upward and downward pitch is suppressed in weightlessness. However, this perceptual anisotropy is initially preserved in the “attached” condition and disappeared only after practice in weightlessness. The reported effects of weightlessness on the early processing stages (vestibular and optokinetics) that underlie the perception of self-motion may, therefore, be partially overcome by haptic cues, reflecting the fusion of multisensory cues and top-down influences on visual perception, based on the affordances of the local environment.