Introduction

Many neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have examined brain activation related to movement frequency (VanMeter et al. 1995; Sadato et al. 1996a; Jenkins et al. 1997; Jäncke et al. 1998; Deiber et al. 1999; Riecker et al. 2003) and to movement velocity (Winstein et al. 1997; Turner et al. 1998, 2003; Desmurget et al. 2004). The results of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) studies obtained by tracking wrist movement showed that increasing activation in the supplementary motor area (SMA), the primary motor cortex (M1), the premotor cortex (PMC), the thalamus and the cerebellum (CB) was related to movement frequency (VanMeter et al. 1995). Turner et al. (2003) indicated that rCBF in the basal ganglia, the CB, and the sensory motor cortex (SMC) are directly related to movement velocity and movement extent in the pursuit tracking task under different control-display gains. With regard to repetitive and sequential finger movement rates, Sadato et al. (1996a, b) demonstrated that there was a rapid rise in rCBF between the slow and the fast movement rates, but no further increase at the very fast rates. In addition, changes in the left SMC and CB likely reflect movement rate (Blinkenberg et al. 1996). Furthermore, a positive-linear relationship between movement rate and fMRI signal change was also observed (Rao et al. 1993, 1996; Schlaug et al. 1996). Based on these findings, there is general agreement that both the SMA and the PMC appear to have an important role in the generation of sequences from memory that fit into a precise timing plan. However, no conclusive data exist on the interactions between movement rate and error correction.

In performing actual movements, predictive functions play an important role in automated on-line movement control, whereas there is general consensus that forward internal models are a fundamental principle in predictive motor control (Kawato 1999; Doya 1999, 2000). In addition, there is evidence that internal models are represented in the CB (Imamizu et al. 2000, 2003). Recently, Boecker et al. (2005) demonstrated the existence of modular representations for predictive force coupling, with the ipsilateral CB playing a major role. However, no conclusive data exist on automated procedures involving movement rate and error correction. In particular, it is not well understood how the CB handles a behavioral situation when the specific internal models are induced by changes in movement rate and in movement error correction.

Hence, the present study was undertaken to address this issue and to investigate the central representation of internal models for automated force production by different movement rates. That is, how the CB contributes to automated error correction accompanied by different movement rates.

Methods

Subjects

Seven right-handed subjects (all Japanese males, 22–37 years of age), who surly used the right hand for wiring and manipulating a tool in their daily activities (Oldfield 1971) and did not suffer from any known neuromuscular disorders, participated in the present study after giving written informed consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board. The ethical committee of the Hiroshima Prefectural Rehabilitation Center approved the experimental procedure for fMRI acquisition in this research study.

Experimental system

Figure 1a shows a schematic representation of the developed manual tracking system for evaluating error correction in the fMRI environment. This system was composed of an fMRI-compatible optical force sensor to measure the force exerted by right index finger abduction (Fig. 1b). The feedback display presented force errors, e, between the reference signal, f r , and the measured force signal, f, with the position of a white circle on the black screen window, and the circle moved up and down in real time (Fig. 1a). If the subject could completely follow the reference signal, the circle was always located at the desired point on the display. The system recorded the force produced by a subject with 1 kHz sampling frequency, and subjects did not notice a time delay in manipulating the force sensor to perform the force production task.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental setup. a Experimental apparatus under an fMRI environment. b Optical force sensor for measuring the force applied by an index finger

The force error correction task by using the right index finger was carried out for a sine-wave-like force change with amplitude 1.0 N on the base line of 2.0 N. All subjects laid face up and used their right index fingers to touch the specified part of the optical force sensor, which was tightly fixed to a wooden plate (Fig. 1b). They were instructed to follow the reference signal by positioning the circle at the desired point (no error level) by using the visual feedback information.

In the fMRI experiments, all magnetic materials, such as the computer unit, were installed outside the scan room as shown in Fig. 1a (see also Maruishi et al. 2004). The optical fibers of the force sensor were connected to the computer unit through a waveguide in the scan room. The visual feedback information was projected onto the fMRI-compatible back-projection screen through the waveguide by a projector outside the scan room.

Experimental design

The influence of the frequency of a reference signal on motor performance and brain activation was examined by using the developed experimental system in the error correction task. In the present study, force production movement signal frequencies were 0.4 and 0.8 Hz. It is known that there is a rapid rise in rCBF between the slow and the fast movement rates, whereas no further increase of the very fast movement rates (Sadato et al. 1996a). In addition, there is a limitation in human ability to correcting errors under the conditions of tracking-like movement. Participants practiced for at least a 5 min to familiarize themselves with the two different frequencies.

In the fMRI experiment, the subject closed his eyes and displayed no motor activity for 1 min in the “REST” condition, and then performed the error correction task according to the visual feedback information projected on the screen for 1 min in the “MOTION” condition. Each scan session lasted 5 min and proceeded along the following designed block paradigm: REST—MOTION—REST—MOTION—REST. The start and end commands for MOTION were delivered to the subject by using an fMRI-compatible headphone.

fMRI acquisition

The fMRI experiment was conducted on a 1.5-T SYMPHONY system with a head-coil (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). We collected T2*-weighted transverse echo-planar images (EPI) (FOV 192 mm × 192 mm, matrix size 64 × 64, in-plane resolution 3 mm × 3 mm, flip angle 90°, TE 60 ms) with blood oxygenation level-dependent contrast. Twenty-four contiguous axial slices of 6-mm thickness were collected in each volume by using an interleaf method. An automatic shimming procedure was performed before each scan session. In each session, 51 functional volumes were collected within a single scanning session with an effective repetition time of 6.0 s/vol. The first volumes obtained in the sessions were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects (see Maruishi et al. 2004).

Behavioral data analysis

Motor performance in each session was quantitatively evaluated by using the total sum of force errors during two MOTION periods, E, defined as

$$ E = \sum\nolimits_{i = 1}^{N} {e(i)\Updelta t} $$

where Δt is the sampling time of the data measurement and N is the number of data measured in the MOTION periods.

fMRI data analysis

Preprocessing and statistical analyses of fMRI data were performed by using SPM2 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK; http://fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) implemented in MATLAB 7.0 (Mathworks, Sherborn, MA, USA). Images were realigned to the first volume of the EPI time-series by using rigid-body transformation. Volumes were then normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template by using linear affine transformations. Normalized images of 3 mm × 3 mm × 6 mm were spatially smoothed by a Gaussian kernel of 8-mm full-width at half-maximum. Areas of significant change in the brain were estimated by using the general liner model in a blocked design of the contexts, convolved with the hemodynamic response function. Temporal filtering was achieved by using autoregressive modeling [AR(1)] to model high-frequency noise, and a high-pass filter (cutoff 128 Hz) was applied to remove low-frequency noise.

Direct comparisons between the contexts were conducted to find brain activations that were greater at the higher frequency than at the lower frequency (0.8 Hz versus 0.4 Hz) as well as those that were greater at the lower frequency than at the higher frequency (0.4 Hz versus 0.8 Hz). We analyzed the time series of the images obtained from the seven subjects as one group to increase the sensitivity of the fMRI analysis. This implies that the statistical analyses used in the present study are group fixed-effect analyses (P < 0.001, uncorrected). Anatomical identification was carried out by superimposing the maximum activation foci both on the MNI template and on the normalized structural images of each subject. Anatomical labels of activated regions were made based on the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) by Talairach Daemon (http://www.talairach.org/index.html), after a nonlinear coordinate transformation from MNI to Talairach coordinates (Brett et al. 2001). The location of the dentate nucleus was reconfirmed by using an MRI atlas of the cerebellar nuclei (Dimitrova et al. 2006).

In addition, based on results of the whole-brain analysis, local region-of-interest (ROI) analysis was carried out by using the Marsbar toolbox (Brett et al. 2002; http://marsbar.sourceforge.net) to further investigate brain activations within each noted region. Each ROI was defined as the location of the peak voxel in each region, and was generated by taking a spherical mask surrounding the peak voxel with an 8 mm diameter. We then calculated the percent signal change (PSC) from the time course of hemodynamic response in each subject. Subject C’s PSC in the CB for 0.8 Hz was discarded in calculating correlation because his outlier was beyond the level of two-SD.

Results

Motor performance

Table 1 shows individual values of the force errors, E, calculated from the recorded data for the two reference frequencies, 0.8 and 0.4 Hz. The effect due to frequencies was statistically significant, as the effect was larger at 0.8 Hz (P < 0.05). From self-reports obtained from each subject, all subjects felt that it was easier to correct errors at 0.4 Hz than it was at 0.8 Hz.

Table 1 Force errors in the two different rates of a reference signal

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Table 2 summaries the results of fMRI analyses on anatomical location, MINI coordinates, the activated voxel counts and the peak intensity (maximum Z-score), demonstrating clusters that were significant for direct contrasts in a couple of conditions (uncorrected, P < 0.001). A comparison of the 0.8 Hz results with the 0.4 Hz results revealed that the magnitude of activation in the bilateral PMC significantly increased in proportion to the frequency of a reference signal, whereas foci of activity also could be found in the right M1, the insula, Broca’s area, and CB (see also Fig. 2a). This result follows the previous studies on rate-effect that activations of PMC and CB increase in proportional to movement rate during voluntary movements (cf. VanMeter et al. 1995). On the other hand, the left CB, including the dentate nucleus and tonsil, were more strongly activated at the lower signal frequency than for the higher signal frequency (Fig. 2b). Activations of these cerebellar regions during simple finger movement tasks were reported in previous studies (cf. Penhune and Doyon 2005).

Table 2 Anatomical localizations, activated voxels, and Z-scores of the peak activation that showed a significant difference in direct comparisons between task conditions
Fig. 2
figure 2

Results from direct comparisons between task conditions of a reference signal frequency (P < 0.001, uncorrected) and region-of-interest (ROI) analysis. The arrow indicates the center of ROI. a Greater activations in the direct comparison of 0.8 Hz with 0.4 Hz. b Greater activations in the direct comparison of 0.4 Hz with 0.8 Hz. c Relationships between local brain activations and individual force errors in the right PMC at two different movement frequencies (0.4 and 0.8 Hz). A vertical length is PSCs and a horizontal side is force errors. d Relationships between them in the left CB. All representations are same as c. However, Subject C’s PSC in the CB for 0.8 Hz (the black square with sharp-mark) was discarded in calculating correlation because his outlier was beyond the level of two-SD

Two ROIs were selected based on Table 2, where both the number of voxels and the Z-value were largest in each of the direct comparisons. The first ROI was the region including the right PMC (x = 58, y = 0, z = 46) and the second was found in the left CB (x = −18, y = −52, z = −38). Figure 2c, d shows the relationships between local activations in the two regions and motor performance during movement tasks, respectively. The local activations in the right PMC for 0.8 Hz tended to be larger than for 0.4 Hz, but there was no significant relationship between individual local activations and the amount of movement errors (Fig. 2c). In the left CB, on the other hand, there were significant positive-linear relationships between them for both reference frequencies, whereas the regression coefficient in the case of 0.4 Hz was larger than in the case of 0.8 Hz (Fig. 2d). These results indicated that the CB might play an important role in error correction and that the individual differences of this function reflect brain activation.

Discussion

Action monitoring and error detection are processes that are necessary to adapt motor behavior to the requirements of the current task to be performed. In the present study, therefore, to investigate the relationship between brain activation and movement error correction, we used a force-error correction system that simultaneously and correctly recorded fMRI signals and force errors. Although both functions of the PMC and the CB are triggered by the external signal and it is well known that functions of the CB were deeply related to movement speed, the present results show that the movement-related M1 activity is similar to muscle activity. That is, the present study is consistent with previous findings, confirming that M1 activity is closely related to movement. In addition, the present results show that force errors correspondingly increased with increasing movement rates. Activation of the left CB increased depending on a decrease in the movement rate, whereas activation of the ipsilateral (right) PMC increased depending on increasing movement rates. Furthermore, there were statistically significant relationships between individual force errors and CB activations in both movement rates, but these relationships were not observed in the ipsilateral (right) PMC. Based on these results, human neural mechanisms are related to intervention between error corrections and movement rates.

As described above, the results of the fMRI study demonstrate that ipsilateral PMC and left CB activities definitely changed during error correction movements. In particular, ipsilateral PMC activations increased with an increasing movement rate. Surprisingly, however, CB activations did not increase with increasing movement rates, but instead increased with a lower movement rate. Thus, activation changes in the PMC and the CB with different movement rates might be interpreted as functional interactions because it is known that both activities are related to voluntary movements induced by an external trigger (cf. Porter and Lemon 1993). Although the ipsilateral PMC is sensitive to changes of the movement rate, the CB plays a dominant role in error correction in slow and well-controlled movement. It is expected that these tendencies observed in the right finger will be sharpened when the subjects perform with the left index finger, because subdominant hand movements cause an increase of activity in the CB (Jäncke et al. 1999; Taniwaki et al. 2006). Recently, by using PET, Penhune and Doyon (2005) demonstrated that activity in the CB hemispheres early in learning is likely related to error correction mechanisms that optimize movement kinematics, resulting in improved performance. The present results related to activation in the CB hemispheres, especially the left CB, could be explained in line with the above-mentioned similar activity. The present sequential movement was simple and similar to situations experienced early in learning, especially for the case of the slower movement rate (0.4 Hz). This evidence indicates that there are functional interactions between these two motor cortices that might correspond to different movement rates. In other words, this functional interaction is modulated by task-demanded movement attributes, such as the speed/accuracy trade-off phenomenon (Plamondon and Alimi 1997). In fact, the subjects felt that it was more difficult to correct force errors at the higher frequency because they had to concentrate on gauging the rapid change of errors via the visual feedback display rather than concentrating on controlling their finger motion. They did not know if their motor outputs were helping to reduce errors. On the other hand, all of them felt that it was easy to gauge and correct errors at the lower frequency because they had time to precisely control their motion in response to the error information. These results indicate that the CB works more effectively for learning when the change of error information is harmonized with voluntary motor output as well as sensory input, whereas the PMC activates to compensate the subjective gap between expectations and results. It is suggested that the PMC and the CB play an important role in this movement law and the present results of both brain activations might reflect functional interactions.

The novel finding of the present study was that the amount of individual errors at both movement rates were significantly related to individual CB activations. The CB has traditionally been viewed as an integral part of the motor system (Raymond et al. 1996). CB activation is highly correlated with the SMC since they are involved in the somatosensory feedback of the movement, organizing a perfect transfer of the internal image of the movement into the actual physical conditions of the external world. Therefore, new perspectives on CB function are closely linked to the emergence of cognitive function (attention shifting, Allen et al. 1997; memory rehearsal, Desmond et al. 1997; sensory exploration, Gao et al. 1996). In the present study, there were significant relationships between individual force errors and CB activations. One possible explanation for the present results, therefore, could be in line with viewpoints of internal model hypotheses related to cognitive neuroscience. That is, the CB neurons related to individual abilities in error correction correspond to individual visual affective input and rapidly prime task-relevant systems in order to enhance neural responsiveness (Allen et al. 1997). In the present study, subjects with large force errors showed higher CB activation and vice versa. This evidence indicates that the subjects with a more highly functional internal model produce few errors and do not need higher CB activation. That is, these subjects could automatically perform the task. This mental effort did not activate the CB. Thus, the significant relationships between individual force errors and CB activation might reflect functional differences in individual internal models developed by different subjects.

On the other hand, it is suggested that the PMC may have a role in preprogrammed processes linked to sequential motor actions, and therefore have an important role in the generation of sequences from memory that fit into a precise timing plan. In particular, the right PMC (ipsilateral PMC in the present study) has a role in working memory (Sadato et al. 1996b). As described above in the low movement rate study, there were no relationships between individual force errors and ipsilateral PMC activation. This evidence suggests that interactions of the PMC and the CB are induced by performing the error correction task, but offer a potentially causal link between them for on-line performance of error correction at different movement rates. In particular, the present evidence suggests the idea that when an error is detected at the slower movement rate, descending output from the CB nuclei allows for rapid, on-line adjustments in the motor patterns without requiring extensive cortical intervention. This error correction hypothesis implicitly assumes that the CB has evolved so that it can now provide a more genetic error correction role, modified by an individual’s internal model in addition to external movement (Ivry and Fiez 2000).

With regard to the neural mechanisms of the interactions between the cortical (PMC) and the subcortical (CB) motor circuits, the present results suggest that these motor circuits are exclusively devoted to controlling movement rate and error correction. More recently, using fMRI, Galléa et al. (2008) demonstrated that SMA and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex activation increased with error and task difficulty independent of the accuracy of motor control. Similarly, the present results suggest a clear functional dissociation between PMC and CB in controlling movement rate and error correction and have provided new insights into the interactions between actions and cognition. Thus, our findings in the present study have confirmed above-mentioned broad functional implications, but more investigations are needed to clarify further neural mechanisms related to human error correction and the control of movement rate. The use of an on-line motor control paradigm eliciting errors allowed us to show error-related brain activity pattern independent to decision making.