Introduction

Several studies have recently shown that in addition to the gains in performance that emerge concurrently with practice on a novel sequence of movements (within session gains), robust gains in the speed and accuracy of performance can evolve after the practice experience has ended, i.e., between sessions, delayed gains (e.g., Fischer et al. 2002; Karni et al. 1998a, b; Walker et al. 2002). These delayed gains in performance presumably reflect procedural memory consolidation processes (Karni et al. 1998a, b; Ungerleider et al. 2002; Walker et al. 2002, 2003; Korman et al. 2003; Maquet et al. 2003). There is, however, another measure for memory consolidation, established in both human (e.g., Brashers-Krug et al. 1996; Krakauer et al. 2005) and animal (McGaugh 2000) models of memory formation. The latter measure relates to the notion that during the post-acquisition phase, newly acquired experience-dependent neural changes are transformed from tenuous, interference susceptible, into more stable forms (McGaugh 2000). Hence, within the first few hours after the termination of a training experience, subsequent experience may result in the loss of the expected training related gains in the initial task (Brashers-Krug et al. 1996; Shadmehr and Brashers-Krug 1997; Walker et al. 2003; but see Goedert and Willingham 2002).

It is not clear however, what types of experience constitute effective interference for a given task. Effective interference is often caused by a variation on the initial task (Adams 1987; Miall et al. 2004; Krakauer et al. 2005) suggesting that tasks that depend on similar performance parameters and neural resources would be more likely to interfere with each other. Thus, interference occurs if there is a critical overlap between the representations of the two tasks experienced during the consolidation time-window (Tong et al. 2002). Such an overlap may result in the weakening or even the taking over of ongoing neuronal consolidation processes, triggered by the initial training experience, by the subsequent experience (Fonseca et al. 2004).

Previous studies have suggested that the level of experience with the to-be-interfered (initial) task may constitute a critical factor in determining the amount of interference between two tasks, with novice performers more susceptible to interference than expert ones (Shea and Morgan 1979; Del Rey et al. 1982; Shea et al. 1990; Guadagnoli and Lee 2004). Others have argued that major shifts occur between brain representations of motor performance, as a function of experience (e.g., Shadmehr and Holcomb 1997; Korman et al. 2003; Sosnik et al. 2004; Ungerleider et al. 2002; Jueptner et al. 1997). We tested the notion that different motor representations subserve the performance of well and less well-trained handwriting sequences, and therefore determine its interference potential with a given non-writing task. Our results suggest that rather than the task per-se, the level of experience with, and the type of motor sequences performed in the subsequent task, constitute a critical factor in determining its interference potential, even when the resemblance of the movements of the interfering task to the movements of the initial task is minimal.

Materials and methods

Experiment 1

Participants

Forty-three young university students (mean 25.13, SD 3.21), 14 men and 29 women, were paid to train on the FOG learning task. All were right handed, native Hebrew speakers, highly familiar with the Latin alphabet through mainly English script. Participants had no medical conditions that could impair fine motor performance and had no known learning disability. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Tel-Aviv Souraski Medical Center.

Design and procedure

Each participant took part in two study phases, 1 week apart (Fig. 1b). Each study phase consisted of a training session and a test session a day later. In all sessions, the participants performed the tasks while lying supine with their arm supported on their hip and the palm facing up, in direct view of a video camera. In the training session, participants were instructed to oppose the fingers of their dominant hand to the thumb in one of three possible sequences (43124, 42314, 41324) (Fig. 1a). Visual feedback was not afforded. In the initial session, each participant underwent a pre-training performance test (test 1), a training interval and an immediate post-training performance test (test 2). The next day, a delayed performance test (test 3) was administered. Each training interval consisted of 160 repetitions of the sequence divided into 20 training blocks. During training, following preparatory and “start” auditory signals for each block, the initiation of each sequence was cued by an auditory signal at a rate of 0.4 Hz (2.5 s per sequence). Each training block terminated in a “stop” signal. The three performance tests were composed of four, 30 s blocks, each initiated by an auditory “start” signal, during which participants were instructed to tap the sequence repeatedly and continuously as fast and accurately as possible, until given a stop signal. The intervals between test blocks were kept constant (30 s). No feedback was provided. The participants’ performance during the test blocks was video recorded and two measures of performance were determined from the recordings: a. speed-number of correctly completed sequences in a block and b. Accuracy-number of errors made in a block (incorrect sequences). One experimenter scored the video recordings by playback at reduced speed.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a The finger-to-thumb opposition task. The three sequences were similarly constructed and contain identical component movements. b The design of the baseline and the interference conditions, in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. Note that the conditions are identical except for the writing (interference) phase added in the latter condition

Twenty-two participants first completed the baseline condition and then the interference condition and 21 were run in the reversed order. Both conditions were identical except that in the latter condition, participants were required to perform a handwriting (interference) task immediately after test2 (Fig. 1b). The two conditions were run on separate sessions, 1 week apart. The order of the conditions (phases) was pseudo-randomized across participants.

In the interference task, participants wrote two, randomly ordered, auditory dictated Hebrew words, using either the Hebrew alphabet or the Latin (Heblatin). [The two words were “closet” and “pillar” ( and —“aron” and “amud”, in Hebrew and Latin alphabets, respectively)]. Participants were randomised into two groups; one group of 21 subjects was instructed to write in Hebrew (familiar writing condition), and the other group, with 22 subjects, in Heblatin (unfamiliar writing condition). Writing was done with a non-inking pen on A4 paper laid upon a digitizing tablet (WACOM, Intuos 2, 17.5′′ × 17.1′′× 1.5′′, active area 12′′ × 12′′, sampling rate of 100 Hz, spatial accuracy ± 0.025 cm, spatial resolution 0.05 cm) mounted perpendicular to the bed in a custom holding device. A double mirror system was used to enable participants get a clear view of their hand movements only during writing. The workspace for the handwriting task was composed of four horizontally parallel lines (each 8 cm long, and spaced 1.3 cm apart) on the lower right side of the paper (since writing in Hebrew progresses from right to left). Each participant chose (based on comfort) one line that was then used throughout the session. Participants were instructed to rest the pen tip on a cross, placed to the right of each line, and return to this point after writing each word. Overall, the task consisted of 16 repetitions of each of the two target words in a random order (total of 32 words) divided into 8 blocks of writing (4 words in each block) separated by 30 s of rest. Each trial (word) was cued by an auditory signal with 3.5 s afforded for its completion. The words were presented amplified through loudspeakers.

Experiment 2

To further explore the loss of performance gains after Hebrew interference (including the negative delayed gains), at 24 h post-training relative to the performance at the end of the practice session, 12 additional participants (2 men, 10 women; mean age 25.37, SD 1.4) were trained in the FOS and wrote in Hebrew immediately after test 2 (that followed the termination of the training session). The design and procedures of Experiment 2 were identical to those used in the Hebrew condition of Experiment 1. Each participant completed two study phases, baseline and Hebrew interference 1 week apart. Half the participants first completed the baseline condition and then the Hebrew interference condition and the rest were run in the reversed order. This design allowed for a full, within-subjects repeated measures comparison.

Results

Experiment 1

Table 1 presents the average number of correctly completed sequences and the average number of errors summed across the test blocks at the beginning of training, by the end of training and at 24 h post-training, in each of the two conditions for each group. All analyses were designed as within-subject comparisons because of the small groups and the individual differences in the speed and accuracy of performance. Only the effect of the order of conditions was tested in a between-subject comparison, as each participant was tested in one of two possible orders of conditions. Two separate repeated measures analyses were run for each group, with either the number of sequences performed correctly during the block (speed) or the number of errors per block (accuracy) i.e., number of sequences performed incorrectly, as the dependent variables. Here, and elsewhere, results were corrected for non-sphericity violation using Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment.

Table 1 Performance of the FOS task before training (test1), after the training session (test2) and on the following day (test3) in the two groups (mean and standard deviations). P values refer to comparisons (dependent t-tests) between the relative gains in the baseline and corresponding interference conditions, i.e., for Δ1–2 and Δ2–3

To test for order effects (i.e., whether initial performance and the gains accrued in training within the session differed when baseline condition or the interference condition were tested first) across both groups, a repeated measures general linear model was applied in which condition (with two levels baseline or interference), test (with two levels test 1, test 2, corresponding to initial performance and the performance at the termination of the training session, respectively) and block (with four levels 1st, 2nd, 3rd, and 4th block) were within-subjects variables and order (with two levels baseline first or interference first) was a between-subjects variable. No significant order effects were found for either speed [F(1,41) = 2.7, P = 0.106] or accuracy [F(1,41) = 0.12, P = 0.730].

Training on a given sequence of movements resulted in robust within-session (early) gains in both speed and accuracy, in the two groups (Table 1). This held true, irrespective of whether interference experience was given after the termination of the training session. A repeated measures general linear model was applied for each group separately, in which condition (with two levels baseline or interference), test (with two levels test 1, test 2, corresponding to initial performance and the performance at the termination of the training session, respectively) and block (with four levels 1st, 2nd, 3rd, and 4th block) were within-subjects variables. There was no significant difference between the two conditions in each of the two groups in terms of within session gains. For speed, the main effect for test were significant [Hebrew group, F(1,20) = 103.6, P < 0.001; Heblatin group, F(1,21) = 194.3, P < 0.001], with superior performance in test2 compared to test1. For accuracy, the main effect of test was significant in the Hebrew group [F(1,20) = 7.8, P = 0.011], but showed only a trend for improvement in the Heblatin group [F(1,21) = 3.0, P = 0.096], although overall participants made only a small number of errors in either test periods (Table 1). Finally, the interaction of condition and test was not significant, in both groups, for speed [Hebrew group, F(1,20) = 1.0, P = 0.320; Heblatin group, F(1,21) = 2.2, P = 0.157] and accuracy [Hebrew group, F(1,20) = 0.81, P = 0.378; Heblatin group, F(1,21) = 0.95, P = 0.341].

Training also resulted in significant gains in both speed and accuracy, when performance at 24 h post-training was compared to performance at the end of the training session; i.e., significant gains have evolved after the termination of training, during this interval, in both groups (delayed, between-sessions, gains). However, the delayed gains were significantly reduced when an interference experience of, specifically, writing in Hebrew was given. A repeated measures general linear model was applied for each group separately, in which condition (with two levels baseline or interference), test (with two levels test 2 and test 3, corresponding to the performance at the termination of the training session and on the following day, respectively), and block (with four levels 1st, 2nd, 3rd, and 4th block) were within-subjects variables. Two separate analyses were run, with either speed or accuracy as the dependent variables. For performance speed, there was a significant main effect for test in both groups [Hebrew group, F(1,20) = 54.5, P < 0.001; Heblatin group, F(1,21) = 75.7, P < 0.001], with superior performance in test3 compared to test2. However, the interaction of condition and test was significant in the Hebrew group [F(1,20) = 4.5, P = 0.045], with smaller delayed performance gains when interference was given. This interaction was not significant in the Heblatin group [F(1,21) = 0.00, P = 0.976]. Furthermore, the main effect for accuracy, i.e., a decrease in the number of errors at test3, was significant in the Heblatin group [F(1,21) = 24.8, P < 0.001] but not in the Hebrew group [F(1,20) = 1.9, P = 0.187]. The interaction of condition and test was not significant for accuracy presumably because accuracy was almost at ceiling, in both groups [Hebrew group, F(1, 20) = 1.9, P = 0.174; Heblatin group, F(1,21) = 0.71, P = 0.407]. No significant between-subjects effect of order was found for either speed [F(1,41) = 2.0, P = 0.163] or accuracy [F(1,41) = 0.09, P = 0.762].

Since testing constituted by necessity additional training in the task, the block-by-block improvement in speed was examined in the three tests. As previously described (Korman et al. 2003), there was a significant improvement in speed only in test1 (Hebrew group F(1,20) = 4.4, P = 0.048, F(1,20) = 15.7, P < 0.001, baseline and interference condition, respectively; Heblatin group F(1,21) = 7.7, P = 0.011, F(1,21) = 30.7, P < 0.001), baseline and interference condition, respectively). There was no significant trend for improvement between blocks in test2 and test3, in both groups, in both conditions. The significant delayed gains in performance cannot, therefore, be ascribed to practice effect that stem from the testing.

To further test the difference between the delayed gains accrued in the interference condition compared to the gains accrued in the baseline condition, the absolute gains in performance (i.e., the difference between tests 2 and 3 (Δ2–3) was computed in each condition (baseline or interference). For accuracy, the number of errors made in a given test was subtracted from the number of errors made in the preceding test (i.e., a reduction in errors = a positive Δ). Paired-samples t tests were used to compare the Δs achieved in the baseline and the corresponding interference conditions separately for the two groups (Hebrew or Heblatin) (Table 1). As can be seen in Table 1, the relative delayed gains (Δ2–3) were reduced in the Hebrew interference condition (on average only 4.4 sequences compared to 7.7, 8 and 8 sequences in the baseline and the Heblatin interference condition and corresponding baseline, respectively). The differential effect of writing Hebrew or Heblatin on the learning of the movement sequence can be seen in Fig. 2, which presents the average delayed gains for speed and accuracy, in both groups (Hebrew or Heblatin). While writing in Heblatin after the termination of the training session resulted in as robust gains as in the corresponding baseline condition, writing in Hebrew resulted in clearly reduced gains in speed, compared to the corresponding baseline condition gains. The absolute gains accrued during the training session (within-session, between tests 1 and 2, Δ1–2) showed that the gains for both speed and accuracy were not significantly different in the interference conditions, as compared to their corresponding baseline conditions in both groups (Table 1).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Delayed gains (Δ2–3) in speed (number of sequences, upper panel) and accuracy (reduction in errors, lower panel) in the baseline and corresponding interference conditions, in the Hebrew and Heblatin groups, in experiment 1

In the interference condition, six participants of the Hebrew group failed to show any improvement in speed (and even demonstrated some loss of performance relative to the end of the practice session; i.e., negative delayed gains) at test3 as compared to test2, while only one to two participants failed to improve in the Heblatin interference condition and in the two baseline conditions (Fig. 3a). There was no significant difference between each interference condition and its corresponding baseline in terms of accuracy (Table 1 and Fig. 3b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Individual data: the difference (Δ2–3) in number of completed sequences in test3 relative to test2 (a) and the difference in the total number of errors in test2 relative to test3 [note that the number of errors made in the preceding test was subtracted from the number of errors made in the following test (i.e., a reduction in errors = a positive Δ)] (b) performed by each participant in the Hebrew, Heblatin and the corresponding baseline conditions, in experiment 1. The horizontal lines indicate the group average differences in each condition. Note that negative scores (locate under the zero line) indicate negative delayed gains, e.g., an inferior performance in test 3 relative to test 2

The absolute gains within the training session and between-sessions (Δ1–2 and Δ2–3, respectively) were not correlated with either the order of conditions (baseline condition completed before interference condition or vise versa) or the specific sequence of opposition movements used in training (43124, 42314, or 41324) (for performance speed r = 0.03, P = 0.789, and r = 0.11, P = 0.309, between order and Δ1–2 or Δ2–3, respectively; r = −0.19, P = 0.071, and r = 0.02, P = 0.894, sequence and Δ1–2 or Δ2–3, respectively). In addition, no significant correlations were found between order and Δ1–2 or Δ2–3 (r = 0.17, P = 0.108, and r = −0.08, P = 0.445, respectively) and between sequence and Δ1–2 or Δ2–3 (r = 0.01, P = 0.929, and r = 0.00, P = 0.977, respectively) for accuracy.

There were no significant correlations between the performance gain of individual participants in test2 and their performance gains in test3 in any of the three conditions; baseline (r = 0.06, P = 0.709), Hebrew interference (r = −0.32, P = 0.147), or Heblatin interference (r = −0.18, P = 0.434). Thus, the within-session gains were not correlated with the delayed, between-session gains in performance.

Experiment 2

In Experiment 2, we replicated the Hebrew condition of Experiment 1. Experiment 2 was run to substantiate the finding that handwriting in the participants’ native script could significantly reduce the expected delayed gains in the FOS task and moreover, result in an actual loss of performance, relative to the performance at the end of the practice session (negative delayed gains), by 24 h post-training. There was no significant difference between the baseline and the Hebrew interference conditions in the early, within-session gains (Fig. 4). A repeated measures general linear model was run separately for speed and for accuracy, with condition (with two levels baseline or interference), test (with two levels test 1, test 2) and block (with four levels 1st, 2nd, 3rd, and 4th block) as within-subjects variables and order (with two levels baseline first or interference first) as a between-subjects variable. No significant order effect for either speed [F(1,10) = 0.02, P = 0.886] or accuracy [F(1,10) = 0.87, P = 0.371] was found. Significant main effects of test and block were found for speed [F(1,10) = 138.9, P < 0.001; F(1,10) = 7.2, P = 0.023] showing that on both conditions, participants performed better in test2 compared to test1. No significant effects were found for accuracy, however the error rate was small in all phases of training (0.812 ± 0.18, 0.687 ± 0.18, mean and SD of number of errors in test1 and test2, respectively, in the baseline condition; 0.646 ± 0.15, 0.500 ± 0.14, mean and SD of number of errors in test1 and test2, respectively, in the Hebrew interference condition). There was a significant test and block interaction in speed, reflecting the finding that there was block-by-block improvement in test1 but not in test2 (F(1,10) = 6.164, P = 0.032). All the other main effects and their interactions were not significant for either speed or accuracy.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Group performance (average number of completed sequences in a block) at tests 1, 2 and 3 in the baseline and Hebrew conditions, in Experiment 2

To examine the differences between the baseline and the Hebrew interference conditions in terms of the delayed, between-sessions gains (speed and accuracy), a similar analysis was used with condition (with two levels baseline or interference), test (with two levels test2, test3) and block (with four levels 1st, 2nd, 3rd, and 4th block) as within-subjects variables and order (with two levels baseline first or interference first) as a between-subjects variable. No significant order effects were found for speed [F(1,10) = 0.00, P = 0.954], however, order did have a significant effect for accuracy [F(1, 10) = 7.3, P = 0.022] though here also, the error rates were very small (0.313 ± 0.46, 0.490 ± 0.46, mean and SD of number of errors; baseline before interference, and reversed order, respectively). For performance speed, there was a significant main effect of test [F(1,10) = 20.4, P < 0.001] with participants performing better in test3 then in test2, irrespective of condition. The main effect of test for accuracy was also significant [F(1,10) = 13.7, P = 0.004]. However, a significant interaction between condition and test was found [F(1,10) = 8.4, P = 0.016] indicating the inferior performance in the Hebrew interference condition compared to the baseline condition at test3 (Table 1 and Fig. 4). No additional significant main effects or interactions were found either for speed or for accuracy.

As in experiment 1, the absolute gains in performance [i.e., the difference between tests 1 and 2 (Δ1–2) and between tests 2 and 3 (Δ2–3)] were computed in each condition (baseline or interference) for speed and accuracy. For Δ1–2, a measure of the within-session gains, there was no significant difference between the two conditions in either speed or accuracy [paired t test, t(11) = 0.10, P = 0.918; t(11) = 0.07, P = 0.949, respectively]. For Δ2–3, i.e., the delayed gains, however, there was a significant difference in speed between the two conditions [t(11) = 3.0, P = 0.012). The difference in the delayed gains, for accuracy, was not significant [t(11) = 1.3, P = 0.203)].

Figure 5 depicts the absolute gains in performance speed at test2 relative to test1 (Δ1–2) and at test3 relative to test2 (Δ2–3) in the Hebrew condition, on an individual basis. In the baseline condition, all of the participants showed some improvement during the 24 h post-training interval. However, in the Hebrew interference condition, 6 participants out of the 12 (50%) showed an actual loss of performance, in terms of speed, by 24 h post training, relative to performance speed at the end of the practice session (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Individual data: the absolute gains, for speed, at test2 relative to test1 (Δ1–2) and at test3 relative to test2 (Δ2–3) in the Hebrew condition, in Experiment 2

Discussion

Altogether, the results of the current experiments show that the evolution of delayed gains in performance, accrued in practice on the FOS, could be significantly interfered with if, shortly after the termination of the training session, the participants wrote a small number of common words in their native handwriting. In a number of participants (28–50%), an actual loss of the within-session performance gains occurred in the 24 h post-training interval in the Hebrew interference condition (negative delayed gains). However, the same number of words in a less practiced mode of handwriting—Heblatin—resulted in no interference, with as robust within and between session gains as those accrued when no interference was introduced.

The strength of the current results derives from the within-subjects comparisons afforded by the study design with each participant providing his or her own control data. Thus, although the interference effects in both experiments were small in absolute terms (each additional sequence in a test block stands, nevertheless, for five additional correct opposition movements), the interference by handwriting in the participants native script resulted on average in a 50% reduction in the expected delayed gains, and often in no gains whatsoever.

Recent studies reported significant interference when shortly after the training on a given FOS, training on a different (reversed movement) sequence (Walker et al. 2003) or the performance of the Tower of Hanoi task (E. Vakil et al., Psychonomics Society abstracts 2002) was introduced. The within-session gains however, were maintained in both studies. Interference related loss of both the within-session and the delayed gains was reported in tasks other than the FOS (Shadmehr and Brashers-Krug 1997; Hauptmann and Karni 2002; Tong et al. 2002).

It is not clear which factors determine the establishment and the magnitude of interference effects (Adams 1987; Flanagan et al. 1999; Krakauer et al. 1999; Goerdert and Willingham 2002; Hauptmann and Karni 2002; Krakauer et al. 2005). A leading notion is that, an effective interference experience is one that shares some critical motor representation, or internal model, with the initial, interfered, task (e.g., Shadmehr and Brashers-Krug 1997; Tong et al. 2002). The conjecture is that, both the initial training and the subsequent interference experiences activate overlapping neural representations in specific brain areas, but the two experiences differ in the sense that each requires quite different adaptation parameters (Robertson et al. 2004). The need to adapt to the demands of the second task may therefore, eliminate or supersede the settings of the initial one with an advantage for the gains attained in the latest experience (Brashers-Krug et al. 1996; Krakauer et al. 1999; Hauptmann and Karni 2002).

The notion of consolidation was recently challenged by studies showing no diminution of the interference effect with time. Interference was found when the interfering task was carried out 24 h or even a week after the termination of training on the initial task (Goedert and Willingham 2002; Caithness et al. 2004). It may be the case that in both studies, skill learning effects (by necessity requiring memory consolidation) and adaptation effects (which may be long-lasting but do not necessitate consolidation effects, see Hauptmann and Karni 2002) were interchangeably measured. When insufficient training was afforded and presumably when two tasks are interleaved within a session, consolidation processes may not be triggered or alternatively slowed down (Adams 1987; Hauptmann and Karni 2002). Because interference can occur at the adaptation phase, irrespective of consolidation, Hauptmann and Karni (2002) have argued that the evolution of delayed gains is the more sensitive measure for the triggering of consolidation processes.

Motor sequence learning may involve changes in primary motor cortex (M1) activation (Ungerleider et al. 2002). Although other areas have also been implicated, learning related effects in M1 were shown to occur both within a short practice session, corresponding to the within-session performance gains and corresponding to multi-session gains and the long-term retention of some skills (e.g., Friston et al. 1992; Grafton et al. 1992; Kawashima et al. 1994; Karni et al. 1995; Nudo et al. 1996; Doyon et al. 2002; Muellbacher et al. 2001; Robertson et al. 2005; Koeneke et al. 2006). In humans (e.g., Karni et al. 1995, 1998a, b) and monkeys (Nudo et al. 1996) extended motor practice was shown to result in the recruitment of additional M1 units into a local network specifically representing the trained motor sequence. Based on these results, it was proposed that M1 might code not just for simple, single, movements, but also for complex movement sequences (Tanji and Shima 1994; Karni et al. 1998a, b; Georgopoulos 2000). Furthermore, it was suggested that given extensive training, even complex movement sequences may come to be represented in part by low-level, presumably M1, movement modules (e.g., Karni et al. 1998a, b; Sosnik et al. 2004). Based on this notion, Ungerleider et al. (2002) suggested that a given subpopulation of neurons within a representational domain, such as the hand’s, participate in the representation of different movement sequences but that different movement sequences are realized by differential recruitment of these units. The advantage of such a parsimonious system may lay in the potential to support the learning of many parallel skills within a given restricted representation. However, such a system may be more susceptible to interference in a way that even movement sequences of very different attributes can interfere with each other.

The current results suggest that the ordering and scheduling of tasks during training and also in the hours following an effective training session may be critical for long-term motor memory, raising concerns about arbitrary scheduling in laboratory, academic and neurological rehabilitation practice. Thus, writing immediately after practice on a musical instrument may decrease the long-term effectiveness of the music lesson.

Our results also show, that behavioral interference might occur also when the two tasks are apparently very dissimilar and seem to share little or no kinematic or dynamic features. Both writing modes that were used as interference were complex and presumably involved a rather similar set of strokes (Namboodiri and Jain 2004). Given the logic of the representational overlap conjecture, our results suggest that the two handwriting modes do not activate the same motor representations, or rather, that these representations diverge in the extent of their overlap with the FOS representation. One critical difference between the two modes of writing may lie in the participants’ expertise level—they were less skilled in Heblatin writing compared to Hebrew writing. The proposal therefore is that the differential effect of the two handwriting scripts tested in the current study, on the very same motor representation of the FOS, reflects a difference in the respective brain representations of well trained, versus relatively novel sequences of handwriting.

There is some support for the notion that while the representation of well-practiced handwriting sequences is effector dependent and necessitates the activation of low-level motor areas (e.g., M1) rather than more high-level ones for fluent performance, unpracticed writing sequences may require more activation of high-level areas (e.g., the SMA) and depend less on M1 based fluency (Karni et al. 1998a, b; Sosnik et al. 2004, Society for Neuroscience abstract 533.17, 2004). As opposed to well-practiced handwriting, controlled, less practiced handwriting is associated with kinematic non-fluency and shows increased neuronal activity in the contralateral primary sensorimotor cortex, frontal premotor area (lateral pre-motor cortex and SMA), parietal cortex, and the putamen. It was suggested, that during controlled processing, these brain regions might be involved in the generation of the motor output, in sensorimotor integration and in attentional processes (Siebner et al. 2001). Support for the notion of different brain activation patterns underlie the performance of well-practiced versus less practiced handwriting comes from studies that compared well-practiced handwriting in the dominant hand to handwriting in the non-dominant hand (e.g., Hoshiyama and Kakigi 1999; Karni et al. 1998a, b; Robertson et al. 2005). A similar notion has been advanced in motor learning in the monkey (Pavlides et al. 1993; Tanji and Shima 1994).

The extent, to which one task may interfere with another, may be related to the learner’s level of expertise in both tasks (Schmidt 1975; Del-Rey et al. 1982; Shea et al. 1990). Others, (e.g., Guadagnoli and Lee 2004) have interpreted the finding that novices, as opposed to experts, show reduced delayed gains following training on multiple variations of the same task (contextual interference) in terms of a competition on “limited processing capacity” such as attention resources. Our results do not support either explanation, which would predict that the Heblatin condition, both relatively novel and likely to require more attention in its execution compared to the well-practiced Hebrew, was more likely to interfere with the FOS. Thus, one or more of the successive task variations that followed each other within a short time interval, could have constituted an effective interference to the initial task conditions. In experts, the representation of each task variation may be subserved by a specific, well-established routine (e.g., Karni et al. 1998a, b; Korman et al. 2003; Sosnik et al. 2004). Moreover, these routines may be subserved by specific motor representations perhaps embodied in a module consisting of a sub-population of neurons in a given shared representation (e.g., Ungerleider et al. 2002).

Taken together, our results suggest the notion that well-trained handwriting sequences are represented at some critical neuronal level in a manner more overlapping the execution of a recently acquired sequence of finger movements compared to the representation of much less practiced handwriting sequences. These results support the proposal (Korman et al. 2003; Sosnik et al. 2004) that, with extended practice, well-practiced movement sequences undergo not only qualitative changes, movement chunking and co-articulation, but also shift in the hierarchical sense, and come to be represented, at least in part, as novel motor primitives in motor cortex.