Introduction

The startle response is the reflex-like motor reaction elicited by salient sensory stimuli. This response can be produced by acoustic, tactile, or visual stimuli (Landis et al. 1939) and is present in different species ranging from simple invertebrate organisms (Eaton 1984) to mammals including humans (Graham 1975; Koch 1999). The magnitude of the startle response can be modulated by internal factors (e.g., potentiated by fear or attenuated by pleasure; Brown et al. 1951; Schmid et al. 1995) as well as by external variables. For instance, preceding a startling stimulus (or pulse) closely in time by another stimulus (prepulse) can cause a reduction in the magnitude of the startle response when compared to that produced by the startling stimulus alone (Hoffman and Searle 1968), an effect known as prepulse inhibition (PPI). PPI is a neurobehavioral process in which the experience of an initial sensory stimulus attenuates the motoric startle response to a closely following sensory stimulus, a phenomenon that can be used as an operational measure of sensorimotor gating (Swerdlow et al. 2000). Deficits in PPI have been reported in individuals with psychiatric and neurobiological disorders such as schizophrenia (Braff et al. 1978), Tourette's syndrome (Castellanos et al. 1996), obsessive–compulsive disorder (Swerdlow et al. 1993b), and Huntington's disease (Swerdlow et al. 1995). Therefore, studying the neural mechanisms that regulate PPI may provide further information about the physiological bases of these disorders and help in the development of therapeutic treatments for these syndromes.

Hoffman and Ison (1980) put forward a model PPI circuit in which sensory stimuli trigger both fast excitatory and slow inhibitory signals on different pathways that converge to produce the overall startle response. Later studies showed that excitatory information in this “mediatory” PPI circuit reaches the motoneurons through the caudal pontine reticular nucleus (PNC) (Davis et al. 1982), whereas inhibitory signals triggered by stimuli of different sensory modalities converge in the superior colliculus (Meredith et al. 1992), activating the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPT) (Fendt et al. 1994), and in turn inhibiting the PNC through a cholinergic muscarinic PPT–PNC projection (Koch et al. 1993). In addition, other brain regions not included in the mediatory circuit can modulate PPI by regulating activity in the PPT, such as the ventral pallidum, nucleus accumbens (NAC), entorhinal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), hippocampus, and amygdala (Koch 1999). Animal models of schizophrenia in humans include pharmacological manipulations that target these “modulatory” brain areas in rats and produce deficits in PPI similar to those observed in patients with schizophrenia (Geyer et al. 2001).

Systemic administration of dopamine receptor agonists such as amphetamine or apomorphine reduces PPI, a disruption that can be reversed by the co-administration of the typical antipsychotic haloperidol (Mansbach et al. 1988). Furthermore, direct infusion of dopamine in the NAC in rats produces a dose-dependent decrease in PPI (Swerdlow et al. 1990) suggesting that sensorimotor gating deficits associated with dopamine receptor agonists are associated to overactive dopamine activity. Similarly, it has been shown that systemic administration of NMDA receptor antagonists such as dizocilpine or phencyclidine (PCP) reduces PPI in rats (Mansbach and Geyer 1989). However, these disruptions are not reversed by haloperidol (Keith et al. 1991) indicating that NMDA-receptor antagonist induced PPI disruptions are not mediated by the mesolimbic dopamine system. Bakshi and Geyer (1998) studied the effects of localized bilateral infusions of dizocilpine on PPI in different limbic regions, finding significant decreases in PPI after infusions in the amygdala or dorsal hippocampus, and a trend toward reduction in PPI after administration in the MPFC, but no changes after infusions in the NAC, ventral hippocampus, or dorsomedial thalamus. Therefore, reduction of PPI by pharmacological stimulation of dopamine receptors or blockade of NMDA glutamate receptors can be used to determine the contribution of other receptors in the reinstatement of normal sensorimotor gating properties.

Whereas typical antipsychotics (such as haloperidol) exert strong dopamine receptor blockage, atypical antipsychotics target a wider range of receptors. For instance, the atypical antipsychotic clozapine has complex antagonistic effects on dopamine D1–D4, histamine H1, serotonin 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C, norepinephrine α1 and α2, and acetylcholine muscarinic receptors (Coward 1992). By having a wider range of receptor blockage action, clozapine has been shown to effectively reverse both apomorphine- (Swerdlow and Geyer 1993) and dizocilpine-induced (Bakshi et al. 1994; Bortolato et al. 2005; Bubenikova et al. 2005; Caceda et al. 2005) PPI disruptions in rats. In order to develop improved antipsychotic treatments, it is important to study the effects of drugs that specifically target the same receptors affected by clozapine to determine their potential therapeutic or detrimental effects. In previous studies, we have shown that the H1-antagonist pyrilamine significantly attenuates the PPI impairments caused by either amphetamine (Larrauri and Levin 2010) or dizocilpine (Roegge et al. 2007). We also found that the addition of the 5-HT2A/2C antagonist ketanserin reduced the effectiveness of pyrilamine in counteracting amphetamine-induced PPI deficits (Larrauri and Levin 2010).

As mentioned, clozapine has, among others, antagonistic effects on α2-adrenergic receptors. Litman et al. (1996) showed that the addition of idazoxan (an α2-adrenergic antagonist) to a fluphenazine (a typical antipsychotic) treatment resulted in significant reductions in psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophrenia, when compared to the fluphenazine treatment alone. Furthermore, the reduction in psychosis obtained with the idazoxan and fluphenazine treatment was comparable to the one obtained with a clozapine treatment. Hertel et al. (1999) showed that the combination of idazoxan and raclopride (D2-dopamine antagonist) resulted in an increase of dopamine in the MPFC in rats, an effect similar to the one obtained with clozapine (Moghaddam and Bunney 1990). Overall, these results suggest that the superior antipsychotic effects of clozapine may be related to the α2 receptor blockade by this drug. Therefore, the goal of the present study was to examine the effects of the α2-adrenergic antagonist idazoxan on amphetamine-induced (“Experiment 1”) and dizocilpine-induced (“Experiment 2”) PPI disruptions. Based on the reported beneficial effects of idazoxan in the treatment of schizophrenia with typical antipsychotics (Litman et al. 1996), an improvement in sensorimotor gating (as indexed by PPI) after its disruption with amphetamine or dizocilpine is hypothesized.

Methods

Subjects

Adult female Sprague–Dawley rats (n = 12 for “Experiment 1” and n = 11 for “Experiment 2”) obtained from Taconic Farms (Germantown, NY, USA) were tested for auditory PPI of the tactile startle response. Animals were housed in groups of three per cage at an ambient temperature of 20°C on a 12:12-h reverse dark:light cycle (lights off at 0700 h) and had ad-libitum access to water and food. In “Experiment 1”, animals' body weights ranged between 242 and 325 g and were 93 days old at the start of test sessions. In “Experiment 2”, animals were 98 days old at the beginning of test sessions, and their body weights varied between 242 and 298 g. Rats were allowed to acclimate to the housing facilities for 2 weeks after arrival and were acclimated to the test room and experimental apparatus before testing sessions began. Before the beginning of test sessions, animals received eight handling sessions (daily), followed by five sessions of exposure to the testing situation (apparatus and stimuli) conducted 2 days apart. Testing started 2 days after the last exposure session. Animals were maintained according to NIH guidelines, and protocols were approved by the Duke University Animal Care and Use Committee.

Apparatus

Tactile startle responses and prepulse inhibition were measured using the SR-Lab Startle Response System manufactured by San Diego Instruments (SDI). Plexiglas cylinders (9 cm inside diameter), each with a piezoelectric accelerometer attached to its base, were used to confine the animals inside the sound-attenuating test chambers. All piezoelectric accelerometer responses were standardized using the SDI calibrator. Data from the accelerometers gauging the animals' responses were collected in 1-s time windows beginning at the onset of the startling stimuli and sampled at a 1-MHz frequency. The startle response amplitude was calculated as the difference between the peak response in the 200-ms time window after the stimulus onset and the pre-stimulus activity. Auditory stimuli were presented through speakers located in the test chambers above the restraining tubes, and tactile stimuli (air puffs) were delivered through copper tubes (4 mm inner diameter, 75 cm long) connected to a compressed air tank that entered each Plexiglas tube through a small opening on the top of the cylinder. Auditory stimuli in all chambers were calibrated using a digital sound level meter (Extech Instruments), and the air-puff pressure was controlled at 207 kPa (30 PSI, which produced an 82-dB noise signal) through a standard gas regulator connected between the main air pipe and the copper tubes. The background noise throughout test sessions was a 65-dB broad-band noise.

Startle and prepulse inhibition procedure

At the beginning of each test session, there was a 5-min acclimation period where animals were only exposed to the background 65-dB broad-band noise. Following this period, three blocks of trials were presented, in which trials consisted of either a startling tactile stimulus (50-ms air puff) alone (i.e., pulse-alone trial) or an acoustic prepulse stimulus (a broad-band noise of any of three possible intensities, 68, 71, or 77 dB) followed by the startling tactile stimulus (i.e., prepulse–pulse trial). In Block 1, 6 pulse-alone trials (tactile air-puff stimulus) were presented in order to habituate and stabilize the animals' startle responses. During Block 2, 48 trials were randomly presented, 12 pulse-alone and 36 prepulse–pulse trials (i.e., 12 prepulse–pulse trials for each prepulse intensity). Block 3 comprised an additional 5 pulse-alone trials. For all blocks, the intertrial interval varied randomly between 10 and 20 s. In prepulse–pulse trials, auditory prepulse stimuli had a 2-ms rise/fall time and a 20-ms duration, and the lead interval (time difference between the onsets of the prepulse and pulse stimuli) was 100 ms. Data from trials in Block 2 were used to determine percent PPI, which was calculated as the ratio of the difference in response amplitude in pulse-alone and prepulse–pulse trials to the startle response amplitude in pulse-alone trials, multiplied by 100. That is, PPI [%] = 100 × (pulse-alone startle response − prepulse–pulse startle response) / pulse-alone startle response. The entire test period (including the 10-min interval between drug administration and the beginning of the acclimation period) lasted approximately 34 min.

Drug administration

Drugs were administered in the form of cocktail solutions. Before the beginning of each study, all drug combinations (eight cocktails per experiment) were prepared by mixing the appropriate drug amounts in saline solutions. Ten minutes before the beginning of the acclimation period of each test session, drugs were injected subcutaneously in a 1-ml/kg volume of the corresponding drug combination to be analyzed. In “Experiment 1”, drug treatments consisted of combinations of amphetamine (amphetamine sulfate salt; 0 and 1 mg/kg as of salt weight) and idazoxan (idazoxan hydrochloride; 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg as of salt weight). In “Experiment 2”, combinations of dizocilpine (dizocilpine hydrogen maleate; 0 and 0.05 mg/kg as of maleate weight) and idazoxan (idazoxan hydrochloride; 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg as of salt weight) were administered. All drugs were procured from Sigma, St. Louis, Mo., USA, and drug combinations were prepared in 0.9% saline solution. Saline-alone injections (cocktails with 0 mg/kg doses of both drugs) were used as the control condition. In both experiments, the drug treatments were given using a repeated-measures counterbalanced (Latin-square) design, in order to (a) test startle responses randomly across the different phases of the estrous cycle, and (b) reduce the potential impact of drug carryover effects following repeated test sessions. In addition, test sessions were conducted two to three days apart to allow for complete drug washout between successive sessions.

Data analysis

Percent prepulse inhibition of the tactile startle response for the three acoustic prepulse intensities examined was determined for each drug combination. Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were used to assess the effects of the drugs and their interactions on PPI and on the startle response in pulse-alone trials. In “Experiment 1”, interactions between the dopamine agonist amphetamine and the α2-receptor antagonist idazoxan with regard to PPI and startle response amplitude were investigated. In “Experiment 2”, interactions between the NMDA-receptor antagonist dizocilpine and idazoxan were analyzed. The PPI analyses included the same factors than those employed for those of startle responses, with the addition of prepulse intensity. Significant interactions were followed up by tests of simple main effects comparing the impact of the individual drugs and combinations thereof using Fisher's LSD tests. Reduced degrees of freedom (Greenhouse–Geisser) were used when appropriate to offset violations of the sphericity assumption underlying repeated-measures ANOVA tests. Statistical analyses were performed using SuperANOVA (Abacus Concepts, Inc, Berkeley, CA, USA). A p value of 0.05 was used as the threshold for statistical significance.

Results

Experiment 1

The effects of amphetamine and idazoxan on PPI for the three different prepulse intensities analyzed are shown in Fig. 1. A 2 × 4 × 3 ANOVA (amphetamine, 0 and 1 mg/kg × idazoxan, 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg × prepulse intensity, 68, 71, and 77 dB) test on PPI levels revealed a significant main effect of prepulse intensity (F(2,22) = 30.24, p < 0.000005, ε = 0.9), a significant amphetamine × idazoxan interaction (F(3,33) = 3.16, p < 0.05, ε = 0.82), and a trend towards a significant amphetamine effect (F(1,11) = 3.71, p = 0.08), but no significant main effect of idazoxan (F(3,33) = 1.076, p > 0.37). Post hoc analyses indicated that 68-dB prepulses produced less overall inhibition (4.1 ± 3.1%, mean ± SEM) than 71-dB prepulses (18.5 ± 2.6%; p < 0.00005), which in turn caused less overall inhibition than 77-dB prepulses (25.1 ± 3.0%; p < 0.05). Amphetamine-only administration significantly reduced mean PPI (−4.1 ± 6.2%; p < 0.005) when compared to the control saline condition (24.7 ± 5.1%). This disruption in inhibition was effectively counteracted by the 1 mg/kg (14.7 ± 7.5%; p < 0.05) and 2 mg/kg (23.2 ± 4.6%; p < 0.005) idazoxan doses, which increased mean PPI values to levels that were not statistically significantly different from those in the control saline condition (p > 0.26 and p > 0.87, respectively). In addition, compared to the control condition, PPI was not significantly reduced after administration of idazoxan (0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg) without amphetamine (p > 0.86, p > 0.21, and p > 0.51, respectively). No significant interactions were found between prepulse intensity and amphetamine (p > 0.32) or idazoxan (p > 0.15).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Idazoxan interactions with amphetamine-induced PPI disruption by prepulse intensity. The control saline condition corresponds to the 0-mg/kg amphetamine–0-mg/kg idazoxan treatment. Amphetamine-only administration reduced PPI levels when compared to the control saline condition (p < 0.005), and both 1 and 2 mg/kg doses of idazoxan effectively counteracted that disruption (*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.005, respectively). These effects resulted in a significant amphetamine × idazoxan interaction (p < 0.05). Increasing prepulse intensity levels enhanced PPI (p < 0.000005), and no significant interactions were found between prepulse intensity and amphetamine and/or idazoxan. Data represent mean values (±SEM)

Figure 2 presents the effects of amphetamine and idazoxan on the magnitude of the tactile startle response in pulse-alone trials. A 2 × 4 ANOVA (amphetamine, 0 and 1 mg/kg × idazoxan, 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg) on the startle amplitude yielded significant main effects of amphetamine (F(1,11) = 31.52, p < 0.0002) and idazoxan (F(3,33) = 9.32, p < 0.002, ε = 0.65), indicating that both drugs enhanced overall responses. In addition, a significant interaction between amphetamine and idazoxan (F(3,33) = 4.98, p < 0.015, ε = 0.75) on the startle amplitude was found. Administration of 0.5 mg/kg idazoxan without amphetamine caused a significant increase in startle (377.9 ± 69.9) when compared to the control saline condition (258.3 ± 40.8; p < 0.05), whereas co-administration of amphetamine and 0.5, 1, or 2 mg/kg idazoxan resulted in larger startle amplitudes (524.0 ± 51.7; 499.4 ± 64.4; 461.9 ± 63.4; respectively) when compared to the amphetamine-only condition (214.2 ± 29.9; ps < 0.00005). For the conditions in which co-treatment of amphetamine and idazoxan resulted in increased levels of PPI (when compared to the amphetamine-only condition; i.e., amphetamine with 1 and 2 mg/kg idazoxan), correlation tests revealed no significant relationship between those changes in PPI and the observed increases in the amplitude of the startle response (t(10) = 0.14, p > 0.44 and t(10) = 1.09, p > 0.15, respectively). Furthermore, median split analyses including the effects on startle as a factor in the ANOVA tests on PPI yielded significant amphetamine × idazoxan interactions for the 1 and 2 mg/kg doses (F(1,10) = 4.9, p < 0.05 and F(1,10) = 5.1, p < 0.05, respectively) but no significant startle effect × amphetamine × idazoxan interactions (F(1,10) = 0.03, p > 0.85 and F(1,10) = 0.078, p > 0.78), suggesting that the drugs' effects on the startle magnitude were distinct from those observed on PPI.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effects of idazoxan and amphetamine interactions on the tactile startle response (mean ± SEM). The control saline condition corresponds to the 0-mg/kg amphetamine–0-mg/kg idazoxan treatment. Both amphetamine and idazoxan significantly affected the magnitude of the startle response (p < 0.0002 and p < 0.002, respectively). Compared to the control saline condition, 0.5 mg/kg of idazoxan caused an increase in response amplitude (*p < 0.05). Amphetamine co-administration with 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg doses of idazoxan enhanced the startle response when compared to the amphetamine-only condition (+ p < 0.00005). These effects resulted in a significant amphetamine × idazoxan interaction (p < 0.015)

Experiment 2

The effects of dizocilpine and idazoxan on PPI for the three different prepulse intensities examined are shown in Fig. 3. A 2 × 4 × 3 ANOVA (dizocilpine, 0 and 0.05 mg/kg × idazoxan, 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg × prepulse intensity, 68, 71, and 77 dB) test on PPI levels revealed significant main effects of dizocilpine (F(1,10) = 8.81, p < 0.015) and prepulse intensity (F(2,20) = 36.76, p < 0.000001, ε = 0.87), and significant dizocilpine × idazoxan (F(3,30) = 3.26, p < 0.05, ε = 0.82) and dizocilpine × prepulse intensity (F(2,20) = 8.21, p < 0.005, ε = 0.94) interactions. No significant main effect of idazoxan (F < 1) or dizocilpine × idazoxan × prepulse intensity interaction (F < 1) on PPI was found. Post hoc analyses indicated that 68-dB prepulses caused less overall inhibition (13.2 ± 2.8%) than 71-dB prepulses (23.3 ± 2.6%; p < 0.0015), which in turn produced less overall inhibition than 77-dB prepulses (41.2 ± 2.3%; p < 0.0001); and that dizocilpine significantly reduced overall PPI for 68-dB (p < 0.000001) and 71-dB (p < 0.00025) prepulses, but not for 77-dB prepulses (p > 0.2). Dizocilpine-only administration significantly reduced mean PPI (13.9 ± 4.8%) when compared to the control saline condition (33.9 ± 5.9%; p < 0.01). This disruption was effectively counteracted by the 2 mg/kg idazoxan dose (32.5 ± 3.7%; p < 0.015), which increased mean PPI to levels that were not statistically significantly different from those in the control saline condition (p > 0.83). When compared to the control condition, administration of idazoxan (0.5, 1 and 2 mg/kg) without dizocilpine did not cause a significant decrease in PPI (p > 0.71, p > 0.60, p > 0.55). No significant interaction between prepulse intensity and idazoxan (F < 1) was found.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Idazoxan interactions with dizocilpine-induced PPI disruption by prepulse intensity (mean ± SEM). The control saline condition corresponds to the 0-mg/kg dizocilpine–0-mg/kg idazoxan treatment. Dizocilpine significantly reduced PPI levels (p < 0.015). Dizocilpine-only administration reduced PPI levels when compared to the control saline condition (p < 0.01), and the 2-mg/kg dose of idazoxan effectively counteracted that disruption (# p < 0.015). These effects resulted in a significant dizocilpine × idazoxan interaction (p < 0.05). Increasing prepulse intensity levels enhanced PPI (p < 0.000001), and a significant dizocilpine × prepulse interaction (p < 0.005) revealed that dizocilpine disrupted inhibition more effectively for prepulses of lower intensity

Figure 4 presents the effects of dizocilpine and idazoxan on the magnitude of the tactile startle response in pulse-alone trials. A 2 × 4 ANOVA (dizocilpine, 0 and .05 mg/kg × idazoxan, 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 mg/kg) on the startle amplitude revealed a significant main effect of dizocilpine (F(1,10) = 15.89, p < 0.005). No significant main effect of idazoxan (F(3,30) = 1.7, p > 0.2, ε = 0.72) or an interaction between the drugs (F(3,30) = 2.13, p > 0.14, ε = 0.68) was found. Whereas co-administration of dizocilpine with 2 mg/kg idazoxan resulted in increased PPI levels (when compared to the dizocilpine-only condition), correlation tests revealed no significant relationship between the increase in PPI and the changes in the amplitude of the startle response in this condition (t(9) = 0.26, p > 0.39). In addition, median split analyses including the effects on startle as a factor in the ANOVA test on PPI yielded a significant dizocilpine × idazoxan interaction for the 2 mg/kg dose (F(1,9) = 8.5, p < 0.05), but no significant startle effect × dizocilpine × idazoxan interaction (F(1,9) = 0.076, p > 0.78), suggesting that the drugs' effect on the startle magnitude was distinct from that observed on PPI.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effects of idazoxan and dizocilpine interactions on the tactile startle response (mean ± SEM). Dizocilpine significantly increased the magnitude of the startle response (p < 0.005), but no significant main effect of idazoxan (p > 0.2) or an interaction between these drugs (p > 0.14) on the tactile startle response was found

Discussion

In agreement with previous findings using unimodal acoustic prepulse and acoustic startle stimuli (e.g., Mansbach et al. 1988; Sills 1999; Swerdlow et al. 2005), amphetamine-only administration in “Experiment 1” caused a significant decrease in acoustic PPI of the tactile startle response when compared to the control saline condition but had no effect on the magnitude of startle on pulse-alone trials (Sills 1999). Idazoxan administration did not have a significant effect on acoustic PPI by itself, but effectively counteracted the amphetamine-induced PPI disruption in a dose-dependent manner. Both the 1 and 2 mg/kg idazoxan doses significantly counteracted the amphetamine-induced PPI disruption, whereas the 0.5 mg/kg idazoxan dose did not significantly alter the amphetamine-induced PPI reduction. Only the lowest dose (0.5 mg/kg) of idazoxan tested in “Experiment 1” caused a significant increase in startle response in pulse-alone trials when compared to the control saline condition, whereas response amplitude did not significantly change relative to the control saline condition with the 1 and 2 mg/kg idazoxan doses. In addition, co-administration of amphetamine with all doses of idazoxan resulted in an increase in startle response compared to the amphetamine-only condition. Even when significant amphetamine × idazoxan interactions on PPI and startle magnitude were found on “Experiment 1”, the opposing effects of idazoxan on the amphetamine-induced PPI disruption by idazoxan do not seem to result as a consequence of the observed increase in startle response. In fact, co-administration of amphetamine with the lowest idazoxan dose (0.5 mg/kg) caused the largest increase in the startle response magnitude (see Fig. 2), but the amphetamine-induced PPI impairment was not counteracted in this condition (p > 0.15). Furthermore, correlation tests between increases in PPI and changes in the amplitude of the startle response for the cases in which co-treatment of amphetamine and idazoxan (1 and 2 mg/kg) resulted in increased PPI levels (with respect to the amphetamine-only condition) yielded no significant relationships between these factors (p > 0.44 and p > 0.15, respectively), indicating that idazoxan counteracts the amphetamine-induced sensorimotor gating deficit independently of its effects on the startle response magnitude.

A possible mechanism of action for the observed reversal of the PPI disruption by idazoxan may be through a change in dopaminergic transmission due to an interaction between α2 receptors and dopaminergic systems (Litman et al. 1996). Grenhoff et al. (1993) showed that idazoxan increased neural firing in the locus coeruleus (LC), modulating mesolimbic and mesocortical dopaminergic activity. Since reduced dopamine levels in the MPFC result in lower PPI values (Bubser and Koch 1994) it is possible that idazoxan counteracts the amphetamine-induced PPI disruption by increasing the levels of cortical dopamine (Hertel et al. 1999). In addition, marked increases in dopamine output in the MPFC are also caused by the antipsychotic clozapine (Moghaddam and Bunney 1990), which also effectively reverses PPI deficits (Bakshi et al. 1994; Swerdlow and Geyer 1993).

Results from “Experiment 2” showed that, in accord with previous reports, dizocilpine administration caused a significant decrease in PPI and increased startle responses (Mansbach and Geyer 1989; Roegge et al. 2007). A significant prepulse intensity × dizocilpine interaction revealed that the disruptive effects of dizocilpine on PPI were more marked for lower prepulse intensities. Idazoxan administration by itself did not significantly affect PPI. However the highest idazoxan dose tested (2 mg/kg) significantly attenuated the PPI impairment caused by dizocilpine. Neither of the lower idazoxan doses (05 or 1 mg/kg) significantly altered the dizocilpine-induced PPI impairment. In addition, a correlation test between increases in PPI and changes in the amplitude of the startle response for the dizocilpine with 2 mg/kg idazoxan condition (for which PPI levels increased with respect to the dizocilpine-only condition) yielded no significant relationships between these factors (p > 0.39), indicating that the counteracting effects of idazoxan on the dizocilpine-induced PPI deficit were independent of those observed in the startle response amplitude.

Previous studies have provided evidence showing the involvement of the MPFC in the dizocilpine-induced PPI disruption. Bakshi and Geyer (1998) found a trend toward a decrease in PPI after local infusion of dizocilpine in the MPFC, and Schwabe and Koch (2004) reported that dizocilpine failed to disrupt PPI in rats with ibotenic acid lesions in the MPFC. Idazoxan can increase the levels of cortical dopamine (Hertel et al. 1999), which may reduce dopamine release in the NAC by enhancing inhibition of the MPFC glutamatergic neurons that project to the VTA (Karreman and Moghaddam 1996), thus decreasing dopamine levels in the NAC (Koch 1999) and counteracting the dizocilpine-induced PPI disruption.

Studies analyzing the effects of α2-receptor antagonists on PPI disruptions induced by NMDA-receptor antagonists have yielded mixed results. For instance, Bakshi and Geyer (1997) showed that the α1-receptor antagonist prazosin, but not the α2-receptor antagonist RX821002, was able to reverse PCP-induced PPI deficits. In contrast, Palsson et al. (2008) showed that agmatine (a potent α2-adrenergic and imidazoline receptor antagonist) effectively reverses PCP disruptions of PPI. In addition, Ballmaier et al. (2001) analyzed the effects of a combination of idazoxan and the D2/D3 receptor antagonist raclopride on PCP-induced PPI deficits and found no reversal of the PPI disruption with those drugs. However, only one dose of idazoxan was used in their experiment (1.5 mg/kg), and the results of “Experiment 2” suggest that there may be a threshold dose for which the opposing effects of idazoxan on the dizocilpine-induced PPI deficit can be detected (i.e., 2 mg/kg). Therefore, that the lack of effect of idazoxan in reversing PPI deficits caused by NMDA-receptor antagonists reported by Ballmaier et al. (2001) might be attributed to the lower idazoxan dose used in their study. Finally, and in agreement with the results of “Experiment 2”, Sallinen et al. (2007) showed that JP-1302 (a selective α2C-adrenoceptor antagonist) effectively antagonized PCP-induced PPI deficits in rats.

In addition to its ability to restore amphetamine or dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits, idazoxan (as well as other α2-adrenergic antagonists) can reverse haloperidol-induced catalepsy effects in rats (Invernizzi et al. 2003; Kleven et al. 2005) and modulate the effects of amphetamine on locomotion and stereotypies in mice (Luttinger and Durivage 1986). These results seem to further validate the prospective use of α2-adrenergic antagonists for the treatment of some psychiatric conditions. Clozapine is a complex drug with antagonistic effects on several different neurotransmitter receptors, including histamine, serotonin, dopamine, norepinepherine, and muscarinic (Coward 1992), and therefore the study of the effects of these different neurotransmitters on animals models of schizophrenia (such as amphetamine- and dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits; Geyer et al. 2001) might help to elucidate their individual contributions for therapeutic treatments. The results of “Experiments 1” and “2” showing that idazoxan is able to restore both amphetamine- and dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits suggest that the previously reported effectiveness of clozapine in counteracting both apomorphine- (Swerdlow and Geyer 1993) and dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits (Bubenikova et al. 2005; Levin et al. 2007) may be related to clozapine's α2-receptor blockage properties. However, previous studies have also shown that histamine H1-receptor blockage can counteract dizocilpine- (Roegge et al. 2007) and amphetamine-induced PPI deficits (Larrauri and Levin 2010). Thus, it is possible that complex interactions among the different receptors bound by clozapine are responsible for its superior antipsychotic effects, and further studies will be needed to clarify them.

Besides a strong α2-adrenoceptor antagonist affinity (70–80% specific binding; Mallard et al. 1992), idazoxan has also weaker imidazoline-receptor antagonist properties. Thus, it is possible that the restoring effects of idazoxan observed in “Experiments 1” and “2” may be related to its antagonist action on imidazoline receptors. Though no studies have analyzed the specific effects of these receptors on PPI, the stronger specific α2-receptor antagonist affinity of idazoxan, as well as experimental results showing that specific α2C-antagonists can restore PCP-induced PPI deficits (Sallinen et al. 2007), suggest that the observed restoring effects of idazoxan on amphetamine- and dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits may related to the effects of idazoxan on α2-receptors.

A few methodological issues in this study deserve additional consideration. A cross-modal PPI paradigm was used because it allows the determination of the generality of neurotransmitter system involvement in sensorimotor plasticity. Changes in startle responses to auditory stimuli presented after auditory prepulses may arise as a consequence of repeated stimulation of the same sensory neural pathways. The cross-modal approach avoids this particular problem (Levin et al. 2007). The effects of the different pharmacological manipulations on PPI were studied on female rats because (a) women comprise a large percentage of the population with schizophrenia (42%; McGrath 2006), and (b) female rats have an advantage over males for long-term chronic studies inasmuch as their adult body weight stays relatively constant throughout adulthood whereas males continually increases (Bell and Zucker 1971). Also, sexual dimorphism in pharmacokinetics has been reported in rats with respect to amphetamine (female rats show increased locomotor activity and focal stereotypies; Milesi-Halle et al. 2007) and dizocilpine (longer-lasting behavioral changes in female animals; Honack and Loscher 1993). Studies comparing responses of male and female Sprague–Dawley rats have not found gender differences in PPI or startle amplitude (Swerdlow et al. 1993a), and similar effects of clozapine in reversing dizocilpine-induced deficits in PPI have been found in both male and female Sprague–Dawley rats (Bakshi et al. 1994; Levin et al. 2007). Even when female Sprague–Dawley rats show a decrease in PPI during proestrous when compared to estrous or diestrous (Koch 1998), the phase of the cycle becomes a random variable when using a counterbalanced design. The results of Koch (1998) also show that Sprague–Dawley males and females (diestrous or estrous) do not differ in terms of PPI. Concerning the carryover effects inherent of a repeated-measures design, the use of a counterbalanced design protects against potential carryover effects of the drugs. In addition, test sessions were conducted 2 to 3 days apart to allow for the drug effects to disappear between sessions.

In sum, our results show that the α2-adrenergic antagonist idazoxan can effectively attenuate the amphetamine- or dizocilpine-induced PPI disruption in rats. Since the antipsychotic drug clozapine also has antagonistic effects at this receptor type and has previously been shown to counteract both apomorphine- (Swerdlow and Geyer 1993) and dizocilpine-induced PPI deficits (Bubenikova et al. 2005; Levin et al. 2007), the results of our experiments suggest that the α2 antagonistic effects of clozapine may contribute to its efficacy in reversing PPI deficits. By individually analyzing the different systems that affect PPI, a better comprehension of the effects of atypical antipsychotics might be attained, ultimately leading to an improved understanding of sensorimotor gating deficits in neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia. Sensory over-responsiveness is observed in individuals with disorders that also entail PPI deficits (such as autism; Perry et al. 2007, or Asperger's syndrome; McAlonan et al. 2002), and can as well be seen without concomitant psychotic symptoms, such as the case with sensory processing disorder (Miller et al. 2009). In this group for which the full activity of a complex antipsychotic drug such as clozapine is not appropriate for treatment, identification of a more selective compound which effectively improves deficient sensory processing without undue side effects is essential.