Introduction

Given the substantial health risks associated with concurrent alcohol and tobacco use (Hurt et al. 1996; Rosengren et al. 1993; Blot et al. 1988; Klatsky and Armstrong 1992; Vaillant et al. 1991), there has been a growing effort to address smoking cessation in those experiencing alcohol problems. Most studies have been conducted with alcohol-dependent drinkers (Burling et al. 1991; Joseph et al. 1990; Hurt et al. 1994), and less is known about how to promote smoking cessation in non-dependent heavy drinkers who represent the majority of those with problems resulting from alcohol consumption (Institute of Medicine 1990). Moreover, little is known about what effects standard smoking cessation treatments have on alcohol consumption in non-dependent heavy drinking smokers. Alcohol use has been shown to promote smoking relapse across studies using different methodologies (Baer and Lichenstein 1988; McKee et al. 2006; Shiffman 1986; Shiffman et al. 1996; Zimmerman et al. 1990). One way in which smoking cessation medications could promote quitting behavior in this population is by reducing alcohol responses and consumption, thereby attenuating the ability of alcohol to prompt smoking relapse.

While a number of studies document that alcohol reliably increases smoking behavior (e.g., Glautier et al. 1996; Griffiths et al. 1976; Mintz et al. 1985; Mitchell et al. 1995; Shiffman et al. 1994) and potentiates nicotine reward (Rose et al. 2004), relatively fewer studies examine the impact of nicotine on alcohol self-administration. In preclinical studies, acute administrations of nicotine decreased alcohol self-administration behavior (Katner et al. 1997; Nadel et al. 1998), whereas chronic administration of nicotine increased alcohol intake in alcohol-experienced rats (Blomqvist et al. 1996; Clark et al. 2001; Lê et al. 2000; Smith et al. 1999; Soderpalm et al. 2000). Nicotine has also been shown to facilitate the acquisition of alcohol self-administration behavior in rats (Smith et al. 1999) and reinstate alcohol seeking behavior (Lê et al. 2003).

Human laboratory studies have examined the effect of smoked tobacco on alcohol use (Barrett et al. 2006; Madden et al. 1995; Mello et al. 1980, 1987) as well as the effect of nicotine deprivation on the reinforcing value of alcohol (Colby et al. 2004; Cooney et al. 2003; Palfai et al. 2000; Perkins et al. 2000a). Fewer studies have tested the effect of nicotine replacement on subjective alcohol responses (Cooney et al. 2001; Kouri et al. 2004; Perkins et al. 1995) and alcohol self-administration behavior (Acheson et al. 2006). Cooney et al. (2001) found that in a sample of abstinent alcoholics who had quit smoking, those receiving nicotine patch had lower cravings for alcohol compared to those not receiving nicotine patch. Perkins et al. (1995) examined the acute effects of fixed doses of alcohol (0, 0.5 g/kg) and nicotine nasal spray (0, 1.3 mg/kg) on subjective intoxication and cardiovascular responses in daily smokers who were light to moderate drinkers after overnight abstinence. Nicotine was found to attenuate the effect of alcohol on intoxication ratings and produced additive effects with regard to cardiovascular responses. Kouri et al. (2004) examined the effect of transdermal nicotine patch (21 mg/day), compared to overnight nicotine deprivation, on responses to acute doses of alcohol (0.4, 0.7 mg/kg) in moderate to heavy-drinking daily-smoking males. Nicotine and alcohol additively increased heart rate response, similar to findings produced by others (Benowitz et al. 1986; Michel and Battig 1989; Perkins et al. 1995). However, in contrast to findings produced by Perkins et al. (1995), nicotine was found to increase ratings of subjective intoxication, alcohol craving, and tobacco craving.

The only study to examine the effect of nicotine replacement on alcohol self-administration behavior did so in light-smoking social drinkers. Acheson et al. (2006) examined the effect of transdermal nicotine replacement (0, 7, 14 mg/day) on responses to an initial priming drink (0.2 g/kg) and subsequent alcohol self-administration behavior in light-smoking social drinkers (one to ten cigarettes per day; at least four drinks per week). The 2-h self-administration procedure allowed subjects to purchase up to eight additional beverages (0.1 g/kg each), using an individually determined monetary amount, equivalent to a standard drink (see Young et al. 2005). Testing occurred in small groups, and results demonstrated that the priming drink increased heart rate and ratings of desire for additional alcohol regardless of nicotine condition. However, during the self-administration phase, nicotine replacement increased drinking behavior in men, but decreased drinking behavior in women.

Based on these findings, the relationship between nicotine replacement, alcohol responses, and self-administration behavior is complex and appears to be influenced by the degree of experience with alcohol and nicotine, type and dose of nicotine replacement, dose of alcohol, gender, and availability of alternate reinforcers. For the current study, we were interested in examining the effect of transdermal nicotine replacement on subjective and physiological responses to a low-dose priming drink of alcohol and subsequent self-administration behavior in a sample of heavy-drinking daily smokers. While alcohol–nicotine interactions have been documented in lighter users (e.g., Barrett et al. 2006), it has been suggested that the nature of these interactions may be more pronounced in those with heavier use (e.g., Henningfield et al. 1984; Mello et al. 1987). We enrolled individuals with heavier patterns of alcohol and cigarette use (15–25 cigarettes per day; at least nine drinks per week for females; at least 12 drinks per week for males) than the Acheson et al. (2006). Similar to the Acheson et al. (2006) we used an alcohol self-administration paradigm that has demonstrated sensitivity for detecting medication effects (see O’Malley et al. 2002), that includes money as an alternative reinforcer to provide some incentive for not drinking and to enhance the likelihood that the effects of nicotine on the relative reinforcing value of alcohol would be detected (see Higgins 1997; Rodefer et al. 1997).

Heavy-drinking daily smokers completed two laboratory sessions (0, 21 mg/day transdermal nicotine patch) in which they were first exposed to a low-dose alcohol drink [designed to raise blood alcohol levels (BALs) to 0.03 g/dl and prime drinking behavior] followed by a 2-h self-administration period where subjects could choose to consume up to eight additional drinks (each designed to raise BALs by 0.015 g/dl) or to receive monetary reinforcement for drinks not consumed. Drinking topography and the number of drinks consumed during the self-administration period were our primary outcome measures. Secondary measures included alcohol and tobacco craving (Burton and Tiffany 1997; Cooney et al. 2001; Drobes et al. 2000: Mintz et al. 1991), subjective alcohol effects (Madden et al. 1995; Perkins et al. 1995), cardiovascular responses (Kouri et al. 2004; Perkins et al. 1995), and blood alcohol levels (to confirm drinking behavior). Given that nicotine replacement has been shown to attenuate responses to tobacco, and in some cases alcohol [e.g., Acheson et al. 2006 (females only); Cooney et al. 2001; Perkins et al. 1995], we predicted that transdermal nicotine replacement would reduce drinking behavior in heavy-drinking daily smokers. In light of the results of Acheson et al. (2006), we also examined the effect of gender on our primary outcome.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were eligible to enroll if they were 21 to 55 years of age, smoked between 15 and 25 cigarettes per day, drank alcohol at least 3 days per week, and drank at least four drinks per episode for men and at least three drinks per episode for women. Subjects were excluded if they were alcohol-dependent, using illicit drugs, currently seeking treatment for alcohol use or smoking behavior, presented with current severe psychiatric disorders, or had medical conditions that would contraindicate alcohol use or transdermal nicotine use. A total of 19 subjects completed the study (8 women, 11 men). The average age was 26.68 (SD = 9.01), and participants were primarily white (61%; 39% African American), had at least some college education (68%; 16% high school graduates; 16% some high school), and were not married (89%). Participants smoked, on average, 18.98 (SD = 4.13) cigarettes per day, had baseline carbon monoxide readings of 19.95 ppm (SD = 6.96), baseline urine cotinine values of 1079.85 ng/ml (SD = 645.79), and average Fagerstrom nicotine dependence scores (Heatherton et al. 1991) of 5.16 (SD = 1.50; possible range 1–10). Participants consumed an average of 24.04 (SD = 8.67) standard drinks per week, drank an average of 4.05 (SD = 1.23) times per week, had average Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (Babor et al. 1992) scores of 10.63 (SD = 3.32; scores of 8 or greater indicate problematic drinking), and 21% met DSM-IV criteria for current alcohol abuse.

Procedures

Intake sessions

Written informed consent was obtained at the start of the intake session. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al. 1995) was used to exclude individuals who met diagnostic criteria for alcohol dependence or other Axis I disorders. The Timeline Followback (Sobell and Sobell 1993) was used to assess past 30-day drinking and smoking behavior. A physical examination was conducted in addition to an electrocardiogram, urine toxicology, pregnancy tests for women, and basic blood chemistries including liver function tests.

Laboratory sessions

Each subject completed two 14-h laboratory sessions (0 vs 21 mg transdermal nicotine patch), which took place at the Yale General Clinical Research Center. The order of sessions was counterbalanced. For women, laboratory sessions were scheduled during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle to control for potential menstrual cycle effects (e.g., Perkins et al. 2000b). The average time between laboratory sessions was 24.37 days (SD = 13.97), and this interval did not differ across gender. Subjects were paid $120 to complete each laboratory session and received a $50 bonus for completing the study.

Baseline assessment period

Laboratory sessions started at 8:00 a.m. Subjects were asked to not consume alcohol 24 h before the session but were free to smoke as they normally would up until 8:00 a.m. of the morning of the laboratory session. An IV cannulae was inserted to obtain blood samples throughout the laboratory procedure. Baseline assessments of breath CO (MCO2 Monitor, MicroDirect, Auburn, ME, USA), breath alcohol (Alco-Sensor III, Intoximeter, St. Louis, MI, USA), physiologic measures (blood pressure, heart rate, skin temperature), plasma cotinine and nicotine levels, urine drug screen, urine pregnancy screen, height, and weight were obtained. All breath alcohol, pregnancy, and drug screens were required to be negative. Baseline CO readings indicated that all subjects had smoked before the laboratory sessions (CO ≥ 8 ppm).

Nicotine patch absorption period

At 9:00 a.m., a nicotine patch (0 mg or 21 mg Nicoderm CQ Transdermal System®, GlaxoSmithKline Consumer Healthcare) was applied to the subject’s upper arm. Both active and placebo patches were identical in appearance, and subjects and experimenters were blind as to the nicotine patch condition. The absorption period was 6 h so as to achieve peak nicotine levels for the active nicotine condition (Gupta et al. 1993). This time period also allowed for clearance of nicotine obtained through smoking before the laboratory session for the placebo nicotine condition. For the 6 h, subjects were able to watch TV and read. Lunch was provided at 12:00 p.m. to standardize the time and amount of last food intake. Assessments of side effects, plasma nicotine, nicotine withdrawal, alcohol craving, tobacco craving, and physiologic measures were obtained at the end of this period. Side effects associated with nicotine patch (i.e., skin irritation around patch, heart rate greater than 100, dizziness, nausea, and abdominal discomfort) were assessed on five-point scale (absent, minimal, mild, moderate, severe). There were no reports of skin irritation, nausea, or abdominal discomfort. There was one event of heart rate >100 (placebo patch group), and three reports of dizziness (minimal or mild ratings; two in placebo group, one in nicotine group).

Priming dose

The alcohol priming drink was administered at 3:00 p.m. and consisted of one part 80-proof liquor of the subject’s choosing to three parts mixer chosen from a selection of equicaloric, noncaffeinated, non-carbonated drinks. The amount of alcohol was designed to raise blood alcohol levels to 0.03 g/dl and was based on a formula that takes into account the gender, age, and weight of each subject (Watson 1989). This priming dose approximates a single standard alcoholic drink and has been successfully used to prime further drinking behavior (see O’Malley et al. 2002). Subjects were instructed to consume the beverage within 5 min. Blood alcohol level, alcohol craving, tobacco craving, and physiologic measures were assessed 15 min before and 10, 20, 30, and 40 min after the consumption of the priming drink in the order listed. Subjective effects of alcohol were assessed at baseline and 20 and 40 min after priming drink consumption. Using this procedure, we were able to monitor changes in subjective and physiological responses to alcohol during the ascending limb and the beginning of the descending limb of the blood alcohol curve.

Alcohol self-administration

The self-administration procedures were similar to those used in our previous study (see O’Malley et al. 2002). Fifty minutes after the priming drink, subjects were exposed to two consecutive, 1-h ad-lib drinking periods (hour 1 started at +50 min and ended at +110 min; hour 2 started at +120 min and ended at +180 min). During each 1-h period, subjects were permitted to drink up to four alcoholic drinks each designed to raise BALS by 0.015 g/dl or to receive monetary reinforcement ($3 per drink) for each drink not consumed. At the beginning of each 1-h choice period, subjects were presented with four prepared drinks along with a “tab” sheet worth $12. Subjects were informed that these four drinks were available for the next 60 min and that they could choose to consume the beverages or to keep the money (up to $12). At the end of the first hour, any remaining drinks were removed before the start of the second drinking hour in which four additional drinks (and $12 tab) were presented. Thus, subjects could choose to consume up to eight additional drinks over a 2-h period or to receive up to $24. Any unspent drinking tab was paid to the subjects at the end of each laboratory session.

During the two 1-h drinking periods, assessments of blood alcohol levels, alcohol craving, and tobacco craving were collected at the following timepoints in the order listed [+40 min (collected just before the start of the first drinking hour); hour 1 timepoints +80 min, +110 min; hour 2 timepoints +150 min, +180 min]. Subjective ratings of alcohol effects, nicotine withdrawal, and physiologic measures were collected before the start of the first drinking hour and end of each 1-h drinking period (timepoints +40 min, +110 min; +180 min). Drinking behavior was videotaped for later analysis. The timing of assessments was limited to avoid interfering with the evaluation of drinking behavior. Subjects were discharged at 10:00 p.m. at which time their breath alcohol levels had fallen below 0.02%.

Outcome measures

Subjective measures

The Alcohol Urge Questionnaire (AUQ; Bohn et al. 1995) was used to assess alcohol craving. This eight-item measure was designed to assess an individual’s desire to drink alcohol right now [Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), range 1–100). Tobacco craving was assessed with the Tiffany Questionnaire of Smoking Urges–Brief (QSU-Brief; Cox et al. 2001), which consists of ten items to evaluate urges to smoke in response to positive (factor 1) or negative (factor 2) reinforcement (VAS scale, range 1–100). The Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (BAES; Martin et al. 1993) is a 14-item self-report, unipolar adjective rating scale used to measure the stimulant and sedative effects of alcohol (VAS scale, range 1–100). The Alcohol Effects Scale (AES) assessed subjective alcohol effects with five items (high, like, rush, feel-good, intoxicated). Participants indicated on a visual analogue scale (range 1–100) how much of an alcohol effect they were experiencing. This scale was adopted from others found in the literature (Schuckit 1984). A mean score of these five items was calculated. DSM-IV symptoms of nicotine withdrawal were assessed with the eight-item Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale (MNWS; Hughes and Hatsukami 1986). Instructions were worded to assess current symptoms of withdrawal (range 0–32).

Physiologic measures

Blood pressure (systolic and diastolic), heart rate, and skin temperature were assessed using a Dinamap oscillometric monitor (Critikon, Tampa, FL, USA). For each timepoint assessed, two measurements were obtained and averaged.

Nicotine plasma levels

Plasma nicotine was measured by reversed-phase high-performance liquid chromatography with UV detection modified from the literature (Hariharan et al. 1988) to include a micro acid back extraction cleanup step, which allows for cleaner chromatograms. The lower limit of quantitation for nicotine was set to 4 ng/ml.

Blood alcohol levels

Blood samples for BALs taken during the laboratory session were collected in 400-μl microfuge tubes coated with Heparin. After microfuging, the plasma was transferred to microtubes and stored at −20°C. Samples were analyzed within 2 months using an AM1 analyser (Analox Instruments LTD, London, England). BALs were collected as a confirmation of drinking behavior and were not intended for pharmacokinetic analyses.

Drinking behavior

Videotaped drinking behavior was scored for each of the two drinking hours to determine the percent of subjects remaining abstinent, number of drinks consumed, latency to start drinking, number of sips per drink, inter-sip interval per drink, and inter-drink interval (i.e., time between the end of one drink and the start of another, within hour). Latency to start drinking was the number of minutes until the first sip during a given hour of the self-administration phase. Data for subjects who did not drink were handled in two ways. First, abstainers within a given hour were given a score of 60 min on latency to the first sip. In the second approach, these subjects were treated as missing for analyses of latency to the first drink.

Statistical analysis

Data analyses were conducted separately for the nicotine patch absorption period, the priming drink period, and the self-administration period. At the end of the 6-h nicotine patch absorption period, paired t tests were used to examine nicotine plasma levels, subjective measures (AUQ, QSU-B, MNWS), and physiologic measures (blood pressure, heart rate, skin temperature) within nicotine patch condition (0 vs 21 mg). Separate repeated measures multivariate analyses of variance were conducted for the priming drink and self-administration periods to examine BALs, subjective measures (AUQ, QSU-B, MNWS, AES, BAES), and physiologic measures within nicotine patch condition (0 vs 21 mg) and within time.

For the self-administration period, repeated measures analysis of variance (Hour 1, Hour 2) were conducted to examine the primary outcomes of drinking behavior (number of drinks consumed, latency to start drinking, sips per drink, inter-sip interval, and inter-drink interval) within nicotine patch conditions (0 vs 21 mg). Paired comparisons of available data were conducted within each hour by nicotine patch conditions. We also examined percentages of subjects remaining abstinent across each hour by patch conditions using McNemar tests of dependent proportions. Further, we examined gender and order of nicotine patch condition as between subject factors in our analyses involving drinking behavior. We found no significant effects of gender or order on our primary outcomes.

Results

Nicotine patch absorption period

Across the two laboratory sessions, subjects had equivalent baseline nicotine plasma levels at the start of the laboratory sessions (p = 0.48; 0 mg nicotine patch mean = 9.66 ng/ml, SE = 0.75; 21 mg nicotine patch = 9.49 ng/ml, SE = 1.11). At the end of the 6-h absorption period, as expected, active patch demonstrated greater nicotine levels [t(18) = 14.57, p < 0.0005; 0 mg patch mean = 0.54 ng/ml, SE = 0.37; 21 mg patch mean = 18.58 ng/ml, SE = 1.48]. Additionally, active patch decreased QSU-B factor 2 scores [t(18) = 2.25, p < 0.05; 0 mg patch mean = 26.89, SE = 6.10; 21 mg patch mean = 16.72, SE = 4.37], increased measures of systolic blood pressure [t(18) = 3.23, p < 0.01; 0 mg patch mean = 115.16 mmHg, SE = 3.83; 21 mg patch mean = 124.45 mmHg, SE = 3.78], and increased heart rate [t(18) = 3.24, p < 0.01; 0 mg patch mean = 68.66 bpm, SE = 2.37; 21 mg patch mean = 75.68 bpm, SE = 2.79]. Diastolic blood pressure (mean = 66.75 mmHg), skin temperature (mean = 85.84), AUQ scores (mean = 27.08), QSU-B factor 1 scores (mean = 54.43), and nicotine withdrawal scores (mean = 0.63) did not significantly differ.

Priming drink period

Blood alcohol levels

BALs increased over time (F = 135.27, p < .0005; +10 timepoint mean = .0148, SE = .002; +20 timepoint mean = .0224, SE = .002; +30 timepoint mean = .0218, SE = .002; +40 timepoint mean = .0203, SE = .002) and did not differ by patch condition.

Subjective measures

AUQ scores increased over the priming drink period (F = 14.97, p < 0.001; −15 min mean = 27.08, +40 min mean = 36.81), but there was no significant interaction of patch condition by time (p = 0.08). There were significant interactions of patch condition by time for tobacco craving for positive reinforcement (QSU-B factor 1; F = 8.08, p < 0.01) and negative reinforcement (QSU-B factor 2; F = 2.72, p < 0.04]. Active patch decreased tobacco craving for positive reinforcement 10 min after the consumption of alcohol, as evidenced by paired comparisons (0 mg patch mean = 67.65, SE = 7.44; 21 mg patch mean = 58.27, SE = 7.89; p < 0.05). Active patch also decreased ratings of tobacco craving for negative reinforcement before the consumption of alcohol and continued to be lower 10 min (0 mg patch mean = 33.70, SE = 6.63; 21 mg patch mean = 20.11, SE = 4.75; p < 0.05) and 20 min (0 mg patch mean = 32.44, SE = 6.67; 21 mg patch mean = 17.37, SE = 4.49; p < 0.05) after the consumption of the priming drink. Given the pre-alcohol differences in QSU-B factor 2 craving scores, we compared patch conditions using change scores calculated as differences from the −15-min timepoint. Change in QSU-B factor 2 craving scores were not significantly different across patch conditions at the +10- and +20-min timepoints.

Placebo patch increased ratings of alcohol effects (mean score of high, like, rush, feel-good, intoxicated) after the consumption of alcohol (patch × time; F = 7.39, p < 0.02; Fig. 1) with paired comparisons demonstrating differences at +20 and +40 min. BAES stimulation scores decreased over the priming drink period (F = 5.20, p < 0.04; 0 min mean = 1.77, +40 min mean = 1.39). BAES sedation scores did not differ. Nicotine withdrawal scores increased over the priming drink period (F = 9.89, p < 0.01; −15 min mean = 0.63, +40 min mean = 0.82) and did not differ by patch condition.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean visual analogue scale ratings (VAS 1–100) for AES during the priming drink period for each transdermal nicotine replacement condition (0 vs 21 mg/day). AES is a mean score of high, like, rush, feel-good, intoxicated. *p < 0.05 paired comparisons for 0 mg vs 21 mg/day nicotine patch within a timepoint

Physiologic measures

Systolic blood pressure decreased over the priming drink period (F = 10.60, p < 0.01, +0 min mean = 119.74 mmHg, +40 min mean = 115.02 mmHg). Diastolic blood pressure demonstrated a significant quadratic effect of time (F = 9.02, p < 0.01), with values increasing 10 min post-alcohol consumption (+10 min mean = 69.78 mmHg) and then decreasing over the priming drink period (+40 min mean = 63.44 mmHg). Active patch increased heart rate (F = 6.40, p < 0.05; 0 mg patch mean = 71.55 bpm, SE = 2.76; 21 mg patch mean = 77.01, SE = 2.67). Heart rate also increased over the priming drink period (F = 10.98, p < 0.01; 0 min mean = 73.19 bpm, +40 min mean = 75.82 bpm). Active nicotine patch decreased skin temperature (patch × time; F = 5.93, p < 0.03) 10 min after the consumption of the priming drink (0 mg patch mean = 88.61, SE = 0.87; 21 mg patch mean = 85.62, SE = 1.60).

Self-administration period

Drinking behavior

While slightly more subjects remained abstinent during the active (Hour 1 n = 5; Hour 2 n = 6) vs placebo patch condition (Hour 1 n = 2; Hour 2 n = 3), across each hour, the differences in proportions were not significant (see Table 1). The active patch increased the latency to start drinking (F = 6.43, p < 0.02) across the 2-h self-administration period when latencies were coded as 60 min for subjects who abstained from drinking during a given hour of the self-administration period. In the subset of subjects who drank, patch condition did not alter latency to drink, number of sips, inter-sip interval, or inter-drink interval. Drinking behavior did not differ between Hour 1 and Hour 2, as there were no significant multivariate effects of time found for any of the drinking measures. As a probable consequence of delayed drinking onset, the total number of drinks consumed during the 2-h self-administration period was significantly lower in the active patch compared to the placebo patch condition (F = 9.80, p < 0.01).

Table 1 Drinking behavior measures [% or mean (SE)] collected during the 2-h self-administration phase for transdermal nicotine replacement conditions (0 vs 21 mg/day)

We also conducted exploratory analyses to examine potential predictors of significant drinking effects (i.e., mean latency to drink, and drinks consumed). For those variables which demonstrated some indication of nicotine patch effects during the priming drink period (i.e., alcohol craving, tobacco craving, alcohol effects, skin temperature with p < 0.10), their +40 min values (end of the priming drink period) were correlated with average latency to drink and total drinks consumed across both hours within each patch condition. Alcohol craving was positively associated with drinks consumed (0 mg patch r = 0.48, p < 0.05; 21 mg patch r = 0.65, p < 0.01) and negatively associated with latency to drink (0 mg patch r = −0.49, p < 0.05; 21 mg patch r = −0.46, p < 0.05) for both active and placebo patch conditions. For the placebo patch condition only, tobacco craving for positive (r = −0.65, p < 0.01) and negative reinforcement (r = −0.53, p < 0.05) were negatively associated with latency to drink. No significant effects were demonstrated for alcohol effects or skin temperature.

Blood alcohol levels

Compared to the placebo patch condition, subjects in the active patch had lower blood alcohol levels at certain timepoints (condition × time interaction; F = 3.55, p < 0.01), consistent with the increased latency to begin consuming alcohol after nicotine. Paired comparisons of mean BALs for active and placebo patch indicate that BALs were lower halfway through each of the drinking hours [midpoint of the first drinking hour (+80 timepoint: 0 mg patch mean = 0.034 mg%, SE = 0.004; 21 mg patch mean = 0.024 mg% SE = 0.004); midpoint of the second drinking hour (+150 timepoint: 0mg patch mean = 0.050 mg%, SE = 0.008; 21 mg patch mean = 0.034 mg%, SE = 0.008)], but that BALs did not differ by patch condition at other times [before the start of the first drinking hour (+40 timepoint mean = 0.020 mg%), end of the first drinking hour (+110 timepoint mean = 0.034 mg%), or at the end of the second drinking hour (+180 timepoint mean = 0.050 mg%)].

Subjective measures

Alcohol craving (AUQ: F = 6.16, p < 0.03) decreased, whereas alcohol effect scores (AES: F = 5.12, p < 0.04) increased over the ad-lib period (see Table 2). Given the differences in AES scores between the patch conditions before the self-administration phase, we analyzed AES change scores calculated as the difference from the +40 min timepoint by patch condition. The time effect for AES was no longer significant. Tobacco craving for positive reinforcement (QSU-B factor 1 scores; F = 8.86, p < 0.01) and negative reinforcement (QSU-B factor 2 scores; F = 12.19, p < 0.01) increased over the ad-lib period. Nicotine withdrawal scores did not significantly differ. The active patch increased BAES stimulation scores by the end of the 2-h drinking period (patch × time; F = 4.57, p < 0.05).

Table 2 Mean (SE) subjective and physiological effects during the alcohol self-administration phase for transdermal nicotine replacement conditions (0 vs 21 mg/day)

Physiologic measures

Active patch increased heart rate (F = 4.81, p < 0.05). Heart rate also increased over the 2-h drinking period (F = 7.74, p < 0.02), whereas skin temperature decreased (F = 7.51, p < 0.02). Blood pressure did not significantly differ.

Discussion

Using an established alcohol self-administration paradigm (Anton et al. 2004; O’Malley et al. 2002), we found that transdermal nicotine replacement increased the latency to the first drink and consequently appeared to reduce the number of drinks consumed during a 2-h self-administration period, compared to 6 h of nicotine deprivation (i.e., placebo patch), in heavy drinking daily smokers. Although not significantly different, during the active patch condition, a larger proportion of subjects declined all drinks within each hour. When drinks were chosen, however, the number of sips taken per drink, time between sips, and time between drinks were similar for the active and placebo conditions. Thus, we conclude that active nicotine replacement compared to placebo replacement delayed the onset of consumption. Although subjects on nicotine patch compared to placebo patch consumed fewer drinks in the time allotted, it is not possible to determine whether this effect would be maintained, enhanced, or reduced if the time period in which drinks could be chosen was not constrained.

Our drinking outcomes are consistent with Acheson et al. (2006) findings with women. Although Acheson et al. (2006) did not evaluate latency to drink, our results regarding reduced drinking (which are secondary to latency to drink) were similar. However, their study found that nicotine patch increased alcohol consumption in males, whereas we found that nicotine patch also reduced drinking by men. A number of procedural differences between the two studies may account for the differential findings. Acheson et al. (2006) examined light-smoking social drinkers and subjects were tested in small groups, whereas our subjects were heavier drinkers and smokers and were tested individually. In the Acheson et al. (2006) study, the self-administration phase commenced while the blood alcohol levels produced by the priming drink were ascending, while our self-administration phase began when the blood alcohol levels were descending. Alcohol–nicotine interactions have been shown to be sensitive to ascending and descending limb effects (King and Epstein 2005; McKee et al. 2006; Mitchell et al. 1995), and others have demonstrated that medication effects are sensitive to the interval between the priming drink and the self-administration phase (Anton et al. 2004). Additional work is needed to delineate how nicotine replacement interacts with temporal effects of the priming drink on inhibiting or promoting further alcohol consumption.

In the period before the priming drink, we found that active nicotine patch in comparison to 6 h of nicotine deprivation (i.e., placebo patch) attenuated tobacco craving for negative reinforcement, consistent with prior research (e.g., Tiffany et al. 2000). We also found increases in systolic blood pressure and heart rate consistent with the effects of transdermal nicotine replacement on cardiovascular responses (Zevin et al. 1998). Nicotine withdrawal scores, as measured by the MNWS, were minimal after 6 h of cigarette deprivation and were not influenced by nicotine patch condition (see Pickworth et al. 1996).

Our results suggest that treatment with the nicotine patch alters subjective and physiological responses to a standard dose of alcohol. Specifically, after the priming drink, the subjective effects of alcohol (e.g., intoxication ratings) were lower when subjects received active compared to placebo transdermal nicotine replacement. After consuming the priming drink, heart rate was increased and skin temperature was decreased by active nicotine patch. These results are consistent with prior studies which have examined the single and combined effects of alcohol and nicotine on cardiovascular responses (Benowitz et al. 1986; Michel and Battig 1989; Perkins et al. 1995). Pre-alcohol differences in tobacco craving persisted with transdermal nicotine replacement attenuating craving for both positive and negative reinforcement. These findings are counter to those produced by Kouri et al. (2004) after overnight tobacco abstinence. It is difficult to make cross-study comparisons, as Kouri et al. (2004) used stronger alcohol priming doses (0.4, 0.7 g/kg) and removed subjects who failed to have craving responses (2 subjects of 12 total subjects) from the analyses. However, others have demonstrated that nicotine replacement and smoked tobacco resulted in lowered intoxication ratings (Madden et al. 1995; Perkins et al. 1995) and lowered tobacco craving (Mintz et al. 1991) after a fixed dose of alcohol.

During the alcohol self-administration period, alcohol craving was found to decrease, while craving for tobacco increased irrespective of patch condition. Active nicotine patch increased stimulation scores as well as heart rate during this period. Others have found that nicotine replacement can attenuate some of the sedating effects produced by alcohol (Perkins et al. 1995).

We also conducted exploratory analyses to investigate potential mechanisms underlying any alterations in drinking behavior. Alcohol craving at the end of the priming drink period was negatively associated with latency to consume alcohol and positively associated with the number of drinks consumed in both the nicotine and placebo patch conditions, supporting prior findings (O’Malley et al. 2002). Additionally, we found that higher tobacco craving for both positive and negative reinforcement was associated with reduced latency to consume alcohol in the placebo patch condition, but not in the nicotine patch condition. Thus, after 6 h of nicotine deprivation, higher tobacco craving responses seen in the placebo patch condition during the priming drink period may underlie the shorter latency to consume alcohol. Similarly, Epstein et al. (2007) found that tobacco craving for positive reinforcement was associated with blood alcohol levels. While our findings are suggestive of possible mechanisms underlying the initiation of drinking behavior, it should be noted that given our limited sample size, we were unable to conduct a full mediational analysis (see Baron and Kenny 1986).

This study has several limitations. First, the sample size was modest and only generalizeable to the population of heavy-drinking daily smokers. Although our sample size was comparable to other laboratory studies examining the effect of nicotine replacement on alcohol responses (e.g., Acheson et al. 2006; Barrett et al. 2006; Perkins et al. 1995, 2000a), our analyses for gender comparisons were underpowered. Second, only a single priming dose was investigated. However, we have found that an alcohol dose designed to raise BALs by 0.03 g/dl is effective in priming drinking behavior (see O’Malley et al. 2002; Krishnan-Sarin et al. 2007). In the current study, we demonstrated that this priming dose was sensitive to the effects of transdermal nicotine on at least delaying further drinking behavior. Third, no alternative beverage was provided during the self-administration phase, and it is unknown whether reductions in alcohol consumption during the transdermal nicotine replacement condition were specific to alcohol. Fourth, only a single dose of transdermal nicotine replacement was investigated. However, the 21-mg/day dose is the dose of transdermal nicotine replacement most commonly used and most likely to be used by heavy drinkers attempting smoking cessation. Nonetheless, from a mechanistic standpoint, it would be worthwhile to conduct a dose-ranging study of transdermal nicotine replacement in this population of heavy-drinking smokers. Further, we did not include a placebo alcohol condition. As a result, we cannot determine whether the observed effects were a product of transdermal nicotine replacement or transdermal nicotine replacement interacting with alcohol. However, our results for the priming drink period (i.e., cardiovascular responses, subjective alcohol effects) were consistent with the only prior study to incorporate an active and placebo alcohol dose to examine the effect of nicotine replacement on responses to a fixed dose of alcohol (Perkins et al. 1995). Finally, the duration of the session limited our ability to determine whether nicotine also decreases alcohol consumption once it has been initiated or whether the primary effect of nicotine is to delay the initiation of drinking.

Given the substantial health risks associated with concurrent alcohol and tobacco use (Rosengren et al. 1993), addressing smoking cessation in those experiencing alcohol problems is an important area for research. Although some studies have found no differences in the ability to quit smoking on the basis of alcohol problems (e.g., Hayford et al. 1999; Hughes et al. 2003; Hurt et al. 1995; Sobell et al. 1995), many studies demonstrate that individuals with current or past histories of alcohol problems are less likely to be successful at quitting smoking compared to those without alcohol problems (e.g., Daeppen et al. 2000; Di Franza and Guerrera 1990; Hughes 1995). However, little is known about how to best promote smoking cessation in this population of drinkers who are at increased risk for alcohol-mediated tobacco relapse (Baer and Lichenstein 1988; Brandon et al. 1990; Sobell et al. 1990; Shiffman et al. 1994, 1996; Zimmerman et al. 1990).

In summary, we found that transdermal nicotine, compared to 6 h of nicotine deprivation, reduced subjective and physiological responses to a single alcohol drink, increased latency to initiate drinking, and consequently appeared to decrease the amount of alcohol subsequently consumed. It is possible that nicotine reduced the effects of nicotine deprivation on drinking. Some studies find that nicotine deprivation increases drinking behavior (e.g., Palfai et al. 2000), and theories of behavioral choice (Vuchinich and Tucker 1988) suggest that nicotine deprivation may increase the incentive value for alcohol because of the lack of availability of nicotine. In support of this hypothesis, subjects who reported greater tobacco craving in the placebo patch condition exhibited shorter latencies to drink. In addition, because alcohol consumption is a known precipitant of smoking relapse, it is conceivable that nicotine replacement could further facilitate smoking cessation by reducing or delaying drinking. Future laboratory and clinical studies should examine the effect of other smoking cessation pharmacotherapies on alcohol drinking to better understand the value of these treatments for this population of treatment-resistant smokers.