Introduction

It is well established that healthy subjects undergo age-related decline in cognition (Robbins et al. 1994; Rabbitt and Lowe 2000). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV defines age-related cognitive decline as "an objectively identified decline in cognitive functioning consequent to the ageing process that is within normal limits given the person's age. Individuals with this condition may report problems remembering names or appointments or may experience difficulties in solving complex problems" (DSM-IV 1994, 780.9).

Previous work in monkeys (Arnsten et al. 1995), using the dopamine D2 receptor agonist quinpirole to assess the contribution of dopamine (DA) to age-related loss of prefrontal cortical cognitive function, found that DA depletion contributes significantly to age-related cognitive decline. The magnitude of cognitive improvement was less in the aged animals compared with younger monkeys, a finding consistent with a loss of DA from the prefrontal cortex with age. As described by Arnsten and Goldman-Rakic (1985), catecholamine loss in the aged non-human primate is particularly pronounced in the prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices and in the catecholaminergic neurones of the brainstem that project to the cortex.

Methylphenidate, a piperidine-derived central nervous system stimulant, is believed to exert its therapeutic effects via an ability to increase the synaptic concentration of dopamine, through blockade of dopamine transporters (Volkow et al. 1998). Methylphenidate blocks reuptake of DA more effectively than it blocks noradrenaline (NA) and much more than 5-HT (Gatley et al. 1996). In addition, D2 receptor availability in the striatum is correlated with methylphenidate-induced changes in metabolism in frontal and temporal cortices (Volkow et al. 1997). Using therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate in healthy volunteers, Volkow et al. (2001) have shown that the increase in extracellular dopamine levels in the striatum caused by methylphenidate is negatively correlated with age. Primate studies have shown age-related differences in the cognitive response to methylphenidate (Prendergast et al. 1998), with methylphenidate significantly attenuating distractibility in younger animals, but failing to have this effect in aged monkeys.

Methylphenidate is currently the treatment of choice in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adults (Solanto 1998), but is being used increasingly in the treatment of elderly patients with disorders such as dementia (Galynker et al. 1997), Alzheimer's disease (Kittur and Hauser 1999), Parkinson's disease (Camicioli et al. 2001) and geriatric depression (Lavretsky and Kumar 2001). There have been no comprehensive assessments of the cognitive effects of methylphenidate in healthy elderly subjects.

Previous research in young volunteers has shown pronounced effects of methylphenidate on working memory and sustained attention. Young volunteers have shown improved accuracy on the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) spatial working memory task following administration of methylphenidate (Elliott et al. 1997). This finding is consistent with other studies using DA agents in young subjects. Luciana and Collins (1997) showed that spatial delayed working memory functions can be facilitated with the DA agonist bromocriptine and impaired with haloperidol (a DA antagonist). Dietary depletion of DA precursor has been shown to impair spatial recognition memory and spatial working memory (Harmer et al. 2001). Similarly, administration of the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride resulted in a dose dependent impairment at a sequence generation spatial working memory task (Mehta et al. 1999).

With regard to sustained attention, Elliott et al. (1997) observed enhanced performance on the rapid visual information processing (RVIP) sustained attention task in terms of the speeding of responses in young male volunteers after they had received 40 mg methylphenidate. d-Amphetamine, which stimulates the release of presynaptic dopamine, has also been shown to improve sustained attention in a number of studies with healthy young adults (Halliday et al. 1994; McKetin et al. 1999). This improvement is attenuated by dietary depletion of dopamine (McTavish et al. 2001). In addition, Koelega (1993) concluded that vigilance tasks appear to be the most sensitive tasks for detecting the effects of stimulants.

It was of interest to assess the effects of age on response inhibition. Mechanisms of inhibitory control may be at the core of age-related cognitive decline (West 1996). Rubia et al. (2000) have demonstrated a significant effect of age on the prefrontal cortex activation during a motor response inhibition task. Methylphenidate has also been shown to improve stop-signal performance in children with ADHD, enhancing their ability to inhibit prepotent responding (Tannock et al. 1989). It is well established that improvement in stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) is correlated with clinical measures of impulsivity in normal children and those children suffering from ADHD (Logan et al. 1997).

Whilst the most robust and replicated effects of methylphenidate have been on measures of working memory and sustained attention, effects of methylphenidate have also been shown on more complex tasks of executive function. In normal young volunteers, Rogers et al. (1999) showed that methylphenidate enhanced learning of the extradimensional shift compared to the intra-dimensional shift in the intradimensional-extradimensional (IDED) set-shifting task. In the same task, response latencies at both of the shift stages were also lengthened by methylphenidate, with the possibility that improvements in task performance may sometimes be accompanied by performance costs on other measures. Methylphenidate has also been shown to improve performance on the Tower of London test of planning (Elliott et al. 1997). Additionally, with respect to decision-making, it has been shown that ageing is associated with poorer decision-making and a decrease in risk-taking in normal volunteers (Deakin et al. 2001).

The present investigation, therefore, sought to extend the current understanding of the relationship between ageing, cognitive function and dopaminergic neurotransmission by characterising the dose-related effects of methylphenidate on neuropsychological functioning in elderly healthy volunteers. The present understanding of the mechanisms of methylphenidate action and the dopaminergic modulations of attentional function does not provide for differential effects across age groups. If loss of catecholamines contributes to age-related cognitive decline, it might be expected that pharmacological treatments that facilitate their transmission may ameliorate these cognitive deficits. Given the current evidence of an inverted "U" function underlying dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function (Zahrt et al. 1997; Mehta et al. 2001), it could be postulated that elderly subjects with reduced levels of baseline dopamine would show an exaggerated cognitive response to methylphenidate. Alternatively, if pre-synaptic loss of dopaminergic terminals or cell bodies underlies the age-related effects (Wenk et al. 1989), elderly subjects may show an attenuated response to DA-stimulating agents.

The neuropsychological battery for assessing effects of methylphenidate in the elderly was designed to assess working memory and sustained attention (shown to be reliably improved in healthy young subjects), response inhibition (a sensitive dopamine-modulated marker of ADHD) and measures of decision-making and problem solving (given the DSM-IV criteria for age-related cognitive decline).

Materials and methods

Subjects and procedures

Sixty healthy male adult volunteers (see Table 1 for demographics) were recruited by advertisement in the local community. Exclusion criteria included any significant psychiatric history, visual or motor impairment, or the concurrent use of any psychotropic medications or any medication contra-indicated with methylphenidate. In addition, subjects with a history of hypertension, cardiac disorders, epilepsy or drug or alcohol abuse were also excluded. All subjects were given the Folstein Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975) to exclude undiagnosed mild dementia: a score of less than 26 resulted in exclusion. All volunteers were advised not to consume alcohol or caffeine-containing drinks for 12 h before the study. The study was approved by the Cambridge Local Research and Ethics Committee and written informed consent was given by all subjects prior to testing.

Table 1. Mean age, NART and education level for each group. Values shown are the mean and standard errors of the mean for each group. Age is given in years, NART is the predicted verbal IQ score and education level is in years in formal education

A double-blind, between subjects design was used, with participants randomised to receive either a single oral dose of a lactose placebo, 20 mg methylphenidate or 40 mg methylphenidate. Groups were well matched for age, NART verbal IQ (as indexed by the National Adult Reading Test, Nelson 1982) and education level (Table 1).

At oral therapeutic doses (0.3–0.6 mg/kg) methylphenidate is estimated to occupy more than half of brain dopamine transporters (Volkow et al. 2001) with accumulation predominantly in the striatum (Challman and Lipsky 2000). This corresponds to a dose range of 20–40 mg methylphenidate for a 70 kg subject. Peak plasma concentration is obtained 1–3 h after oral administration with a mean plasma half-life of 1.5–2.5 h (Challman and Lipsky 2000). Subjects were therefore tested 90 min post drug administration, for approximately 2 h.

Physiological measures

Blood pressure and pulse measurements were taken using a Criticare Systems Inc. Comfort Cuff (Model 507NJ) at four time points: before drug administration, immediately prior to testing (90 min post-drug), 1 h into testing and on completion of testing.

Psychological measures

Subjects were tested using well-validated tests including measures from the CANTAB battery (Cambridge Cognition, http://www.camcog.com) (Sahakian and Owen 1992). The test battery also included novel variants of some of the original CANTAB tasks, to allow for greater sensitivity in normal volunteers. All subjects received the same tests in the same order. All computerised tasks were run on an Advantech personal computer (Model PPC-120T-RT), and responses registered either via the touch-sensitive screen or a response key, depending on the task. The majority of the tasks have been described elsewhere and readers are directed to the cited references, which were selected for their detailed descriptions.

Visual analogue scales

Subjects were asked to complete visual analogue scales (Bond and Lader 1974) before administration of the drug and at two intervals during the testing session: immediately prior to cognitive testing and 1 h into testing. At each time point subjects were asked to rate their feeling in terms of 16 dimensions. The measures used in this study were alert-drowsy, calm-excited, strong-feeble, muzzy-clear headed, well coordinated-clumsy, lethargic-energetic, contented-discontented, troubled-tranquil, mentally slow-quick witted, tense-relaxed, attentive-dreamy, incompetent-proficient, happy-sad, antagonistic-amicable, interested-bored, and withdrawn-gregarious. The dimensions were presented as 100 mm lines, the two extremes of the emotion (e.g. "alert" and "drowsy") written at each end, and subjects marked where they felt they ranked on each line.

Digit span

This task was taken from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Wechsler 1981). Subjects were asked to repeat increasingly longer sequences of digits initially forwards and then backwards. Sequences started at 2 digits and went up to a maximum of 9. For each sequence length two different series of digits were presented, with a maximum of two points being awarded if both series were repeated correctly. Failure at the second attempt of any particular stage terminated the test. Scores for both the forwards and backwards tests were summed to yield an overall test score. Actual span, the highest number of digits correctly recalled, was also recorded.

Computerised tests taken from the CANTAB battery

A brief description of the key measures for each of the CANTAB tasks are presented in Table 2. The computerised tests were preceded by a "sensorimotor" task designed to familiarise the subjects with the touch-sensitive computer screen and the procedures. As this was a screening task the response measures are not reported here. Tasks taken from the CANTAB Working Memory and Planning battery were the spatial working memory (SWM) task and the spatial span (SSP) task. Visual memory was tested using the paired associates learning (PAL) task, while tests of attention were the rapid visual information processing (RVIP) task and attentional set-shifting task (IDED). The test battery also included a novel variant of the original CANTAB Stockings of Cambridge task, the "one-touch" Tower of London spatial planning task (NTOL).

Table 2. Summary of CANTAB tasks used

Decision making (Gamble) task

This task is described in detail by Rahman et al. (2001). In brief, subjects were shown a display and told that the computer had hidden a yellow token randomly inside one of ten boxes at the top of the screen. The subject was trained to decide whether the computer had hidden the token in a red box or a blue box by touching either the response box marked "red" or "blue". They were then offered bets (in ascending and descending order—the order of presentation of which was randomised across groups) and instructed to try and increase their total points score by placing a bet on their choice being correct. Manipulation of the ratio of red and blue boxes from trial to trial made it possible to examine a subject's decision-making behaviour over a variety of differentially weighted contingencies.

Stop-signal (STOP) task

This classic paradigm (Logan 1994), which measures pre-potent response inhibition, has previously been used to assess ADHD and the remedial effects of methylphenidate in younger subjects (Logan et al. 2000). Subjects were required to make a speeded response on "go" trials (left response for left-pointing arrow, right response for right-pointing arrow), but to withhold their response on "stop" trials (signalled by a 300 Hz tone). Stopping was made difficult by having a preponderance of "go" trials (75%). The timing of the stop-signal was manipulated by means of a tracking algorithm (Osman et al. 1990) in such a way as to allow estimation of SSRT. Subjects performed five blocks of 64 trials each, and were given visual feedback after each block for their average correct "go" reaction time and the number of discrimination errors made (incorrect response on "go" trials). Subjects were not given feedback with regard to successful or failed inhibition, but were urged to do their best to stop, while continuing to respond as fast as possible on "go" trials.

Statistical analysis

To investigate the effect of experimental treatment upon test performance, differences between group mean (or median) performance for single scores were analysed using a one-way ANOVA (three levels) or the equivalent non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA (conformity to a normal distribution was assessed with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). To clarify the nature of any such differences, planned orthogonal contrasts comparing the effect of the two doses of drug and their relationship to placebo were performed where appropriate. In instances where several readings were taken for the same score, a repeated measures ANOVA was used to test the effects of relevant independent within- and between-subjects variables. Analysis was performed on the raw scores, although for the purposes of Fig. 1 and Fig. 2 these values were adjusted for the baseline score.

Untransformed scores are displayed in tables and figures. However, in order to decrease skew and stabilise variances, data was transformed in preparation for parametric analysis. Logarithmic transformations (x=log10y) were performed for latency and arcsine transformations (\(x = 2\;\arcsin \sqrt y\)) for proportional data.

As the motivation for this study was to determine the overall cognitive profile of methylphenidate in elderly subjects, our interest lies equally in ascertaining the lack of an effect on particular variables, as in identifying the presence of a significant group difference on other variables. The former conclusion of a "lack of effect" is subject to type II errors and the latter, the presence of an effect, to type I errors (Howell 1997). Taking this into consideration, we have taken P>0.1 in reporting "no effect" and P<0.05 in reporting an "effect".

Results

Physiological effects: blood pressure and pulse

Physiological readings were taken at four time points during the experiment and the effects illustrated in Fig. 1. Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of drug on systolic blood pressure [F(2,57)=3.98, P=0.024]. This effect was attributable to subjects in both the drug groups showing higher mean systolic blood pressure than those in the placebo group. There were no significant differences in systolic blood pressure between the two doses of methylphenidate [F(1,38)=2.67, P=0.111]. There was a trend towards increased diastolic blood pressure following drug administration, although this failed to reach significance [F(2,57)=2.57, P=0.085]. There was, however, a highly significant effect of drug on pulse [F(2,57)=9.34, P<0.001], with subjects receiving the high dose of methylphenidate showing a significant elevation in pulse in comparison with the low dose group [F(1,38)=8.40, P=0.006] and the placebo group [F(1,38)=8.91, P=0.005]. Over time, this effect on pulse was highly significant (drug×time interaction, F(6,171)=5.02, P<0.001], with methylphenidate (again, particularly at the high dose) attenuating the natural decrease in heart rate over time (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.
figure 1

Effect of methylphenidate on blood pressure and pulse. A main effect of drug on systolic blood pressure was observed (P=0.024). There was also a trend towards an increase in diastolic blood pressure following drug administration but this failed to reach significance (P=0.085). The 40 mg methylphenidate group showed a significant elevation in heart rate, with the drug attenuating the natural decrease in heart rate over time observed with the placebo and 20 mg groups (P<0.001). *P<0.05; **P≤0.01

Fig. 2.
figure 2

The effect of methylphenidate on subjective changes in alertness, proficiency and energy. Over time, subjects receiving 40 mg methylphenidate reported feeling more alert (P=0.005), energetic (P=0.001) and proficient (P=0.040) than subjects in the placebo and low dose groups. *P<0.05; **P≤0.01

Subjective effects: visual analogue scales

As can be seen from Fig. 2, over time, subjects receiving 40 mg methylphenidate reported feeling more alert [drug×time interaction, F(4,114)=3.88, P=0.005], energetic [F(4,114)=5.38, P=0.001] and proficient [F(4,114)=2.74, P=0.040] than subjects in the placebo and low dose groups. Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a between subjects main effect of drug on alertness [F(2,57)=6.20, P=0.004], energy [F(2,57)=8.94, P<0.001] and proficiency [F(2,57)=3.44, P=0.039]. There were no other significant effects of drug or drug×time interactions on any of the other self-reported measures (P>0.05).

Psychological effects

As shown in Table 3, performance on several of the subtests of the CANTAB battery (namely the Digit Span, SSP, RVIP, SWM and STOP tests) did not differ between drug and placebo groups. By contrast, IDED, Gamble and PAL measures did differ significantly between groups. There was a trend towards an effect on latency with the NTOL task. The nature of these group differences is considered in detail below.

Table 3. Summary of test results. Values shown for each variable are the mean and standard errors of the mean for each group

IDED

Methylphenidate produced a highly significant increase in the time taken to select a response, with a main effect of drug on total latency [F(2,52)=6.22, P=0.004]. This difference was found to be statistically significant for the compound discrimination reversal (CDR) stage [F(2,59)=4.42, P=0.019], intradimensional shift (IDS) stage [F(2,59)=6.58, P=0.003] and intradimensional reversal (IDR) stage [F(2,59)=3.80, P=0.028]. There was also a tendency towards increased latency at both the extradimensional shift (EDS) [F(2,58)=2.52, P=0.090] and the extradimensional reversal (EDR) [F(2,52)=2.81, P=0.070] stages. There were no significant differences in latency between the low and high dose of methylphenidate [F(1,33)=1.12, P=0.273]. There were no statistically significant effects on total errors [χ2(2)=0.61, P=0.738], pre-extradimensional shift errors [χ2(2)=2.28, P=0.321] or extradimensional shift errors [χ2(2)=0.42, P=0.812] between the groups.

Gamble

In this decision-making task there was a main effect of drug on deliberation time [F(2,53)=5.01, P=0.010], with subjects on drug taking longer to make their decision as to which coloured box the token was hidden in. There was no significant difference between the two doses of methylphenidate [F(1,38)=1.31, P=0.261]. There was also a tendency for subjects on drug to not choose the most likely outcome compared to the placebo group [F(2,53)=2.56, P=0.086]. Although there were no significant differences between the two drug groups [F(1,38)=1.71, P=0.184], subjects in the low dose were the most impaired compared to placebo [F(1,38)=4.87, P=0.043]. There was no effect of drug on the percentage bet placed on the decision [F(2,53)=0.44, P=0.646]. As expected, response time, choice of contingency, and size of bet all differed as a function of the ratio of red to blue boxes: there was a main effect of ratio on the deliberation time, the probability of choosing the most likely outcome and the percentage bet [F(3,57)>5.44, P<0.002]. With regard to the percentage bet, all groups selected significantly higher bets in the descend condition compared to the ascend condition [F(1,53)=34.43, P<0.001], although there were no significant differences between groups for either condition. Thus, subjects receiving methylphenidate performed similarly to those in the placebo group, but with a significant slowing in deliberation time and the low dose group displaying a trend towards choosing the less likely betting option.

PAL

Subjects in the high dose group displayed a trend towards an improved first trial memory score compared to the low-dose group. This effect only tended towards significance between the groups [F(2,59)=3.11, P=0.052], due to a significant difference between the drug groups [F(1,38)=1.36, P=0.020]. However, there were no significant differences between either dose of methylphenidate and the placebo group [F(1,38)<1.50, P>0.141]. There were no statistically significant differences between the groups with regard to total errors [F(2,59)=1.43, P=0.248] or total trials [F(2,59)=1.60, P=0.211] to criterion.

NTOL

In this task of planning, there was no effect of drug on the ability to correctly solve the problems, as measured by the mean attempts to obtain a correct solution [F(2,57)=0.96, P=0.389]. There was also no significant drug×move interaction with respect to accuracy [F(10,285)=0.67, P=0.750]. Subjects in the low dose drug group, however, displayed a trend towards taking longer to make their first attempt compared to the other two groups, with a tendency towards a main effect of drug on total latency [F(2,57)=3.02, P=0.057]. This difference in latency between the groups, however, did not reach significance at any of the individual difficulty levels of the task [F(2,57)<3.02, P>0.056], with only a tendency for subjects in the low dose group to be slower than the other two groups at the two-move [F(2,57)=3.02, P=0.056] and four-move [F(2,57)=2.60, P=0.083] problems. There was no significant drug×move interaction with respect to latency [F(10,285)=1.45, P=0.159].

Discussion

The results of this study demonstrate that, in elderly subjects, the cognitive effects of methylphenidate are grossly attenuated and distinct from the profile previously described in younger volunteers (Elliott et al. 1997). Methylphenidate had significant cardiovascular and subjective effects in elderly subjects. The higher dose of drug was associated with increases in feelings of alertness, energy and proficiency compared to placebo and low dose methylphenidate. Both doses of drug also resulted in a significantly increased systolic blood pressure, with a tendency towards a slightly elevated diastolic blood pressure. In addition, the high dose group showed a significant elevation in heart rate, with the drug attenuating the natural decrease in pulse over the testing session observed in the placebo and low dose groups.

However, unlike in younger volunteers (Elliott et al. 1997), no significant effects of drug were observed on spatial working memory (SWM), spatial memory span (SSP) or sustained attention (RVIP). There was also no effect of methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) or the number of discrimination errors made in the stop-signal (STOP) task in this older population. Digit span was also unaffected by methylphenidate.

Subtle effects of latency, similar to those shown in younger volunteers (Rogers et al. 1999), were identified in this study. Both doses of methylphenidate resulted in a specific slowing in response time in the IDED and the Gamble tasks. There was also a tendency for subjects in the low dose group to be slower at the NTOL task, although this was not shown in the high dose group and there was no effect on latency at the different difficulty levels of this task between subject groups. Standard reaction time, as measured by the median "go" reaction time on the STOP task and response latency on the RVIP task, remained unaffected by methylphenidate, suggesting that the effect on latency on the IDED, Gamble and NTOL tasks was not a simple psychomotor effect of drug.

Subjects on methylphenidate displayed a trend towards choosing the less likely outcome more often in the Gamble task, particularly at the low dose. However, no differences were observed between the groups in terms of the bets placed on each decision. There was also a trend for methylphenidate to improve performance in the paired-associates learning task at the highest dose, although further analysis revealed no significant differences between the drug groups and the placebo group.

This lack of effect of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and sustained attention is consistent with theories of catecholaminergic loss in aged brains and the inverted "U" pattern of response (Cai and Arnsten 1997). Evidence suggests that either insufficient or excessive stimulation of prefrontal cortex (PFC) dopaminergic receptors is detrimental to cognitive function (Zarht et al. 1997; Mehta et al. 1999, 2001). Arnsten et al. (1994) showed that D1 agonist treatment in aged monkeys produced a biphasic response with low doses improving performance while higher doses resulted in impairment, an effect that mimicked that seen with young, dopamine-depleted monkeys in the same experiment. Full D1-receptor agonists given to elderly rhesus monkeys also impaired (or had no effect on) performance with increasing high doses, despite improving performance following lower doses (Cai and Arnsten 1997). Therefore it is possible that, at the doses used in this study, subjects were displaying similar effects to aged animals on high dose DA agonists and studies with doses lower than 20 mg methylphenidate might reveal improvements in cognitive performance.

Research with animals has indicated that advancing age is associated with marked loss of DA and DA metabolites (Goldman-Rakic and Brown 1981), with a concomitant loss of PFC cognitive function (Rapp and Amaral 1989). Biochemical studies in rats have shown strong correlations between spatial working memory impairments and loss of DA metabolites in the PFC (Luine et al. 1990). In humans, positron emission tomography using 11C-SCH23390 has revealed that the binding potential of dopamine D1 receptors in the frontal cortex decreases by almost 40% with age, despite no evidence of brain atrophy (Suhara et al. 1991). Given the finding in the aged brain of a greater loss of DA from the PFC than from the motor areas (Wenk et al. 1989), it is possible that this explains methylphenidate's lack of effect on performance despite effects of latency similar to those seen in younger subjects being retained.

The significant cardiovascular effects observed after administration of methylphenidate were similar to those observed in younger volunteers (Elliott et al. 1997). These effects of methylphenidate are due to indirect sympathomimetic action as a result of inhibition of the NA transporter (Challman and Lipsky 2000). Indirectly acting sympathomimetics are not selective in their actions at α- and β-adrenoreceptors, causing generalised blood pressure and pulse rate increases. Cardiovascular effects of methylphenidate may therefore be pronounced, in the absence of strong cognitive effects. It is more surprising, however, that the subjective effects of methylphenidate were dissociable from the (lack of) cognitive effects. Rothman et al. (2000) have suggested that the noradrenergic contribution to the subjective effects of stimulant drugs may be underestimated. It is also possible that the increased ratings of alertness and energy may be directly associated with the experience of the cardiovascular effects.

One possible limitation of this study is that the use of a between subjects design might have decreased the statistical power. A between-subjects design was used to avoid practice effects, which would have been anticipated on the more conceptual cognitive tasks such as IDED. However, robust effects of methylphenidate were found on response latency in several of the tests (IDED, Gamble and NTOL), indicating that the lack of performance effects is not due to the study design. The use of higher doses of methylphenidate in this population is not advisable due to the enhanced risk of cardiovascular side-effects. However, it is possible that a repeated dosing schedule of methylphenidate in this population might produce different results. It has been suggested that repeated exposure to stimulants produces a long-lasting change in specific neurochemical systems, particularly the mesolimbic dopamine system (Schenk et al. 1991).

In summary, administration of 20–40 mg orally of methylphenidate to an elderly population of healthy volunteers produces significant cardiovascular effects associated with increased feelings of alertness and energy, but a general lack of effect on most of the cognitive functions monitored. Increases in response latency, similar to those in younger volunteers, were identified. However, these data suggest that methylphenidate may not be beneficial for use in patients reporting age-related memory loss.