Introduction

Reducing the stigma of mental illness has been identified as one of the major challenges of the mental health field [38, 64, 74]. Negative reactions to those with mental illness are thought to contribute to delays in help seeking [8, 21] as well as placing many individuals who have received psychiatric treatment at disadvantages in such areas as employment, income, housing, personal relationships and health care [50, 51, 54, 76, 77]. Aspects of this stigma can also be internalized by those with mental illness with implications for their psychological well-being and self-esteem [20, 48, 62, 79].

In general, approaches to understanding this stigmatization have emphasized the importance of beliefs about the mentally ill [5, 18, 19, 47, 75]. It is suggested that prominent stereotyped beliefs about mental illness include that those with mental illness are (1) dangerous; (2) have contributed to their own misfortune through character weakness; (3) are socially unpredictable and inappropriate; (4) are unlikely to greatly improve with treatment; with the only potentially positive aspect of the stereotype of mental illness being belief that it may be associated with artistic talent or genius [5, 27, 36]. There is particularly strong evidence of significant relationships between wishing to maintain greater social distance towards those with mental illness and beliefs about their dangerousness [3, 5, 46, 75] responsibility for their own illness [52] and the unpredictability or inappropriateness of their social behavior [5, 69].

Angermeyer and Matschinger [5] assessed the relative importance of beliefs about schizophrenia in predicting behavioral intentions that reflect social distance. Using data from a representative sample of over 5,000 from the population of Germany, they found that beliefs about social inappropriateness or unpredictability and dangerousness were the most important predictors of behavioural intentions towards those with schizophrenia. However, the total amount of variance in social distance accounted for by beliefs about schizophrenia and demographic characteristics of respondents was limited to 27%.

Many interventions have been implemented with the intention of improving reactions to the mentally ill by changing beliefs about the nature of mental illness. Unfortunately, there is often no evaluation of their effectiveness. Those few interventions that have been evaluated have often succeeded in changing stated beliefs and attitudes, but have yielded much less evidence of improvement in behavioural intentions toward those with mental illness, [29, 56, 57, 60, 67]. Reviews of the relevant literature have also concluded that any effects of efforts to reduce the stigma of mental illness have largely been restricted to changes in verbally expressed beliefs and attitudes rather than behavioural intentions or behaviour [63, 70, 77]. In addition, there is evidence that desirable changes over time in the public beliefs about mental illness are not necessarily accompanied by significant improvement in behavioural intentions [6].

The above findings are consistent with suggestions in the more general literature on prejudice and stigma that stereotyped beliefs may have been overemphasized as determinants of behavioral intentions or actual behavior [39, 55]. Goffman’s seminal writings on stigma emphasized the importance of a perceived social consensus or norms concerning behavioral responses to stigmatized individuals [31, 43] and there have been several recent calls for more attention to the social normative context of stigmatization in general and stigmatization of mental illness in particular [33, 37, 80]. As Link et al. [48] have noted, “People form expectations as to whether most people will reject an individual with mental illness as a friend, employee, neighbour or intimate partner…” (p 203). It has been demonstrated in other contexts that an individual’s perception of social norms can have an influence on his or her preferred social distance with reference to stigmatized groups independently of personal beliefs about that group [22, 37, 73], but this has not been investigated in the context of mental illness.

The purpose of the current investigation was to examine the relative importance of perceived social norms and beliefs about mental illness as predictors of preferred social distance towards the mentally ill. If perceived social norms are an independent predictor of preferred social distance to those with mental illness, it could have significant implications for the design of interventions to change such reactions [15, 68]. The specific hypothesis tested was that perceived social norms make a significant contribution to behavioural intentions with respect to individuals with schizophrenia or depression independently of beliefs about such illnesses.

Method

Participants

Participants were 200 undergraduate students at the University of Western Ontario in London, Canada who responded to advertisements to participate in the study. Participants were offered $15 as compensation for their time. All participants signed an informed consent and the study protocol was approved by the relevant Ethics Board at the University. There were 90 men and 110 women in the sample, with mean age of 21.5 years (SD = 5.0 years).

Measures

Mental illness vignettes

Many studies of reactions to those with mental illness have asked about reaction to an unspecified “mental illness”, but such a term is rather amorphous and likely to give rise to varying referents for different respondents [49, 59]. An alternative strategy to avoid this ambiguity has been the use of vignettes, which allow assessment of determinants of participant’s reaction to a specific presentation of mental illness. For current purposes we used two vignettes, one concerning an individual with schizophrenia and one concerning an individual with depression. These vignettes had been prepared by Angermeyer and colleagues for their extensive population survey in Germany concerning reactions to mental illness, [46]. Each vignette concerned a hypothetical acquaintance “AB” and was developed to reflect symptoms in the DSM criteria for schizophrenia or depression as established by five experts in psychopathology. Aside from very minor wording changes to improve readability of the vignettes in English, the only substantial modifications of the vignettes were that the gender of the person described was varied so that in approximately half the cases the person was described as male and in the other half as female, and a final sentence was added providing a diagnosis so that subjects were responding to a combination of symptoms and diagnoses. Each subject received only one vignette describing mental illness (either schizophrenia or depression).

Because preferred social distance toward an individual with a mental illness may at least partially reflect general preferences or habits not specific to mental illness, we also had subjects respond to a vignette describing a control, not ill individual (CD). The responses this vignette were always completed before the vignette describing depression or schizophrenia. The vignettes describing schizophrenia, depression and the control comparison are in Appendix I.

Behavioural intentions

Behavioural intentions towards the person described in the vignette were assessed using slightly modified versions of six items adapted by Link et al. [45] from the Bogardus Social Distance Scale [11]. These included items in which respondents indicated on a five point scale (from “I certainly would” to “I certainly would not”) the likelihood that they would engage in a series of hypothetical situations which involved: taking a job where they would be working with the individual; moving into a home next door to the individual; becoming a friend of the individual; renting a room to the individual; recommending the person for a job; and supporting marriage to their sibling or child. These items were completed by participants with response to the person in the control vignette and either the individual with schizophrenia or depression. Responses were averaged across items to provide a cumulative index with higher scores indicating greater social distance. Coefficient alpha for the social distance scale was 0.84.

Beliefs about illness

A set of items was included to assess the five dimensions of beliefs about illness identified by Angermeyer and Matschinger [5] and Hayward and Bright [36]. Given evidence that empathetic behaviour towards another person can be influenced by perceived similarity [9, 10] we also included items designed to assess the extent to which the symptoms of illness are believed to vary on a continuum of similarity with everyday experience. Ten of the items used were adapted from Angermeyer and Matschinger [5] and another ten developed by the authors. Respondents rated each item on a five-point scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Corresponding to the nature of the illness vignette presented to each subject, ratings were made with respect to personal beliefs about either schizophrenia or depression.

Results of a factor analysis using principal axis factoring and promax rotation are presented in Table 1. Promax rotation was used as it was anticipated the factors could be correlated. Consistent with expectations a scree plot suggested a six-factor solution and the resulting item loadings suggest the anticipated dimensions of attribution of personal responsibility; belief in continuity between everyday experience and illness; danger; social inappropriateness; perception of talent and ineffectiveness of treatment. Similar factor structures emerge if the factor analysis is carried out separately for each diagnosis. Scores on each of the six dimensions were computed using the average score for each of the relevant items with higher scores indicating more negative beliefs (greater personal responsibility, less continuity with normal experience, greater danger, more socially inappropriate behavior; less talent/intelligence and poor treatment outcome). The coefficient alphas for the first four scales were respectable (0.74–0.83). The talent/intelligence and treatment outcome scales, however, showed an alpha of 0.57 and 0.48, respectively. Angermeyer and Matschinger [5] also found lower internal consistency for these scales than danger, attribution of responsibility or social inappropriateness. An assessment of the two-week test retest reliability of each of the scales was carried out using 20 respondents and all yielded reliability of at least 0.75.

Table 1 Six dimensions of belief about illness derived from promax rotated principal axis factoring

Perceived norms

There is evidence that it may be important to assess two types of perceived social norms: injunctive norms, which reflect perceptions of what most others would approve of, and descriptive norms, which reflect what others would actually do [13, 16, 40, 72]. Following precedents [13, 78], perceived injunctive norms were assessed by a seven point scale rating the extent with which people who are important to the respondent would approve of each specific behaviour. For instance, participants responded to the item “If you recommended AB for a job, people who are important to you, such as family and friends, would:” using a seven-point scale varying between “Very strongly approve” to “Very strongly disapprove.” For descriptive norms, respondents indicated how likely it was that those people would actually engage in the behaviour. As an example, they responded to the item “How likely is it that people who are important to you, such as family and friends, would themselves actually recommend AB for a job, if they were in your position,” using a seven-point scale anchored by “They certainly would” and “They certainly would not.” In order to obtain an aggregate assessment of perceived norms with respect to all six specific behaviours, we calculated a cumulative score based on all items assessing either descriptive or injunctive normative beliefs. These included norms with respect to working with, moving next door, becoming a friend, renting a room, recommending for a job, and supporting marriage to a family member; with a higher score indicating a belief that others would favour more social distance. The coefficient alpha of this scale was 0.89.

Social desirability

Concern has been expressed that responses to questions about mental illness may be influenced by a bias to present a socially desirable impression [2, 3335]. In order to examine this possibility we included the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale [24]. This 33-item scale is designed to provide an index of a respondent’s tendency to present a socially desirable impression through endorsing as true self-descriptions that are acceptable but improbable, or denying descriptions that are undesirable but probable. Sample items include endorsement of “I never hesitate to go out of my way to help someone in trouble” and denial of “I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my own way.” The alpha coefficient of this cumulative index in the current sample was 0.89.

Results

Effects of gender and target illness

A three-way analysis of variance was carried out to examine any effects of gender of respondent, gender of the person in the scenario and the nature of the illness described on the beliefs about illness or perceived norms. The gender of the person in the scenario had no effect on any of the measures. However, gender of the participants did make a difference. Men attributed a significantly higher personal responsibility for either illness (F = 6.88, df = 1.192, P < 0.01), and perceived normative expectations as less favorable to social contact with the vignette concerning a mentally ill person (F = 4.11, df = 1.192, P < 0.05). There were no other significant main or interaction effects for gender of the respondent.

There were several significant main effects for the type of illness described. In comparison to depression, schizophrenia was associated with a preference for greater social distance (\( \bar {x} = 3.8 \), SD = 0.73 versus 3.4 SD = 0.81 for schizophrenia and depression, respectively; F = 8.74, df = 1.192, P < 0.01); greater belief in danger (\( \bar {x} = 2.4 \), SD = 0.70 versus \( \bar {x} = 2.1 \), SD = 0.77; F = 6.05, df = 1.192, P < 0.05); greater expectation of socially inappropriate behavior (\( \bar {x} = 3.5 \), SD = 0.72 versus \( \bar {x} = 2.9 \), SD = 0.82; F = 25.2, df = 1.192, P < 0.001); less belief in personal responsibility for illness (\( \bar {x} = 2.3 \); SD = 0.88 versus \( \bar {x} = 2.9 \), SD = 0.83; F = 28.67, df = 1.192, P < .001); and less perceived continuity with normal experience (\( \bar {x} = 3.4 \), SD = 0.86 versus \( \bar {x} = 0.44 \); SD = 0.54; F = 85.98, df = 1,192, P < 0.001). There were no significant interactions of target illness with either gender of the person in the vignette or gender of the respondent.

Social desirability

There were no significant correlations between the responses to the social desirability scale and current measures of belief, perceived norms or social distance.

Relationship between personal beliefs and perceived norms

Table 2 presents the Pearson bivariate correlations between each of the measures of personal beliefs and perceived norms. There were some correlations between belief scales. In particular, belief in danger was positively correlated with expectations of socially inappropriate behaviour and belief in greater personal responsibility for illness. Also, greater belief in discontinuity between illness and normal behaviour was associated with less belief in personal responsibility and greater expectation of socially inappropriate behaviour. Beliefs about illness are also associated with perceived norms. Perceived norms favouring greater social distance were positively correlated with belief in danger, social inappropriateness, personal responsibility and lower talent or intelligence.

Table 2 Bivariate Pearson correlations between predictors (n = 200)

Prediction of Social distance

Table 3 presents the correlations of each of the measures of belief, perceived norms and preferred social distance to a normal person with preferred social distance to the vignette featuring schizophrenia or depression. Concerns about danger and inappropriate social behaviour were significantly related to a preference for greater social distance to the vignette concerning either form of illness, with belief in personal responsibility also being correlated with social distance to the person with schizophrenia. It is also apparent that social distance to an ill person is correlated with social distance to the person in the control vignette suggesting there is some common variance reflecting a general preference for social distance. With respect to social distance toward the vignettes concerning an individual with schizophrenia or depression, the largest correlations were with perceived normative expectations about behaviour. In both cases the greater the belief that others important to the respondent would disapprove of and not engage in contact with the ill person, the greater the personally preferred social distance.

Table 3 Correlates of preferred greater social distance

Multiple regression analyses were carried out to assess the independence and relative importance of predictors of preferred social distance to the vignettes describing either schizophrenia or depression. The significant correlates of social distance in Table 3 were all entered into regression analyses for the prediction of social distance separately for schizophrenia and depression, using the SPSS “enter” option. In order to attain statistical significance as a predictor of social distance in such an equation, a variable must predict variance in outcome independently of other predictors [71]. The results are presented in Table 4. There was no evidence of significant multicollinearity between predictors with the relevant SPSS tolerance statistics varying between 0.63 and 0.94 [71]. With respect to social distance toward the person in the schizophrenia or depression vignette, belief that the illness is associated with socially inappropriate and embarrassing behaviour, preferred social distance to the control person and perceived social norm were each independent predictors. For schizophrenia, belief in danger being associated with the illness was at borderline significance as an independent predictor of greater social distance.

Table 4 Regression equations predicting social distance

Subsequent stepwise regression analyses were carried out to assess the relative importance of each predictor in adding incrementally to the prediction of social distance. In the case of schizophrenia, the order of entry was perceived norms as the single most important predictor with only belief about social inappropriateness predicting additional variance. For depression, perceived norms was the first entry into the prediction equation followed by preferred distance to the normal target with none of the other predictors adding significantly to the variance accounted for in social distance.

Discussion

A central focus of theory and research about the stigmatization of mental illness has been on the importance of negative stereotypical beliefs [5, 18, 36]. Our findings support previous reports showing that beliefs concerning inappropriateness or disruptiveness of social behaviour by those with mental illness and their potential dangerousness are the stereotype dimensions that show the greatest relationship to preferred social distance [5, 52, 75]. Such findings are also consistent with models of stigmatization that emphasize the survival functions of avoiding groups who are seen as dangerous or with whom interaction is seen as more costly than beneficial [42].

Our data also reinforce previous reports that there is only a modest proportion of variance in social distance that can be accounted for by the beliefs associated with the stereotype of mental illness. For instance, with respect to schizophrenia, Angermeyer & Matschinger, in their population survey in Germany, found that beliefs about danger, social disruptiveness, responsibility and prognosis when combined with demographic factors accounted for about 27% of the variance in preferred social distance. Similarly, in a population survey in the Netherlands, van’tVeer et al. [75] found that demographics and beliefs about mental illness accounted for 20% of variance in preferred social distance. In our data, measures of stereotypical beliefs about schizophrenia plus gender accounted for 28% of such variance. The differences that we found in the beliefs about and social distance to depression and schizophrenia also parallel reports from America, Australia, Britain, Germany and Japan [4, 23, 33, 58].

The modest relation between reported beliefs about mental illness and behavioural social distance is consistent with research in other areas suggesting that personal beliefs and attitudes about groups are not always strongly related to behavioural discrimination [25, 28] and that such beliefs may have been overemphasized as determinants of negative behavioural reactions to stigmatized groups [37, 39, 55]. As noted earlier, there is also evidence that interventions that change beliefs about the nature of mental illness often have little impact on behavioural intentions [56, 57, 60, 61, 67].

Stigma as a construct emphasizes the existence of a shared social consensus or expectation that members of the stigmatized category are to be avoided or marginalized in social interaction [26, 31, 42, 44]. Social behaviours with respect to the target group or person are not only influenced by the beliefs and attitudes of an individual, but also the individual’s perception of the expectations of others about how one should behave [1, 16, 73]. There is now evidence that perceived norms, as measured in this study, are independent predictors of many forms of social behaviour [7, 40, 41, 78], including responses to groups who are stigmatized or subject to discrimination [22, 30, 37, 68].

The current report is the first, of which we are aware, to examine the importance of perceived norms as an independent influence on behavioural intentions to those with mental illness specifically. We found that perceived norms made an independent contribution to the prediction of social distance beyond that attributable to beliefs about mental illness. In the case of the schizophrenia vignette, personal beliefs about the nature of the illness accounted for 29% of the variance in preferred social distance. This increased to 51% with the addition of perceived social norms. For depression, illness beliefs accounted for 13% of the variance in preferred social distance, and this increased to 34% with the addition of perceived norms. In addition, perception of norms was the most powerful single predictor of social distance, as indicated by behavioral intentions, with respect to both schizophrenia and depression. Uncertainty in personal beliefs about a person or concept has been found to increase the influence of perceived norms in determining behavioral intentions and behavior [14, 16]. Issues related to mental illness and the behavior of those with such illness are sometimes contentious even among experts and can involve subtle concepts. Although the issue of certainty of beliefs related to the stigmatization of the mentally ill has not been extensively investigated, it would be interesting in future to test the prediction that the influence of perceived norms will be greater as confidence in personal beliefs about illness is lower.

There were two other noteworthy findings in the current study. The first is that a measure of bias towards responding in a socially desirable manner did not correlate with any of the measures of beliefs, perceived norms or social distance. The second is that there was a correlation between preferred social distance towards a control vignette featuring a person without illness and the same measure with respect to a vignette concerning schizophrenia or depression. This issue has been largely ignored in previous research on social distance towards the mentally ill, which has often implicitly assumed that such behavioral intentions are solely determined by personal dispositions focused specifically on mental illness. The current findings suggest that behavioural responses to those with mental illness may partially reflect more general preferences concerning social interaction with anyone. Such dispositions are likely related to relatively broad traits such as extraversion, social anxiety or shyness within cultures and likely to vary considerably between cultures.

One limitation of the current study is the use of a paper and pencil measure of social distance. This is a common measure in research on the stigmatization of those with mental illness [49] and such measures of behavioral intention can predict significant variation in actual behavior [7]. Nevertheless, it will be important in future to more fully assess the importance of beliefs about mental illness and perceived norms in predicting overt behavior. From some perspectives, the use of university students as respondents is also a limitation. While the responses of such students are of interest and importance in their own right, one could question whether findings based on such a sample will generalize to a wider population. In addition, our recruitment procedure may have attracted students with a particular interest in mental health issues or introduced other biases. As noted earlier, our findings regarding the importance of specific dimensions of belief about mental illness and the total variance in social distance that they account for is very similar to that from previously published general population surveys. We intend, however, in future to assess the generalizability of our findings regarding the importance of perceived norms in non-student samples.

The finding that perceived social norms are important predictors of social distance towards those with mental illness could have implications for the design of more effective interventions to reduce the marginalization of the mentally ill. In recent years, there have been evaluations of the impact of interventions designed to modify people’s perception of normative behaviour or expectations, or increase the salience of norms that are supportive of a target response. Such interventions generally provided feedback to individuals concerning how their behavior compares to their reference group or provided models of desired behavior in an effort to change perceived norms [12, 15, 17, 22, 32, 53, 65, 68]. These methods have generally been found effective in bringing about changes in behaviour, and a recent report suggests that messages conveying likely social approval (related to injunctive norms) may have even more reliable impacts on behaviour.[66] Although the design of effective “normative messages” requires subtlety [15], there is now evidence of such interventions being successful in bringing about behaviour change in such contexts as recycling [15, 65], littering [12], energy conservation [17], and inter-racial behaviour [22, 68]. Efforts to creatively apply such normative approaches to reducing the stigma of mental illness certainly seem warranted.