Introduction

Caring for people with psychosis has been associated with subjective burden and loss, depression, distress, reduced quality of life, lower social support and stigma [20, 34, 36, 41, 42]. One way of understanding this impact of care has been through carers’ appraisals of the difficulties, as it has been shown that their reactions to their role can be as important as the problems they encounter [2, 17, 40].

There have been various approaches to this. One method involves rating comments, or expressed emotion (EE), during an interview, using prosodic variables such as tone and pitch as well as content. EE has been found to be a robust predictor of poor outcome in both schizophrenia and other physical and mental health conditions [9, 16, 46]. Another approach is the study of carer attributions, which also relate to EE [7, 14]. For instance, highly critical relatives are particularly likely to attribute to patients more control over their symptoms and problems, and to hold them responsible for their difficulties [7].

In health psychology, one conception of illness appraisal has been based on the self-regulatory model of Leventhal and colleagues [29]. These authors have proposed that when individuals are faced with a health threat, they formulate a cognitive representation of their illness in terms of five core constructs. These comprise identity (the symptoms and name of the illness), cause (the aetiology), cure/control (the extent to which the illness is modifiable or curable), consequences (the personal, social and financial effects of the illness) and timeline (the perceived length of time the illness will continue). These are held to predict both practical and emotional responses to a health threat, such as the amount of distress, or whether a person will seek or accept treatment. These illness perceptions have been investigated in physical health conditions, where they do indeed predict a range of outcomes including well-being [23].

In health psychology, there has also been interest in discrepancies in illness perceptions between patients and their carers. Heijmans et al. [24], in an elegant study, found that carers of those with Addison’s disease (AD) tended to have more concerns than patients (maximizers), whereas carers of those with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) tended to minimise concerns. Thompson et al. [43], found that if carers were overprotective with stroke patients, the patients became more dependent. Weinman et al. [48], found that carer minimisation of difficulties predicted poor patient outcomes, and suggested that in psychiatric illness this might indicate that problems were not being taken seriously. For people with psoriasis, Richards et al. [38] found that dissimilar beliefs about illness consequences related to worry in carers.

The self-regulatory model and the measurement of illness perceptions have recently been extended to those with psychosis and their carers [22, 30, 31]. Barrowclough et al. [5] showed that more critical relatives perceived themselves to have less control over the illness, thought it would last longer, and perceived a greater number of symptoms. Lobban et al. [32] found that where participant-relative dyads diverged in their illness perceptions, the carers were more likely to be high EE and to maximise negative outcomes compared to patients.

Given this background and the paucity of studies in psychosis so far, we aimed to investigate the relationships in carer–patient dyads between illness perceptions, affect and carer EE, following a recent relapse of psychosis. We also wished to look at the effects of discrepant illness perceptions on both carer and patient affect.

We tested the following hypotheses:

  • High EE in carers (criticism and over-involvement) would be related to pessimistic illness perceptions of psychosis, specifically perceptions of the consequences, how long it would last (timeline) and whether anything could be done to improve things (cure/control), for both patients and carers.

  • Negative illness perceptions would be related to disturbed affect in carers (distress, depression and low self-esteem)

  • In order to replicate Lobban et al. [32] we predicted that discrepant illness perceptions between carers and participants would relate to high EE ratings. Further we predicted that such discrepancies would also relate to disturbed affect (stress/anxiety, depression and low self-esteem) in both carers and patients.

  • Finally, we hypothesised that carers and patients would have discrepant illness perceptions (carers would be maximisers) in high EE dyads and concordant illness perceptions in low EE dyads.

Method

Participants

The study sample consisted of patients recruited for the Psychological Prevention of Relapse in Psychosis (PRP) Trial (ISRCTN83557988) and their immediate carers. PRP is a UK multi-centre randomised controlled trial of cognitive behaviour therapy and family intervention for psychosis, designed to test hypotheses both about outcome and about the psychological processes associated with psychosis for carers and patients.

The trial was located in four NHS Trusts in London and East Anglia, in the UK. Within each of these Trusts, recruitment was from specified inpatient and outpatient teams that agreed that all patients meeting the eligibility criteria would be asked to participate in the trial. These services were canvassed at least fortnightly for patients with psychosis who were relapsing. Patients meeting the inclusion criteria for the study, (see below), were asked to provide informed consent. If they had carers, they were included if they were the carer in most contact with the patient (i.e., for at least 10 h a week, including telephone calls), had been in a caring role with the patient for at least the previous 3 m, and had a command of the English language sufficient for interview and potential participation in psychological therapies. Consent was then sought from the patient for their carers to enter the trial. Carers were not approached unless this consent had been obtained. Only the primary carer was sought for each patient, and if consent was refused, no other carer from that family was approached.

Patients were recruited at the time of a re-emergence of positive symptoms, either from a previously recovered state or from a state of persisting symptoms. For people with persistent symptoms, a significant exacerbation in positive symptoms was required, typically leading to hospital admission. Patient inclusion criteria were: current clinical diagnosis of non-affective psychosis (schizophrenia, schizo-affective psychosis, delusional disorder; ICD-10, F20); age 18–65 years; a second or subsequent episode starting not more than three months before patients consented to enter the trial; and a rating of least four (moderate severity) on at least one positive psychotic symptom of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al. [26]) at the first time of meeting.

Eighty-two patients and their carers who had consented to take part in the PRP trial and completed a majority of the assessments were included in this study.

The study design was cross-sectional. The data were all obtained by trained independent research assessors, who interviewed identified patients and then their carers, and administered questionnaires during the baseline phase of the randomised controlled trial and before allocation.

Carers were recruited consecutively. 208 patients who met criteria were in contact with carers but 114 refused access to them (54%). Our ethics committee required that carers did not have to give a reason for this refusal, information that is thus unavailable. This was about the same refusal rate for patient participation in PRP overall (55%). A further 11/94 carers themselves refused consent (12%). Again, no details were available for the group that refused consent. One further carer did not provide full data on the IPQ ratings.

The study had ethical approval; reference MREC 01/1/14.

Carer measures

Illness Perceptions Questionnaire (IPQ) [47]

The IPQ consists of items measuring 5 core illness constructs: identity, cause, consequences, cure/control, timeline. The IPQ is a reliable and valid measure of illness perceptions in psychosis [30]. We used a version of the original IPQ modified by Weinman and Garety for use in people with psychosis [25], with the words problem/illness used instead of just illness. The constructs used in this study were consequences, timeline and cure/control, comprising seven, three and six items respectively in accordance with Weinman’s [47] original IPQ. Items were measured on a scale of 1–5, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Carers were asked to rate how they thought the illness had affected the participant.

When carer and participant IPQ’s were compared directly, the items in each component were matched to ensure that we were looking at identical scales; for these comparisons, we used 6 items for consequences, five for cure/control and 2 timeline items. Internal reliability values for these identical modified scales were satisfactory, although those for consequences were lower (carer perception of patient consequences, Cronbach α = 0.55; patient perception of consequences α = 0.53; carer cure/control α = 0.77; patient cure/control α = 0.67; carer chronic timeline items α = 0.74; patient chronic timeline α = 0.92 [45]).

In order to assess discrepancies between patient and carer scores, we looked directly at the scores for each dyad, and subtracted patient from carer scores. This meant that positive scores indicated that carers were more pessimistic than patients (maximisers). Cure/control was reversed scored for consistency. In order to see if it was the size of the discrepancy rather than the direction that was indicative, we then modified the discrepancy score by removing the sign, to obtain a total discrepancy score.

Camberwell Family Interview (CFI) [44]

The CFI is a semi-structured interview during which carers are asked how they get on with the person who has had a recent episode of psychosis. It covers family relationships, arguments, time spent together, symptoms, and role functioning. With consent this interview was audiotaped, and subsequently rated for EE, by trained research assessors. Five scales are rated, critical comments (CC, frequency count), hostility (0, 1, 2 or 3), warmth (0–5), emotional over involvement (EOI) (0-5) and positive remarks (frequency count). More than 6 CCs, any hostility, or a rating of 3 or above on EOI categorise a carer as high EE. The interviews were taped recorded, and rated by assessors who had been trained by Dr Christine Vaughn After training, all assessors had obtained high correlations or phi coefficients on all EE scales: >0.76 for CC, hostility, EOI, warmth, positive remarks and overall EE category. In the current study we focussed on CC, EOI and overall EE.

Rosenberg Self-esteem scale [39]

This measure consists of 10 items, each measured on a four-point scale, from strongly agree to strongly disagree. After reverse scoring, the items were totalled and divided by 10 to produce an overall self-esteem score, where a high score represents low self-esteem.

The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) [22]

The 28-item version was used, with scoring of 0,1,2,3... It has a total score and four subscales of somatic symptoms, stress, social functioning, and depression. In the current paper we focus on the stress and depression subscales.

Participant measures

Illness Perceptions Questionnaire [47] (see above).

Rosenberg Self-esteem scale (see above) [39].

Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [11]

This established measure consists of 21 items, each measured on a scale of 0–3. The total BDI-II score thus ranges from 0 to 63, with a high score representing more symptoms. Depression is measured for the previous two weeks. Birchwood et al. [13], have reported a high correlation (r = 0.91) between the BDI and the interview based Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia [1], confirming that the BDI can be used for assessing depression in psychosis.

Beck Anxiety Inventory [10]

This measure consists of 21 items, each measuring common anxiety symptoms. The total anxiety score ranges from 0 to 63, with a higher score representing greater anxiety. Anxiety is measured in relation to the previous week.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were conducted using SPSS for Windows (version 12.01). Using analysis of variance we initially looked at the relationships between high EE and carer and participant consequences, timeline and perceptions of cure/control. We repeated this for criticism and for EOI, the latter having been dichotomised: ≥3 indicated high EOI. We used Spearman’s correlations to test relationships between effects on carers using the stress (B) and depression (D) subscales of the GHQ and the Rosenberg self-esteem score (SE). Differences in patient and carer illness perceptions were investigated for their relationships with EE in cross tabulations, with post hoc tests of significance. Finally carer and patient affect were correlated with IPQ discrepancy ratings.

Results

The sample consisted of 82 carer/participant dyads. Analyses were done on those with a full data set for the specific comparisons (N = 66–82). Demographic details for patients and carers are given in Table 1. Patients were mainly male (72%), unemployed (79%), white (83%), and single (63%). They had a mean age of 36.2 years, and an average 11.2 years of illness before the most recent episode. Sixty nine percent had a diagnosis of schizophrenia and 13% a diagnosis of schizoaffective disorder. The mean total PANNS score for the patient group was 67.3 (SD = 14.4). On the BDI II the mean score for patients was 24.9 (SD = 13.2), and of 20.5 on the BAI (SD = 14.7). On the Rosenberg, patients had a mean score of 2.46 (SD = 0.65).

Table 1 Demographic details for patients and carers (N = 82 days)

Carers had an average age of 52.4 years, and were mainly female (69%). Half were parents, and 40% were employed. Aspects of their contact time and ratings on EE are provided in Table 2. There was some expected overlap between high criticism and high EOI, in that 10 carers had both. Hostility coincided with criticism apart from one carer, and was not therefore considered separately in any of the following analyses. Carers were predominantly low EE (N = 53, 65%), and revealed relatively low levels of criticism (mean = 3.2). There was no relationship between EE and any demographic variable.

Table 2 Expressed Emotion ratings (EE) and contact time for carers

First, high and low EE dyads were examined in relation to the carer and patient-rated IPQ constructs of consequences, timeline and cure/control. All IPQ variables were rated in two ways, based respectively on the carers’ views and the patients’ views of the implications of the illness for the patient. Contrary to prediction, there were no significant relationships between EE and IPQ variables. When EOI was examined separately using analysis of variance, there was a single significant difference: patients in high EOI dyads rated the consequences of their illness less seriously (mean = 3.43, SD = 0.7) than those in low EOI dyads (mean = 3.84, SD = 0.57) (F = 6.14, P = 0.02). There were no significant relationships between criticism and any patient or carer IPQ variables.

Next we looked at whether carer IPQ variables were related to carer affect (Table 3). There were four significant correlations; carers who rated patients as having little cure or control had significantly greater stress (spearman’s rho = −0.25 P = 0.03), greater depression (rho = −0.26, P = 0.02) and poorer self-esteem (rho = −0.41, P = <0.001); carers who thought that the illness had a long timeline also had significantly higher stress (rho = 0.22, P = 0.05).

Table 3 Carer Illness Perceptions and carer affect (N = 82)

Discrepant scores on consequences and control were correlated (rho = 0.38, P < 0.001) as were consequences and timeline (rho = 0.43, P < 0.001). Carers emerged as significantly more pessimistic than patients about both consequences (carer mean = 4.02 (SD = 0.56), patient mean = 3.75 (SD = 0.62), paired t = 2.91, P < 0.005) and timeline (mean for carers = 3.86 (SD = 0.83), mean for patients = 3.47 (SD = 1.25), paired t = 2.86, P = 0.006), but not for cure/control (mean for carers = 3.56 (SD = 0.73), mean for patients = 3.43 (SD = 0.77), paired t = 1.15, ns). In other words carers generally tended to regard problems more seriously than patients.

We examined whether high EE dyads revealed more discrepant scores than low EE dyads, using analysis of variance. However no significant differences emerged. We then correlated IPQ differences with carer and patient affect. We found that when carers were more optimistic than patients about consequences, this seemed to relate to greater patient anxiety (rho = 0.35, P = 0.003), more depression (rho = 0.398, P < 0.001), and poorer self-esteem (rho = 0.38, P = 0.001). Carer optimism about illness persistence was also correlated with patient anxiety (rho = 0.26, P = 0.026). When carers were more pessimistic about future control of the illness, patients had relatively good self-esteem (rho = 0.29, P = 0.016), but carers experienced greater stress (rho = 0.39, P = 0.001) and depression (rho = 0.28, P = 0.019), and lower self-esteem (rho = 0.40, P = 0.001). Finally, irrespective of direction, the magnitude of divergent views between carers and patients about illness consequences related to carer emotional over-involvement, a component of EE (rho = 0.26, P = 0.022). This relationship remained significant (rho = 0.23, P = 0.045) when criticism was controlled for.

Discussion

We were not able to confirm previous findings [5, 32] that high EE related directly to negative illness perceptions in carers, nor that high EE dyads had more discrepant illness representations. This may have been because our EE ratings were lower than in other samples, so that we had a majority of carers in the low EE category. Our sample was thus unusual compared to previous literature. This may have been due to our low consent rates, as high EE dyads may have opted out of the study, but we were unable to check this. It may also have been the result of the rural residence of many of the participants. It was unlikely to have been due to errors in rating, as we used the Camberwell Family Interview, the ‘gold standard’ for measuring EE, and our EE ratings were trained to criteria. While the lower levels of criticism might have led to some lack of power, our high EE sample was still larger than those of previous studies in the area, but our results may not be generalisable.

What we did find was that negative carer illness representations were related to carer affect. Carer distress, depression, and low self-esteem was also associated with the thought there was little participants could do to control their symptoms. Moreover, carers who were more stressed were also more pessimistic about the persistence of illness. Barrowclough et al. [5] found relationships between carer depression, burden and negative consequences on the IPQ, but only high EE in carers was related to negative perceptions of cure control. Fortune et al. [21] found greater distress in carers who perceived negative illness consequences and felt strongly that patients could control their illness.

Lobban et al. [32] did not show relationships between overall EE and carer illness perceptions either, but did show that high EE carers (N = 14) were more likely to be maximisers of illness perceptions, compared to low EE carers (N = 35). We found that all carers tended to have more pessimistic illness perceptions than patients, and this was not related to EE. Instead we found that patients in high emotional over-involvement dyads (a component of high EE) rated illness consequences more optimistically. However, we do not know the direction of this relationship. Either patients in these dyads tended to minimise or perhaps deny any consequences for themselves, or patients who felt that there were fewer consequences had more over-involved carers.

We have previously shown that patients with high EE carers have significantly more anxiety and depression, but not more symptoms of psychosis [28]. Barrowclough et al. [6] demonstrated that critical evaluation by family members was associated with negative evaluation of self in patients. Lysaker et al. [33] recently found poor coping with stress in people with schizophrenia was related to their anxiety. Bentsen et al. [12] had previously shown that carer over-involvement related to greater patient anxiety and depression. In the current study we were able to show that disagreement about consequences and the amount of patient control in psychosis was associated with disturbed affect in patients as well as in carers, although the direction of causality is not clear. However, it is consistent with evidence from physical health that negative illness perceptions can relate to poor outcomes [23]. Divergent views, particularly minimisation, appear to be one response to serious ill health in carer-patient dyads [24]. We have shown that such disagreement can also be relevant to psychosis.

The results are of theoretical interest, in that they suggest that the meta-cognitive representation of illness has relevance for our understanding of psychosis in the same way that it has for other health conditions. Neither these representations nor discrepant views related directly to measures of EE as we had predicted. However, discrepant views did relate to negative affect in both patients and carers, and negative affect has been found to relate to EE in other studies. Discrepant illness representations may be a more direct way of understanding how patients and their families feel about, and try to cope with, the health threat posed by psychosis, and how they go on to make decisions about actions they will take as a result.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, it was a cross sectional study, and we were not able to infer causality or look at illness perceptions over time. Secondly, we did not compare participant IPQ ratings with symptoms. We have investigated this in another study [22], in which we found that, as we predicted, patients distressed about their symptoms had more negative illness perceptions but not more symptoms of psychosis. Thirdly, our sample of carers and participants had relatively low EE and generally did not come from inner city locations, and this may limit the generalisability of the results. Fourthly, unlike Figueras and Weinman [19], we did not use medians to establish discrepant IPQ ratings, but used a straightforward difference score. This may have weakened the power of some of our analyses. Fifthly, many of our correlations, although significant, were modest. Our results thus need to be replicated.

Clinical applications

Family interventions in psychosis [3, 4, 8, 18, 27] have a relatively well-established evidence base [15, 35, 37]. However, if illness perceptions in families relate to mood, this provides a theoretical basis for specific interventions for those living with psychosis. One therapeutic pathway is to facilitate communication and negotiated problem solving. Encouraging both participants and carers to be realistic and respectful of each other’s perceptions, neither overly pessimistic nor dismissive, and able to listen to a contrary view, is part of more productive and solution focussed change. This study suggests that we can use illness perceptions to understand that when carers and patients have different views about the health threat of psychosis, this can impact on mood. This awareness could help therapists to focus on ways of resolving such discrepancies, and improving negative mood, for both carers and patients.